Skip to main content

Full text of "Sexual Behavior In The Human Male"

See other formats


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN 
THE HUMAN MALE 


ALFRED C. KINSEY 

Professor of Zoology, Indiana University 

WARDELL B. POMEROY 

Research Associate, Indiana University 

CLYDE E. MARTIN 

Research Associate, Indiana University 


W. B. SAUNDERS COMPANY 

PHILADELPHIA AND LONDON 


1949 



Copyright, 1948, by W. B. Saunders Company 


Copyright under the International Copyright Union 


All Rights Reserved 

.This book is protected by copyright. No part of it 
may be duplicated or reproduced in any manner 
without written permission from the publisher. 


Published, January 5, 1948 
Second Printing, January 19, 1948 
Third Printing, January 22, 1948 
Fourth Printing, January 23, 1948 
Fifth Printing, February 2, 1948 
Sixth Printing, February 24, 1948 
Seventh Printing, March 5, 1948 
Eighth Printing, August 12, 1948 
Ninth Printing, June 14, 1949 


MADE IN U.S.A . 

PRESS OF 

W. B. SAUNDERS COMPANY 
PHILADELPHIA 



TO 

the twelve thousand persons who 
have contributed to these data 
AND TO 

the eighty-eight thousand more who, 
someday, will help complete this study 




PREFACE 


Seen from the four points of the compass a great mountain may present 
aspects that are very different one from the other — so different that bitter 
disagreements can arise between those who have watched the mountain, 
truly and well, through all the seasons, but each from a different quarter. 
Reality, too, has many facets — some too readily disputed or denied by 
those who rely only on their, own experience. Nor can science itself rightly 
lay claim to finality or the complete comprehension of reality, but only to 
honesty and accuracy of the additional facets it may be permitted to dis- 
cover and report. I say “may be permitted” since the human race is familiar 
with the suppression of truth in both small matters and great. The history 
of science is part of the history of the freedom to observe, to reflect, to 
experiment, to record, and to bear witness. It has been a perilous and a 
passionate history indeed, and not yet ended. 

Living creatures possess three basic characteristics or capacities — growth, 
adaptation, and reproduction. In human biology, the reproductive func- 
tion has been the least and the last studied, scientifically. To the National 
Research Council’s Committee for Research on Problems of Sex belongs 
the credit for sponsoring a more significant series of research studies on sex 
than has been accomplished perhaps by any other agency. Among these 
studies the findings of Dr. Alfred C. Kinsey and his associates at Indiana 
University deserve attention for their extent, their thoroughness, and their 
dispassionate objectivity. Dr. Kinsey has studied sex phenomena of human 
beings as a biologist would examine biological phenomena, and the evi- 
dence he has secured is presented from the scientist’s viewpoint, without 
moral bias or prejudice derived from current taboos. 

Certainly no aspect of human biology in our current civilization stands 
in more need of scientific knowledge and courageous humility than that of 
sex. The history of medicine proves that in so far as man seeks to know 
himself and face his whole nature, he has become free from bewildered 
fear, despondent shame, or arrant hypocrisy. As long as sex is dealt with 
in the current confusion of ignorance and sophistication, denial and indul- 
gence, suppression and stimulation, punishment and exploitation, secrecy 
and display, it will be associated with a duplicity and indecency that lead 
neither to intellectual honesty nor human dignity. 

These studies are sincere, objective, and determined explorations of a 
field manifestly important to education, medicine, government, and the 



VI 


PRJbjeACh 


integrity of human conduct generally. They have demanded from Dr. 
Kinsey and his colleagues very unusual tenacity of purpose, tolerance, 
analytical competence, social skills, and real courage. I hope that the 
reader will match the authors with an equal and appropriate measure of 
cool attention, courageous judgment, and scientific equanimity. 

Alan Gregg 

The Medical Sciences 
Rockefeller Foundation 



ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 


First of all, acknowledgment should be made to the 12,000 persons 
who have contributed histories, and particularly to the 5300 males who 
have provided the^ data on which the present volume is based. These per- 
sons represent each and every age, from children to the oldest groups ; they 
represent every social level, of several racial groups. If these persons had 
not helped there would have been no study. It has taken considerable 
courage for many of them to discuss such intimate aspects of their his- 
tories, and to risk their confidences with the scientific investigators. They 
have contributed in order that there should be an increase in our knowledge 
of this important aspect of human biology and sociology. It is unfortunate 
that we cannot name each and every one involved. Those who have spent 
time in persuading their friends to cooperate have contributed in a very 
special way; and to them we are especially indebted for devoted and effec- 
tive service. 

Herman B W^ells, the President of Indiana University, Fernandus 
Payne, Dean of the Graduate School of the University, the Trustees, and 
others in the Administration of the University have constantly encouraged, 
materially supported, and stoutly defended the importance of this research. 

The Rockefeller Foundation has contributed a major portion of the cost 
of the program during the past six years. Dr, Alan Gregg, as Director for 
the Medical Sciences of The Foundation, has encouraged a wide-scale, 
long-time project which would adequately cover all social levels and all 
aspects of sexual behavior in our society. 

The National Research Council’s Committee for Research on Problems 
of Sex, as a part of the Medical Division of the Council, has administered 
the funds granted by The Rockefeller Foundation. It has encouraged and 
advised on many aspects of the research. Especial mention should be made 
of the cordial support given by Dr. Robert M. Yerkes, who has served as 
Chairman of the Research Council’s Committee since its inception more 
than twenty-five years ago. Under Dr. Yerkes’ guidance the Committee has 
contributed to a long list of notable projects on the sex endocrines; on the 
behavior of chimpanzees at the Yerkes Laboratories at Orange Park, 
Florida; on the behavior of lower mammals, particularly under the direc- 
tion of Dr. Frank A. Beach, formerly at the American Museum of Natural 
History and now at Yale University; and on the human studies which were 
published by Peck and Wells (1923, 1925), by Hamilton (1929), by Kath- 
erine Davis (1929), by Terman et al. (1938), by Landis (1940, 1942), and 
by still others. 

vii 



ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 


viii 

The statistical set-up of the research was originally checked by Dr. 
Lowell Reed of the School of Hygiene and Public Health at The Johns 
Hopkins University. A long list of persons experienced in sampling and in 
other aspects of statistics has been constantly available for consultation. 
We are especially indebted to Dr. Frank K. Edmondson, Chairman of the 
Department of Astronomy at Indiana University, for continual guidance 
and supervision of the details of the statistical methods which have been 
used. 

Dr. R. L. Dickinson of the New York Academy of Medicine has rendered 
peculiar service based on his long experience with research projects in the 
field of human sex behavior. As the first head of the National Committee 
on Maternal Health, Dr. Dickinson encouraged a long list of pioneer 
studies, particularly on the clinical aspects of human reproduction and 
sexual behavior; and we have had the benefit of his accumulated experi- 
ence, and his constant advice on many of the details of this research. 

A number of other persons, additional to those whose names appear as 
authors of the present volume, have served for various periods of time as 
members of the full-time staflf on this project. Dr. Glenn V. Ramsey, now 
of Princeton University, was responsible for a series of histories from 
younger boys 'and served on this staff for a short time before going into 
the Army as clinical psychologist during the recent war. Dr. Vincent Nowlis, 
now psychologist at the Child Welfare Research Station of the State Uni- 
versity of Iowa, and Dr. Robert E. BugJ^ee, now of the Department of 
Biological Sciences at the University of Rochester, were formerly mem- 
bers of this staff, took some of the histories, and contributed to the labora- 
tory handling of the data. Dr. Nowlis has critically reviewed the whole 
manuscript of this volume. Dr. Paul H. Gebhard, trained in anthropology 
at Harvard University, has been a member of the staff during the past 
year. Mrs. Elizabeth Murnan has given full time as statistical calculator 
during the past three years. Mrs. Velma Baldwin, Mrs. Enola Van Valer 
Trafford, and Mrs. Ellen Lauritzen Welch have served as secretaries and 
research assistants on the project. Mrs. Hedwig Gruen Leser has been 
the German translator on the staff. 

Finally, much of the success of this project is to be attributed to our 
wives, without whose encouragement and specific help, and without whose 
support it would have been impossible to have carried this project through 
to its present point of development. 

None of these persons is responsible for any of the errors which we, 
the authors, may have made in our calculations or in our interpretations 
of the data; but the credit for any merits which this volume may have 
should be shared by those to whom we have made acknowledgment. 


The Authors 



PUBLISHER’S FOREWORD 

This volume is presented as an objective factual study of sexual be- 
havior in the human male. It is based on surveys made by members 
of the staff of Indiana University, and supported by the National Re- 
search Council’s Committee for Research on Problems of Sex by means 
of funds contributed by the Medical Division of The Rockefeller Foun- 
dation. 

This book is intended primarily for workers in the fields of medicine, 
biology, psychology, sociology, anthropology and allied sciences and for 
teachers, social workers, personnel officers, law enforcement groups, and 
others concerned with the direction of human behavior. 

W. B. Saunders Company 




CONTENTS 


Preface by Alan Gregg v 

Acknowledgments vii 


PART I. HISTORY AND METHOD 


1. Historical Introduction 3 

Objectives in the Present Study 5 

Development of Present Study 9 

Difficulties encountered, 11. Cooperating groups, 13. 

The Taxonomic Approach 16 

In biology, 17. In applied and social sciences, IS. Statistical basis, 20. 

Status of Previous Sex Studies 21 

2. Interviewing 35 

Making Contacts 36 

Establishing Rapport 41 

The Confidence of the Record 44 

Technical Devices in Interviewing 47 

Putting the subject at ease^ 47. Assuring privacy, 47. Establishing rap- 


port, 48. Sequence of topics, 48. Recognizing the subject’s mental status, 
49. Recording at time of interview, 50. Systematic coverage, 50. Supple- 
mentary exploration, 51. Standardizing the point of the question, 51. 
Adapting the form of the question, 52. Avoiding bias, 52. Direct ques- 
tions, 53. Placing the burden of denial on the subject, 53. Avoiding 
multiple questions, 54. Rapid-fire questioning, 54. Cross-checks on ac- 
curacy, 54. Proving the answer, 55. Forcing a subject, 55. Limits of the 
interview, 56. Avoiding personal identifications, 57. Avoiding contro- 
versial issues, 57. Overt activities versus attitudes, 57. Interviewing 


young children, 58. 

The Interviewer’s Background of Knowledge. 59 

3. Statistical Problems 63 

Nature of the Data 63 

Coding 71 

Supplementary Data 73 

The Twelve-way Breakdown 75 


Sex, 75. Race-cultural group, 75. Marital status, 76. Age, 76, Age at 
adolescence, 77. Educational level, 77. Occupational class of subject, 77. 
Occupational class of parent, 79. Rural-urban background, 79. Religious 
groups, 79. Religious adherence, 79. Geographic origin, 81. 


Size of Sample 82 

Diversification of Sample 92 

Hundred Percent Samples 93 


XI 



Xii CONTENTS 

Controlling Partial Samples. 101 

Order of Sampling 104 

Synthesizing a U.S. Sample 105 

Statistical Analyses 109 


Individual frequencies, 110. Group frequencies, 110. Frequency curves, 

111. Group averages, 111. Means, 112. Standard deviation of the mean, 

112. Medians, 113. Percents of individual outlet, 114. Percents of group 
outlet, 114. Correlation coefficients, 1 14. Accumulative incidence curves, 
114. 


4. Validity of the Data 120 

Re-takes 121 

Comparisons of Spouses 125 

Other Cross-checks 128 

Memory versus Physical Findings 130 

Smooth Trends 132 

Hundred Percent Samples 133 

Comparisons of Interviewers 133 

Stability of Techniques 143 

Immediate versus Remote Recall 148 

Older versus Younger Generations 150 

Conclusions 150 


PART IL FACTORS AFFECTING SEXUAL OUTLET 


5. Early Sexual Growth and Activity 157 

Erotic Arousal and Orgasm 157 

Pre-adolescent Sex Play 163 

Homosexual play, 168. Heterosexual play, 173. Animal contacts, 174. 

Pre-adolescent Orgasm 175 

Adolescence 182 


6. Total Sexual Outlet 193 

Frequency of Total Outlet 193 

Individual Variation 195 

Factors Effecting Variation 203 

Low Frequencies and Sublimation 205 

High Frequencies of Outlet 213 

7. Age and Sexual Outlet 218 

Adolescent Sexual Activity 219 

Maximum activity, 219. Social significance, 221. Institutional problems, 

223. 

Sexual Aging 226 

Old Age and Impotence 235 

Masturbation and Age 238 

Nocturnal Emissions and Age 243 

Petting to Climax, and Age 245 

Pre-marital Intercourse and Age 249 

Marital Intercourse and Age 253 



CONTENTS XIll 

Extra-marital Intercourse and Age 257 

Homosexual Activity and Age ; 259 

Animal Contacts and Age 261 

Post-marital Outlets and Age 262 

8. Marital Status and Sexual Outlet 263 

Social and Legal Limitations 263 

Total Sexual Outlet 268 

Sources of Sexual Outlet. . . 273 

Masturbation, 273. Nocturnal emissions, 277. Pre-marital petting, 277. 
Heterosexual intercourse, 277. Homosexual contacts, 285. Animal con- 
tacts, 289. 

Post-marital Outlets 294 

9. Age of Adolescence and Sexual Outlet. 297 

Onset of Sexual Activity 299 

Frequencies of Total Outlet 303 

Factors Involved 309 

Sources of Outlet 313 

Masturbation, 313. Pre-mantal inlercourse, 313. Homosexual outlet, 

315. Other outlets, 315. 

Aging versus Early Activity 319 

Conclusions 325 

10. Social Level and Sexual Outlet 327 

Defining Social Levels 329 

Educational level as a criterion, 330. Occupational class as a criterion, 

331. Realities of social levels, 332. 

Incidences and Frequencies of Sexual Outlet 335 

Total outlet, 335. Masturbation, 339. Nocturnal emissions, 343. Hetero- 
sexual petting, 345. Pre-marital intercourse, 347. Intercourse with pros- 
titutes, 351. Marital intercourse, 355. Homosexual contacts, 357. Ani- 
mal intercourse, 362. 

Attitudes on Sexual Techniques 363 

Sources of erotic arousal, 363. Nudity, 365. Manual manipulation, 367. 

Oral eroticism, 369. Positions in intercourse, 373. 

Patterns of Behavior 374 

Masturbation, 375. Petting, 377. Pre-marital Intercourse, 381. Extra- 
marital intercourse, 383, Homosexual contacts, 383. 

Social Implications 384 

In clinical practice, 386. In social service, 387. In the Army and Navy, 

388. In everyday contacts, 389. In the law, 389. 

Social Levels among Negroes 393 

11. Stability of Sexual Patterns 394 

Patterns in Successive Generations 394 

Comparisons of accumulative incidences, 396. Comparisons of frequen- 
cies, 399. 

Vertical Mobility: at an Early Age 417 

Occupational classes 2 and 3, 426. Occupational class 4, 427. Occupa- 
tional class 5, 433. Occupational class 6, 433. Occupational class 7, 436. 



Xiv CONTENTS 

Vertical Mobility: at Later Ages 436 

Transmission of Sexual Mores 440 

12. Rural-Urban Background and Sexual Outlet 449 

Frequencies of Total Outlet 451 

Specific Sexual Outlets 453 


Masturbation, 453. Nocturnal emissions, 453. Petting to climax, 453. 
Pre-marital intercourse, 455. Marital intercourse, 455. Homosexual out- 
let, 455. Animal contacts, 459. 


13. Religious Background and Sexual Outlet 465 

Total Sexual Outlet 469 

Masturbation 472 

Nocturnal Emissions 476 

Pre-marital Petting to Climax 477 

Pre-marital Intercourse 477 

Marital Intercourse 479 

Homosexual Outlets 482 

Religious Bases of the Mores 483 


PART III. SOURCES OF SEXUAL OUTLET 


14. Masturbation 497 

Definition 497 

References 498 

Incidences and Frequencies 499 

Incidences, 499. Pre-adolescent activity, 499. Adolescent activity, 506. 

In various groups, 507. 

Techniques 509 

Correlations with Other Outlets 511 

Significance of Masturbation 512 

15* Nocturnal Emissions 517 

References 518 

Incidences and Frequencies 519 

Content of Nocturnal Sex Dreams 525 

Relation to Other Outlets i 527 

16. heterosexual Petting 531 

References 532 

Incidences and Frequencies 533 

Techniques in Petting 54O 

Social Significance of Petting 542 

17. Pre-marital Intercourse 547 

References 543 

Incidences and Frequencies 549 

Nature of Pre-marital Intercourse ' 557 

Significance of Pre-marital Intercourse 559 



CONTENTS 


XV 


18. Marital Intercourse 563 

References 564 ' 

Incidence and Significance 564 

Frequencies 569 

Coital Techniques in Marriage 571 


Extent of petting, 572. Mouth stimulation, 573. Breast stimulation, 574. 
Genital stimulation, manual, 575. Genital stimulation, oral, 576. Posi- 
tions in intercourse, 578. Anal eroticism, 579. Speed of male orgasm, 
579. Nudity, 581. Preferences for light or dark, 581. 


19. Extra-marital Intercourse 583 

References 584 

Incidences and Frequencies 584 

Relation to Other Outlets 589 

Social Significance 591 

20. Intercourse with Prostitutes 595 

References 596 

Incidences and Frequencies 597 

Techniques 604 

Significance of Prostitution 605 

21. Homosexual Outlet • 610 

References : 611 

Definition 612 

Previous Estimates of Incidence 617 

Incidence Data in Present Sfudy 623 

Frequencies 631 

The Heterosexual-Homosexual Balance 636 

Bisexuality 656 

Scientific and Social Implications 659 

22. Animal Contacts 667 

References 669 

Incidences and Frequencies 669 

Nature of Contacts 674 

Social Significance 675 

23. Clinical Tables 681 

Definitions 682 

How To Use the Tables 685 

Single White Males 686 

Married White Males 710 

Previously Married White Males 730 

Appendix on Sample Size 736 

Bibliography 766 


Index 


789 




Part I 


HISTORY AND METHOD 

CHAPTER 1 . HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 
CHAPTER 2 . INTERVIEWING 
CHAPTER 3 . STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 
CHAPTER 4 . VALIDITY OF THE DATA 




Chapter 1 

fflSTORICAL INTRODUCTION 

The present volume is a progress report from a case history study on 
human sex behavior. The study has been underway during the past nine 
years. Throughout these years, it has had the sponsorship and support of 
Indiana University, and during the past six years the support of the 
National Research Council’s Committee for Research on Problems of Sex, 
with funds granted by the Medical Division of The Rockefeller Foundation. 
It is a fact-finding survey in which an attempt is being made to discover 
what people do sexually, and what factors account for differences in sexual 
behavior among individuals, and among various segments of the population. 

For some time now there has been an increasing awareness among 
many people of the desirability of obtaining data about sex which would 
represent an accumulation of scientific fact completely divorced from 
questions of moral value and social custom. Practicing physicians find 
thousands of their patients in need of such objective data. Psychiatrists 
and analysts find that a majority of their patients need help in resolving 
sexual conflicts that have arisen in their lives. An increasing number of 
persons would like to bring an educated intelligence into the consideration 
of such matters as sexual adjustments in marriage, the sexual guidance of 
children, the pre-marital sexual adjustments of youth, sex education, sex- 
ual activities which are in conflict with the mores, and problems confronting 
persons who are interested in the social control of behavior through reli- 
gion, custom, and the forces of the law. Before it is possible to think 
scientifically on any of these matters, more needs to be known about the 
actual behavior of people, and about the inter-relationships of that be- 
havior with the biologic and social aspects of their histories. 

Hitherto, there have not been suflncient answers to these questions, for 
human sexual behavior represents one of the least explored segments of 
biology, psychology, and sociology. Scientifically more has been known 
about the sexual behavior of some of the farm and laboratory animals. In 
our Western European- American culture, sexual responses, more than any 
other physiologic activities, have been subject to religious evaluation, 
social taboo, and formal legislation. It is obvious that the failure to learn 
more about human sexual activity is the outcome of the influence which 
the custom and the law have had upon scientists as individuals, and of the 
not immaterial restrictions which have been imposed upon scientific inves- 
tigations in this field. 


3 



4 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


There are cultures which more freely accept sexual activities as matters 
of everyday physiology Malinowski 1929), while maintaining exten- 
sive rituals and establishing taboos around feeding activities. One may 
wonder what scientific knowledge we would have of digestive functions if 
the primary taboos in our own society concerned food and feeding. Sexual 
responses, however, involve emotional changes which are more intense than 
those associated with any other sort of physiologic activity. For that rea- 
son it is difficult to comprehend how any society could become as con- 
cerned about respiratory functioning, about digestive functioning, about 
excretory functioning, or about any of the other physiologic processes. It 
is probable that the close association of sex, religious values, rituals, and 
cdStom in most of the civilizations of the world, has been primarily con- 
sequent on the emotional content of sexual behavior. 

Sexual activities may affect persons other than those who are directly 
involved, or do damage to the social organization as a whole. Defenders of 
the custom frequently contend that this is the sufficient explanation of 
society’s interest in the individual’s sexual behavior; but this is probably a 
post factum rationalization that fails to take into account the historic data 
on the origin of the custom (May 1931, Westermarck 1936). It is ordinarily 
said that criminal law is designed to protect property and to protect 
persons, and if society’s only interest in controlling sex behavior were to 
protect persons, then the criminal codes concerned with assault and 
battery should provide adequate protection. The fact that there is a body 
of sex laws which are apart from the laws protecting persons is evidence of 
their distinct function, namely that of protecting custom. Just because they 
have this function, sex customs and the sex laws seem more significant, 
and are defended with more emotion than the'laws that concern property 
or person. The failure of the scientist to go further than he has in studies 
of sex is undoubtedly a reflection of society’s attitudes in this field. 

Scientists have been uncertain whether any large portion of the popula- 
tion was willing that a thoroughly objective, fact-finding investigation of 
sex should be made. It is quite probable that an investigation of the sort 
undertaken here would have been more difficult some years ago; but we 
have found that there is now an abundant and widespread interest in the 
possibilities of such a study. Thousands of persons have helped by con- 
tributing records of their own sexual activities, by interesting others in the 
research, and by providing the sort of constant support and encourage- 
ment without which the pursuit of this study would have been much more 
difficult, if not impossible. Even the scientist seems to have underestimated 
the faith of the man of the street in the scientific method, his respect for 
the results of scientific research, and his confidence that his own life and 
the whole of the social organization will ultimately benefit from the accu- 
mulation of scientifically established data. 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 


5 


OBJECTIVES IN THE PRESENT STUDY 

The present study, then, represents an attempt to accumulate an objec- 
tively determined body of fact about sex which strictly avoids social or 
moral interpretations of the fact. Each person who reads this report will 
want to make interpretations in accordance with his understanding of 
moral values and social significances ; but that is not part of the scientific 
method and, indeed, scientists have no special capacities for making such 
evaluatiufts. 



Figure 1. Sources of histories. 


One dot represents 50 cases 


The data in this study are being secured through first-hand interviews. 
These, so far, have been limited to persons resident in the United States. 
Histories have come from every state in the Union, but more particularly 
from the northeastern quarter of the country, in the area bounded by 
Massachusetts, Michigan, Tennessee, and Kansas (Figure 1). It is intended 
that the ultimate sample shall represent a cross-section of the entire popu- 
lation, from all parts of the United States. The study has already included 
persons who belong to the following groups: 


Males, females 
Whites, Negroes, other races 
Single, married, previously married 
Ages three to ninety 
Adolescent at different ages 
Various educational levels 
Various occupational classes 


Various social levels 
Urban, rural, mixed backgrounds 
Various religious groups 
Various degrees of adherence to reli- 
gious groups, or with no religion 
Various geographic origins 



6 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


The study should ultimately include series of cases which will justify 
a description of the sexual patterns for each of these segments of the popu- 
lation. Whenever it is significant to have data for the American population 
as a whole, such calculations may be obtained by weighting and combin- 
ing the figures for the individual groups (Chapter 3). 

It is basically most important to know the story for each group in detail. 
By pragmatic tests of the effect of adding additional histories to the 
samples, it has been found that about 300 cases are necessary for a good 
understanding of any group in this study (Chapter 3). The size of the total 
sample necessary to analyze any larger part of the population thus depends 
Upon the total number of sub-groups which it is deemed desirable to 
investigate. 

To date, about 12,000 persons have contributed histories to this study. 
This represents forty times as much material as was included in the best of 
the previous studies; but 12,000 histories do not provide sufficient material 
for comprehending even those groups which are most frequently encoun- 
tered in the population. In addition there are other groups which must be 
studied because they are significant in analyses of more general problems, 
or because they occupy an unique or critical position in the ontogenies of 
particular patterns of behavior. It is now estimated that 100,000 histories 
will be necessary to carry out such a project. With a considerably expanded 
staff, it should be possible to secure that many histories in the course of 
another twenty years, and this is the goal toward which the present pro- 
gram is oriented. 

Of the histories now in hand, about 6300 are male, and about 5300 of 
these are the white males who have provided the data for the present 
publication. The generalizations reached in this volume are limited to those 
groups on which more or less adequate material is now available, or to 
those smaller groups which fall in line with the trends established for the 
whole series of data, But no generalizations can yet be made for many 
important elements in the population. For instance, it is not yet possible to 
give more than a suggestion of what happens among males beyond fifty 
years of age. We have only begun to accumulate data for the highly impor- 
tant chapter that involves infants and. very young children. Older, un- 
married males, and males who have previously been married, present an 
interesting situation which is only glimpsed in the present volume. The 
story for the rural population is quite incomplete, as is also the record for 
a number of the religious groups. Factory workers and manual labor 
groups are not sufficiently represented in the sample. Large sections of the 
country are not yet covered by the survey, although it is certain that there 
are striking geographic differences in patterns of sex behavior. The story 
for* the Negro male cannot be told now, because the Negro sample, while 
of some size, is not yet sufficient for making analyses comparable to those 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 


7 


made here for the white male. At no place has the sample been large enough 
to allow more than a six-way breakdown in the statistical analyses (Chap- 
ter 3), although enough important factors are now recognized to call for 
a twelve-way breakdown. A later revision of the present volume may be 
based on a further accumulation of male histories. 

A volume on the female, comparable to the present volume on the male, 
should be possible in the not too distant future; and volumes on particular 
groups and on special problems in human sexuality can appear when 
there is a suflScient increase in the total accumulation of data. It is now 
planned to publish volumes on: 

Sexual behavior in the human female 

Sexual factors in marital adjustment 

Legal aspects of sex behavior 

The heterosexual-homosexual balance 

Sexual adjustments in institutional populations 

Prostitution 

Sex education 

Other special problems 

All kinds of persons and all aspects of human sexual behavior are being 
included in this survey. No preconception of what is rare or what is com- 
mon, what is moral or socially significant, or what is normal and what is 
abnormal has entered into the choice of the histories or into the selection 
of the items recorded on them. Such limitations of the material would have 
interfered with the determination of the fact. Nothing has done more to 
block the free investigation of sexual behavior than the almost universal 
acceptance, even among scientists, of certain aspects of that behavior as 
normal, and of other aspects of that behavior as abnormal. The similarity 
of distinctions between the terms normal and abnormal, and the terms 
right and wrong, amply demonstrates the philosophic, religious, and cul- 
tural origins of these concepts (Chapter 6) ; and the ready acceptance of 
those distinctions among scientific men may provide the basis for one of the 
severest criticisms which subsequent generations can make of the scientific 
quality of nineteenth century and early twentieth century scientists. This 
is first of all a report on what people do, which raises no question of what 
they should do, or what kinds of people do it. It is the story of the sexual 
behavior of the American male, as we find him. It is not, in the usual sense, 
a study of the normal male or of normal behavior, any more than it is a 
study of abnormal males, or of abnormal behavior. It is an unfettered 
investigation of all types of sexual activity, as found among all kinds of 
males. 

There has not even been a distinction between those whom the psy- 
chiatrist would consider sexually well-adjusted persons and those whom he 



8 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


would regard as neurotic, psychotic, or at least psychopathic personalities. 
To have so hmited the study would, as with moral evaluations, have con- 
stituted a pre-acceptance of the categories whose reality and existence were 
under investigation. That this agnostic approach has been profitable is 
evidenced throughout this report by the data we have obtained on the 
high incidences and considerable frequencies, among well-adjusted per- 
sons, of behavior which has usually been considered to be both rare and 
abnormal. The study should constitute a considerable brief for the avoid- 
ance of classifications until there is an adequate understanding of the 
phenomena involved, especially if such classifications reflect evaluations 
that have no scientific origins. 


This is a study of all aspects of human sexual behavior, and not a study 
of its biologic aspects, or of its psychologic aspects, or of its sociologic 
aspects, as separate entities. What the human animal does sexually may be 
the concern of many academic departments, but the behavior in each case 
is a unit which must be comprehended as such and simultaneously on all 
of its several faces. Consequently, the persons involved in this research 
have been chosen because of their special backgrounds in a diversity of 
disciplines : anthropology, biology, psychology, clinical psychology, animal 
behavior, and the social sciences; and it is planned that persons trained in 
still other fields shall in time join in the research. Throughout the nine 
years of the study, many hours have been spent in consultation with 
specialists outside this staff, particularly in the following fields : 

Anatomy Neurology 

Animal behavior Obstetrics 


Anthropology 
Astronomy (statistical) 
Biology 

Child development 
Criminal law 
Endocrinology 
General physiology 
Genetics 
Gynecology 
Human physiology 
Institutional management 
Law enforcement 
Marriage counseling 
Medicine (various branches) 
Military authorities 


Penology 

Psychiatry 

Psychoanalysis 

Psychology, general 

Psychology, clinical 

Psychology, experimental 

Public health 

Public opinion polls 

Sex education 

Social work 

Sociology 

Statistics 

Urology 

Venereal disease 


Rarely has any project had more specific help from specialists in so many 
contingent areas. It is unfortunate that the number of persons involved is 
too large to allow specific acknowledgment by name. 

While the present volume may be of immediate use to many persons, 
its publication should emphasize the limitations of our present knowledge 
and should serve a most useful function if it enlists additional support for 
the continuance of the research and its pursuit to its ultimate goal. Eighty- 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 


9 


eight thousand other persons will need to contribute histories if the survey 
is to cover the major segments of the population. It is further hoped that the 
publication of this over-all study will encourage specialists in various fields 
of biology, human physiology, psychology, sociology, and other disci- 
plines to undertake research on problems which are little more than 
exposed by the present survey. 

DEVELOPMENT OF PRESENT STUDY 

The techniques of this research have been taxonomic, in the sense in 
which modern biologists employ the term. It was born out of the senior 
author’s long-time experience with a problem in insect taxonomy. The 
transfer from insect to human material is not illogical, for it has been a 
transfer of a method that may be applied to the study of any variable popu- 
lation, in any field. 

As a teacher in biology, the senior author had had his students bring him 
the usual number of questions about sex. On investigating biologic, 
psychologic, psychiatric, and sociologic studies to secure the answers to 
some of these questions, the author, as a taxonomist, was struck with the 
inadequacy of the samples on which such studies were being based, and the 
apparent unawareness of the investigators that generalizations were not 
warranted on the bases of such small samples. Stray individuals had been 
studied here, a few of them there, forty males in the next study, three hun- 
dred females in the most detailed of the case history studies (Landis et al. 
1940). More extended samples had been used only in the questionnaire 
studies, but they were of doubtful validity in connection with a subject like 
sex (Chapter 2). All of the studies taken together did not begin to provide 
a sample of such size and so distributed as a taxonomist would demand in 
studying a plant or animal species, or a student of public opinion would 
need before he could safely describe public thinking or predict the future 
behavior of any portion of the population. The sex studies were on a very 
different scale from the insect studies where, in the most recent problem 
(preliminarily reported in Kinsey 1942), we had had 150,000 individuals 
available for the study of a single species of gall wasp. 

In many of the published studies on sex there were obvious confusions 
of moral values, philosophic theory, and the scientific fact. In many of the 
studies, the interest in classifying types of sexual behavior, in developing 
broad generalizations, and in prescribing social procedures had far outrun 
scientific determinations of the objective fact. T^e seemed ample oppor- 
tunity for making a scientifically sounder study of human sex behavior. 

The difficulties that might be encountered in undertaking such a study 
promised to be greater than those involved in studying insects. The 
gathering of the human data would involve the learning of new techniques 
in which human personalities would be the obstacles to overcome and 



SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


io 

human memories would be the instruments whose use we would have to 
master. Analyses of the factors involved in the human behavior would be 
more difficult, because the sources of variation in behavior are much more 
complex than the sources of variation in structural characters in insects. 
The complexities of such a study constituted a test of the capacities of our 
science. However, enough success had been achieved in some of the pre- 
vious sex studies to make it apparent that there were at least some people 
who could be persuaded to contribute records of their activities; and we 
had had enough contacts with persons of other social levels, in city com- 
munities, in farm areas, in the backwoods, and in the remote mountain 
areas from which we had collected gall wasps, to lead us to believe that 
we might be able to secure cooperation from a wide variety of people. The 
more recently published research provided a considerable basis for deciding 
what should be included in a sex history, and our background in both 
psychology and biology made it apparent that there were additional 
matters worth investigation. A few of our closer friends gave encourage- 
ment to the plan, and in July of 1938 we undertook to take the ffist his- 
tories. 

It was a slow matter learning how to secure subjects and learning what 
interviewing techniques were most effective. Textbook prescriptions on 
how to conduct an interview did not prove effective. Our experience in 
teaching and in meeting people in the entomological field-work were better 
guides toward winning confidences and ^securing honest answers. It took 
six months to persuade the first sixty-two persons to contribute histories; 
but our techniques were developing, and we began to secure subjects more 
rapidly, as the following record will indicate: 

Total Number of Histories 


Year 

Increment 

Total 

1938 (6 months) 

62 

62 

1939 

671 

733 

1940 

959 

1692 

1941 

843 

2535 

1942 

816 

3351 

1943 

1510 

4861 

1944 

2490 

7351 

1945 

2668 

10019 

1946 

1467 

( 11486 

1947 (part) 

728 

12214 


Our skills have been steadily developed, but the increasing ease with 
which we have found people willing to contribute their histories is largely 
the product of a spreading knowledge of the existence of the study among 
tens of thousands of people, and of an increasing understanding of its 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 


11 


significance. For several years now it has been possible to find more his- 
tories than we have been able to handle. The first histories came largely 
from college students, because they were most available; but after the first 
year there has been a constant expansion of the sample to cover all seg- 
ments of the population. The question of securing a sufficiently large and a 
well distributed sample, even with a hundred thousand histories as the 
goal, is no longer a problem. 


Six persons have had a share in the gathering of the histories. The record 
is as follows; 


Interviewers Involved 


Interviewer 

Histories taken 

Percent of total histories 

Kinsey 

7036 

57.6 

Pomeroy 

3808 

31.2 

Martin 

890 

7.3 

Others 

480 

3.9 


It has been necessary to develop techniques for coordinating the work 
of those associated in the research, so that the data secured by the several 
interviewers might fairly be added together; but this did not prove an 
impossible undertaking (Chapter 3). It has also been necessary to test the 
reliability of every other technique, at every point in the program (Chapter 
3). During the first year the value of personal interviewing as opposed to 
the questionnaire technique was subjected to some testing. Since the first 
year, there has been an expansion of 22 per cent in the list of items covered 
in each history. A system was developed for coding the data taken in an 
interview. The basic problems of setting up a punch-card (Hollerith) sys- 
tem for analyzing the data were worked out. We learned how to make 
contacts that would bring histories from all segments of the population. 
By the end of the first nine months the scope of each history, the form of 
the record, and the techniques of the interview had been developed to very 
nearly their present form. Since then, there have been few changes except 
for the addition or deletion of some of the items included in a history. 

Difficulties Encountered. Before undertaking the study it was understood 
that gathering human behavioral data would involve social questions that 
are not involved in the gathering of scientific material less directly affecting 
the emotional lives of people. During the first year or two we were repeat- 
edly warned of the dangers involved in the undertaking, and were threat- 
ened with specific trouble. There was some organized opposition, chiefly 
from a particular medical group. There were attempts by the medical asso- 
ciation in one city to bring suit on the ground that we were practicing 
medicine without a license, police interference in two or three cities, investi- 
gation by a sheriff in one rural area, and attempts to persuade the Univer* 






12 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


sity’s Administration to stop the study, or to prevent the publication of the 
results, or to dismiss the senior author from his university connection, or 
to establish a censorship over all publication emanating from the study. 
Through all of this, the Administration of Indiana University stoutly 
defended our right to do objectively scientific research, and to that defense 
much of the success of this project is due. In one city, a school board, 
whose president was a physician, dismissed a high school teacher because 
he had cooperated in getting histories outside of the school but in the same 
city. There were other threats of legal action, threats of political investiga- 
tion, and threats of censorship, and for some years there was criticism from 
scientific colleagues. It has been interesting to observe how far the ancient 
traditions and social custom influence even persons who are trained as 
scientists. 

There seem to have been two chief sources of these objections. Some of 
the psychologists contended that sexual behavior involved primarily psy- 
chological problems, and that no biologist was qualified to make such a 
study. Some of the sociologists felt that the problems were for the most 
part social, and that neither a biologist nor a psychologist was the right 
person to make a sex study. A few of the psychoanalysts felt that sexual 
behavior could not properly be studied by anyone but a psychoanalyst. 
One group of physicians objected that taking histories constituted clinical 
practice, and that all such studies should be made by clinicians inside of 
clinics. 

r 

The second type of objection came from some scientists who, while 
admitting that sex studies of other animals were desirable, doubted whether 
human studies could be put on the same objective bases as other scientific 
studies. They objected that however well established the data might be, it 
would be inexpedient to publish them, for society was not ready to face 
such facts. Various persons, particularly leaders in sex education, con- 
tended that human sex behavior was primarily a question of the emotions, 
that no scientific study had ever succeeded in measuring the emotions, 
and (anyway) that if it were possible to make such measurements the data 
were too dangerous to publish, at least until all of the hundred thousand 
histories were gathered. Well-meaning but still timid advisors suggested 
that the fact of the existence of the study should be kept secret until there 
was actual publication. Several scientists admitted that such a study might 
be desirable, but more or less openly intimated that even scientists should 
make moral evaluations as they interviewed subjects and analyzed data. 
Several scientific friends urged that the study be confined to “normal” 
sexual behavior, without raising any questions of the validity of the 
generally accepted distinctions between normal and abnormal behavior. 

None of these scientific rationalizations, however, was as interesting as 
that of the hotel manager who refused to allow us to take histories — 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 13 

“Because I do not intend that anyone should have his mind undressed in 
my hotel.” 

Withal, it is not certain that the persons who have objected to this 
study have offered more of a hazard than the deserts and the mudholes, 
the mountain walls and the chasms, and the sometimes hostile natives with 
whom we had to deal in the course of the insect surveys. Never has the 
interference materially slowed up the present study. There were always 
persons who were willing to cooperate, as against the few who tried to 
interfere. There have been 12,000 who have believed in the research 
strongly enough to contribute their histories; and there are tens of thou- 
sands more who are now ready to contribute, as soon as we have time to 
interview them. 

After the first few years the interference largely passed, and the over-all 
history of the present study would be totally misunderstood if the emphasis 
were placed anywhere except on the remarkable record of cooperation 
from persons of every kind and from groups of every sort. For every 
scientist who has obstructed, there have been hundreds of others, in every 
field, who have helped. The list of consultants given above will indicate 
how diverse a group of specialists has been interested. 

Cooperating Groups. Scores of psychiatrists and physicians have 
offered every sort of cooperation, including opportunities to lecture to 
medical groups, and to discuss the data with them in seminars and in 
clinical sessions ; help in securing Histories from medical students and from 
medical faculties; help in securing histories from patients; the free use of 
working space in medical school buildings and in private oflices; access 
to library and laboratory facilities; and access to clinical records. It is 
doubtful if the medical and psychiatric group has ever contributed 
more generously to a study which was not primarily under medical 
direction and which was not wholly in the field of medicine. The 
history provides an outstanding instance of the sort of coordination of 
clinical experience and research science which will become increasingly 
necessary if either clinicians or research scientists are to gain any final 
understanding of human problems. 

Medical and psychiatric groups, from some of whose members his- 
tories have been secured, include: 

American Association of Marriage Counselors 

Association for Psychoanalytic and Psychosomatic Medicine 

Bellevue Psychiatric Hospital 

Children’s Hospital, Philadelphia 

Cornell University Medical College 

Frederick Douglass Memorial Hospital, Philadelphia 

Hahnemann Medical College 

Indiana University Medical School 

Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia 

Margaret Sanger Research Bureau, New York 



14 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Menninger Clinic, Topeka, Kansas 

New York Academy of Medicine 

New York Medical College 

New York Psychoanalytic Society and Institute 

New York State Psychiatric Hospital and Institute 

New York University Medical School 

Psychiatric Institute, Philadelphia 

Private Psychiatric Clinic, Chicago 

Psychiatric Clinic for Chicago Courts 

Temple University Medical School 

University of Illinois Medical College 

University of Pennsylvania Medical School 

U. S. Army Training Program in Neuropsychiatry 

U. S. Public Health Service 

Womans Medical College, Philadelphia 

Yale University Medical School 

Hundreds of educators have cooperated, at every level of the educational 
system, from kindergarten schools, through public and private schools, to 
colleges, and the professional schools. Persons who have been students at 
528 American colleges and universities have contributed histories to the 
record, The educational institutions which have been attended by these 
persons include the following: 

Anderson, Indiana, High School 
Bradley Institute (Illinois) 

* Brooklyn College 

* Bryn Mawr College 
Butler University 
California, University of 
Chicago Junior Colleges (var.) 

* Chicago, University of 
College of City of New York 

* Columbia University 

* Cornell University 
Depauw University 

Drexel Institute (Philadelphia) 

Franklin College (Indiana) 

* Fort Hays State College (Kansas) 

* Harvard University 
Hunter College (New York) 

* Illinois, University of 

* Indiana University 
Michigan, University of 
Minnesota, University of 

* New York University 
Northwestern University 
Ohio State University 
Pennsylvania State College 

* Pennsylvania, University of 
Princeton University 

Peoria, Illinois: Junior and Senior High Schools 
Pestalozzi School (Chicago) 

Philadelphia Public High Schools 

Private schools: in Chicago, Wilmington, and Philadelphia (in the Germantown 
area, the Main-line area, and elsewhere) 

* Institutions from which 100 or more subjects have contributed histories. 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 


15 


* Purdue University 

^ Swarthmore College 

“ Temple University 
Western Reserve University 
Wisconsin, University of 
Yale University 

The administrations of penal and correctional institutions naturally 
questioned whether their inmates would be disturbed if we attempted to 
secure sex histories from them. However, a number of institutions have now 
cooperated, and others have extended invitations to work with their popula- 
tions. Institutional heads, judges of courts, parole and probation officers, 
and police officers have expressed a considerable interest in the bearing of 
this research on problems of law enforcement. The inmate populations in 
these institutions have voluntarily cooperated in splendid fashion. From 
this material we shall ultimately publish a volume on the legal aspects of 
sexual behavior, and one on the problems of sexual adjustment within 
institutions. In addition, these prison populations have augmented our 
understanding of economically and educationally lower social levels, and 
of the broken marriages which are in the histories of a high proportion of 
the penal inmates. Groups from which histories have been secured include: 

Central States Association of Parole and Probation Officers 

Chicago : City Courts 

Chicago : Randall House for Negro Boys 

Cleveland : A suburban Court group 

Delaware : Kruse School for Negro Girls 

Indiana : State Penal Farm ^ 

Indiana: State Woman's Prison 
Indianapolis: Board of Public Safety 
Kansas : State Police 

National Association of Superintendents of Women’s Correctional Institutions 

New York City: Courts 

New York City: Florence Crittenton Home 

New York City: Police Commissioner 

New York State: Woman’s Reformatory 

Ohio : Bureau of Juvenile Research 

Philadelphia: Probation officers 

South Bend, Indiana: Courts 

Other institutions, neither penal nor correctional, from which individuals 
have contributed histories include: 

Michigan: State Training School (for feeble-minded) at Coldwater 
Mishawaka, Indiana: cSldren’s Home 
Philadelphia: Salvation Army Home for Children 
Philadelphia: Salvation Army Industrial Home 
Philadelphia: Salvation Army Home for Unmarried Mothers 
Philadelphia: Sheltering Arms, Home for Unmarried Mothers 
St. Joseph County, Indiana: Department of Public Welfare 

Persons not connected with any kind of institution have been more 
difficult to contact. Nevertheless, it has been possible to stir up considerable 
community interest in the study, and friends who tell their friends develop 

2 



16 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


a geometrically expanding network of contacts which ultimately provide a 
broad sample of any community. The histories of such persons constitute 
a major source of the data used in this study. Individuals who have con- 
tributed histories have been associated with the following social or civic 
organizations: 

American Museum of Natural History: Staff 

Church Groups: Bloomington (Indiana), Nicodemus (Kansas), Chicago, Phila- 
delphia, Edinburg (Indiana) 

Conscientious Objectors : Philadelphia (3 groups), Bloomington 
Family Groups: Philadelphia, New York, Chicago, Bloomington 
Hitch-hikers 

Homosexual Communities : Cliicago, New York, Philadelphia, Indianapolis, St. Louis 
Individual Histories : From all the communities in which we have worked 
Journalists, Editorial, and Publishing Groups: Philadelphia, New York 
Marriage Counseling Groups: New York, Philadelphia, Cleveland, State College 
(Pennsylvania) 

Men’s Service Clubs : Elkhart (Indiana) 

Negro Lower Level Communities : Chicago, Indianapolis, Gary (Indiana) 

Negro Professional and Middle Class Communities: Chicago, Columbus (Ohio), 
Philadelphia, Gary (Indiana), Hill City (Kansas), Nicodemus (Kansas), Peoria 
(Illinois) 

Nurses’ Groups: Philadelphia 
N. Y. A. : Bloomington (Indiana) 

Parent-Teacher Groups in Private Schools: Chicago, Philadelphia, Wilmington 
Parent-Teat:her Groups in Public Schools: Anderson (Indiana), Edinburg (Indiana), 
Philadelphia, Peoria (Illinois) 

Physicians’ Patients: New York, Chicago, Philadelphia 

Probation, Parole, and Court Groups: Pliiladelphia, Cleveland, Chicago, South 
Bend (Indiana) ^ 

Professional Groups (various): New York, Chicago, Philadelphia 
Salvation Army, Staff Workers and Clients: New York, Philadelphia, Chicago 
Sex Education Groups: Philadelphia, New York, Cleveland 
Social Worker Groups: Philadelphia, New York, Chicago, Southern Indiana, 
South Bend (Indiana), Cleveland 
Travelers on Trains 

Underworld Communities: Chicago, Peoria (Illinois), Indianapolis, New York 
City, Gary (Indiana) 

Umversity Woman’s Group, AAUW : Wilmington (Delaware) 

White, Lower Level Communities: Chicago, Peoria (Illinois) 

White, Middle Class, and Upper Level Communities: Chicago, New York, Phila- 
delphia, Bloomington, South Bend, Elkhart (Indiana), Anderson (Indiana) 
Wistar Institute, Philadelphia 

Y. M. C. A.: Peoria (Illinois), Indianapolis, Elkhart (Indiana) 

Y. W. C. A.: Anderson (Indiana), Philadelphia, Chicago 

Before undertaking this research, we had known something of people 
as they appear to their friends and to their neighbors ; but now we have had 
a chance to learn more about them. Understanding something of their 
satisfactions and heartaches, and the backgrounds of their lives, has 
increased our sympathetic acceptance of people as they are. 

THE TAXONOMIC APPROACH 

Since the technique of this research has been taxonomic, a word needs 
to be said about the taxonomic method. 



fflSTORICAL INTRODUCTION 


17 


In Biology. Taxonomy is a development of systematic botany and 
systematic zoology. Historically, these are the oldest of the biologic 
sciences. Their original functions were those of naming, describing, and 
classifying species and the higher categories. Modern taxonomy still has 
those same functions, but its techniques are very different (Ahlstrom 1937, 
Crampton 1925, Dice 1932, Dobzhansky 1937, 1941, Erickson 1941, Hile 
1937, Hubbs and Miller 1942, Hubbs and Johnson 1943, Huxley 1940, 
Kinsey 1942, Mayr 1942, Miller 1941). Since the differences between the 
present study and previous work on human sex behavior are essentially 
the same as the differences between modern taxonomy and the older sys- 
tematics, it will be profitable to compare the two. 

Modern taxonomy is the product of an increasing awareness among 
biologists of the uniqueness of individuals, and of the wide range of varia- 
tion which may occur in any population of individuals. The taxonomist is, 
therefore, primarily concerned with the measurement of variation in series 
of individuals which stand as representatives of the species in which he is 
interested. 

In order to have his sample representative the taxonomist must deal 
with much larger series than the older systematist ever thought of handling. 
Where the systematist used a single individual or a few individuals as the 
bases of his description and of his understanding of a species, the taxono- 
mist undertakes population sampling on such a scale as may involve 
hundreds of individuals from each locality, and tens of thousands of 
individuals from the species as a whole. If individuals are collected in a 
fashion which eliminates all bias in their choosing, and in a fashion which 
includes material from every type of habitat and from the whole range of 
the species, it should be possible to secure a sample which, after measure- 
ment and classification, will indicate the frequency with which each type 
of variant occurs in each local population, or in the species as a whole. If 
the sample is adequate, the generahzations should apply not only to the 
individuals which were actually measured, but to those which were never 
collected and which were never measured at all. Obviously, the correctness 
of such an extension of the observed data depends upon the size of the 
sample, and upon the quality of the sample ; and the capacity of the taxono- 
mist is to be measured by the skill he demonstrates in choosing and securing 
that sample. The next two chapters will contain a description of the tech- 
niques by which the material for the present study has been obtained. 

Beyond describing the groups involved, the taxonomist may also analyze 
some of the factors which account for differences between the individuals 
and between the populations of individuals which he is studying. These 
analyses depend upon comparisons of groups with backgrounds which are 
similar, except for some one item which may be identified as the source of 
the differences between the groups. This is the sort of analysis which an 



18 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


experimentalist makes when he compares an operated and a control group 
of animals. The experimentalist creates the backgrounds and controls the 
environmental factors wliich are the suspected agents of his results. The 
taxonomist finds the different backgrounds where they are already estab- 
lished in nature and, if his investigation is accurate, can reason as the 
experimentalist does about causal factors. 

Descriptive taxonomy provides an over-all survey; the experimental 
techniques are better suited to the examination of ultimate details. The 
taxonomist charts the paths which specialists may subsequently follow. Il 
is the function of the taxonomist to show the magnitude of the whole 
group which he has studied, so specialists will know how large an order 
must be satisfied if their generalizations are to apply to any significant 
portion of that group. 

In Applied and Social Sciences. Medicine, psychiatry, psychology, so- 
ciology, economics, anthropology, and the other social sciences are, 
after all, faced with the same problems which have confronted biologists 
in their attempts to describe and classify basic phenomena. They, similarly, 
need to secure such an over-all understanding of their one, highly variable 
animal, the human, as will ^‘show the magnitude of the whole group'’ and 
make it clear “how large an order must be satisfied if their generalizations 
are to apply to any significant portion of that group.” 

Unfortunately, it has not always beeq, realized that problems in social 
fields involve the understanding of a whole species. Much of the publica- 
tion in these fields is concerned with observations on a few individuals 
from whom generalizations are too often extended to any and to all other 
segments of the population. Observations on children, on senescent adults, 
on social groups, on gangs, or on whole towns are usually observations on 
particular groups, although they are presented as typical of life in all of 
America. Even when the data are experimentally derived, as in medicine 
and more recently in psychology, the problem of understanding the whole 
of the human species is still present. Experiments, whether operations, 
drug injections, physiologic tests, or psychologic manipulations, are usually 
limited to a few individuals when, in actuality, they should include persons 
of both sexes, of all ages, and from all sorts of socio-economic, educational, 
and religious backgrounds, if the conclusions are to be applied to the 
human species in general. 

College students, of college age, mostly from middle-class and urban 
homes — often with the sex unrecorded — are the subjects for a high pro- 
portion of the observations and experiments of academic investigators. 
No caution is given the reader that individuals of other ages, with differ- 
ent educational backgrounds and different social origins, might react 
differently to the same sorts of experimental situations. Even among 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 19 

psychologists, the apparatus used in the experiment may be chosen with 
more care than the human subjects of the investigation. 

In the medical, psychologic, and social sciences, there are a number of 
studies of single individuals who are described in elaborate detail (c,g., 
Allport 1942, Bios 1941, Brown 1937, Carlson 1941, Conwell 1937, Hillyer 
1927, Hoopes 1939, ^‘Inmate” 1932, Johnson 1930, Judge Baker Fnd. 1922, 
Karpman 1935, 1944, Kellogg 1933, Prince 1905, Rogers 1942, Shaw 1930, 
Thomas and Znaniecki 1918-1920, Wright 1945). It is implied that the 
observations or the therapeutic techniques used in the one case are appli- 
cable to other individuals in the general population. Psychiatrists and 
psychoanalysts have been particularly involved in such publication, and 
the anthropologists have led in this field (c.g.. Barton 1938, Beers 1908, 
Dyk 1938, Ford 1941, Hatt 1931, Landes 1938, Linderman 1930, 1932, 
McGraw 1935, Radin 1920, 1926, Simmons 1942, Spott and Kroeber 1942, 
Underhill 1936, Washbourne 1940, etc.). The idea is old. Linnaeus extolled 
the lone moss which was worth a life-time of study, and Tennyson thought 
of the flower in the crannied wall as the key to the secrets of the universe. 
Such detailed studies of single individuals have often represented a certain 
high degree of industry and scholarship, but they are dangerous as sources 
of generalizations about larger segments of the population: Like descrip- 
tive systeraatics at its worst, such detailed studies of individual cases are 
the antitheses of analyses based on large and statistically well selected 
samples of the sort the modern Jaxonomist employs. 

There are sociologic studies (e.g,, Burgess and Cottrell 1939), which 
appear statistical because they carefully define the group which was studied 
without, however, making any effort to select a sample which would be 
homogeneous and representative of any larger portion of the total popu- 
lation. Obviously, conclusions based on such studies are applicable only 
to the particular sample which was available to the particular investigator, 
and it is practically certain that no one will ever again meet, at any other 
time, in any other place, another group of persons similarly constituted. 

Sometimes social scientists hobnob as tourists in some social milieu 
sufficiently removed from their own to make it possible for them to 
acquire ‘‘impressions” and “hunches” about “social patterns” and “moti- 
vations of behavior” in whole cultures. This method has the merit of 
requiring a minimum of time — much less than the public opinion polls or 
the taxonomists need for arriving at their generalizations. Nevertheless, to 
some students the day seems overdue when scientists studying human 
material will forsake barbershop techniques and attempt to secure some 
taxonomic understanding of the human population. 

Some persons are appalled at the idea of having to undertake a large- 
scale coverage of thousands of individual cases before they are allowed to 
generalize about the whole. Contacts with the statistics of small samples 



20 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


have provided rationalizations for some of this inertia ; but no statistical 
techniques can make a small sample represent any type of individual which 
was not present in the original body of data. 

In the past dozen years, economic surveys, agricultural surveys, the 
public opinion polls, and a research group in the Census Bureau (McNemar 
1940, 1946, Gallup and Rae 1940, Blankenship 1943, Gallup 1944, Cantril 
1944) have shown the way in which a human population must be analyzed 
before there can be any understanding of any large segment of that 
population. Developed without benefit of the biologists’ experience with 
taxonomy, the public opinion techniques are, nevertheless, an illustration 
of taxonomic procedure. During the recent war, problems of sampling in 
the field of social problems received increased attention from the statisti- 
cians and the biostatisticians. Public health surveys are now utilizing 
modern methods of sampling. With increasing frequency the business world 
has learned to depend upon analyses of consumer reactions in the com- 
mercial field. The predictions in such surveys usually lie within 1 to 5 per 
cent of the subsequent performance (Katz 1941, Gallup 1944). In contrast, 
we should guess that many of the generalizations coming from the tradi- 
tional studies in the social sciences might prove erroneous in something 
between 20 artd 90 per cent of the cases, if one attempted to apply them to 
any considerable portion of the population. It is unfortunate that the 
products of academic studies are not more often put to the dollars and 
cents tests which have provided the ip^centives for increasingly better 
techniques in economic and public opinion surveying. 

Statistical Basis. Modern taxonomy is statistical in its approach. In 
many quarters there is an honest distrust of sampling techniques because 
there is a distrust of all statistical procedures. There is a widespread feel- 
ing that statistics are cold and that they cannot measure human emotions 
which, after all, are involved in all sexual activities as well as in many other 
human problems. It is objected that statistics can deal with incidences 
and frequencies and provide means for calculating average individuals, 
but that average individuals do not reaUy exist, and that measurements of 
such hypothetic individuals provide no insight into the particular persons 
with whom the clinician must deal. Such objections involve, however, a 
misunderstanding of the utility of a statistical approach. It is, precisely, 
the function of a population analysis to help in the understanding of par- 
ticular individuals by showing their relation to the remainder of the group. 
Given the range of variation, the mode, the mean, the median, and the 
shape of the frequency distribution for the whole group, the clinician can 
determine the averageness or uniqueness of any particular person, and 
comprehend the extent to which generalizations developed for the whole 
group may be applied to any particular case (see Clinical Tables, Chapter 
23). Without such a background, each individual becomes unique and 
unexplainable except through an elaborate investigation of him as an 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 


21 


isolated entity. On the other hand, if there are adequate data on the group, 
a major portion of the work involved in understanding a particular indi- 
vidual is thereby eliminated, even though it is true that he may still be 
so unique that he will need some special study. 

The possession of some “statistical sense” would seem to be a funda- 
mental requirement for anyone attempting to investigate any species, in- 
cluding the human. By “statistical sense” we refer to one’s capacity to dis- 
tinguish the specific from the universal and to recognize the difference be- 
tween a phenomenon which is common and one which is rare. One shows 
a statistical sense when he is interested in knowing how often a particular 
thing is true, and how often something different might be so — in short, 
what the incidence of each variant is in the population as a whole. The 
investigator who is satisfied to report a single set of observations is lacking 
in a statistical sense. The clinician who has made a dozen tests of a partic- 
ular therapeutic technique, and reports them as though they were appli- 
cable to anybody and everybody, is no scientist, for he lacks a statistical 
sense. Every scientist needs to cultivate his ability to distinguish between 
facts that are known to be true only for particular individuals, and facts 
which are known in such variety, for so many different kinds of individuals, 
that they may be added up to an understanding of a whole population. 

The present study should provide an instance of the taxonomic method 
applied to a problem that lies primarily in the field of human behavior 
and sociology. If the results of this investigation seem significant, the study 
will have been justified not only because of its findings but, what may prove 
to be of as much import, because of its demonstration of a method that 
can be used in other fields of research on human problems. 

STATUS OF PREVIOUS SEX STUDIES 

Although we have said that scientists- have largely avoided investigations 
of human sexuality, leaving this one of the most poorly explored fields in 
biology, psychology, or sociology, it should be emphasized that there is no 
aspect of human behavior about which there has been more thought, 
more talk, and more books written. From the dawn of human history, 
from the drawings left by primitive peoples, on through the developments 
of all civilizations, ancient, classic. Oriental, medieval, and modern, men 
have left a record of their sexual activities and their thinking about sex. 
The printed literature is tremendous, and the other material is inex- 
haustible. For bulk, the literature cannot be surpassed in many other fields ; 
for scholarship, esthetic merit, or scientific validity it is of such mixed 
quality that it is difficult to separate the kernel from the chaff, and still 
more difficult to maintain any perspective during its perusal. It is, at once, 
an interesting reflection of man’s absorbing interest in sex, and his astound- 
ing ignorance of it; his desire to know and his unwillingness to face the 
facts; his respect for an objective, scientific approach to the problems 



22 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


involved, and his overwhelming urge to be poetic, pornographic, literary, 
philosophic, traditional, and moral. Fortunately the scientific observer is 
not called upon to judge the merits of these diverse and contradictory 
approaches. All of them give evidence of what people think and do sexually, 
and that is sufficient to make them scientifically significant. 

The data on most physiologic functions of the human animal are, for 
the most part, to be found in the scientific treatises and journal articles 
published by physiologists. In contrast, the data on sex are spread through 
every field of history, literature, art, science, the social sciences, philosophy, 
religion, and the academically not respectable but still important materials 
of pornography. These last, as any archeologist or anthropologist well 
knows, may be of considerable importance in the interpretation of a 
human culture. There is a surprisingly large body of unpublished manu- 
script material in this field. A sex library, and any scholarly review of it, 
would have to cover material drawn from practically all of the following 
fields: 


Biology 

Anatomy 

Embryology 

Physiology 

Endocrinology 

Genetics 

Taxonomic method 
Human evolution 
Biostatistics 
Psychology 
General 
Experimental 
Clinical 
Abnormal 
Social 

Child and adolescent 
Comparative (anthropoids and lower 
mammals) 

Sociology 
General 
Criminology 
Penology 
Special problems 
Marriage and the family 
Anthropology 
Cultural 
Physical 
Ethnography 
Archeology 
Classical 
Medicine 
Obstetrics 
Gynecology 
Pediatrics 

Clinical endocrinology 
Urology 

Fertility and sterility 


Contraception 
Pharmacology 
Public health 
Hygiene 
Social hygiene 
Psychiatry 
Psychoanalysis 
Marriage Counseling 

Modern marriage manuals 
^ Classic manuals 
Child Development 
Personnel Programs 
Public Opinion Surveying 
Radio Programs 
Philosophy 
Ethics 
Religion 
Creeds 

Moral philosophy 
Sex cults 

History of religions 
Education 

Child development 
Sex education 
History 
Law 

Legal procedure 
Criminal law 
Marriage law 
Paternity law 
History of law 
Law Enforcement 
Police 

Parole and probation 
Censorship 
Military law 

Institutional management 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 


23 


Literature 

Fiction 

Essays 

Poetry 

Classical, of all cultures 

Biographies 

Travel 

Drama 

Journalistic, newspapers and mag- 
azines 

Propaganda 
Songs and ballads 
Folklore 
Linguistics 
Slangs and argots 

Arts 

Graphic 

Sculpture 

Photography 

Moving pictures 

Music 

Dance 

Stage 

Erotica, of Modern, Medieval, Classic, 
and Ancient Cultures 
Nude art 
Sculpture 
Art models 

Photographic materials 
Amateur drawings, stories, etc. 
Diaries 


Cartoons 
Moving pictures 
China and pottery 
Utensils 

Household implements 
Architectural designs 
Symbolism 
Mjasic 

Songs and ballads 
Limericks 
Wall inscriptions 
Vocabularies 
Literature 

Heterosexual 

Homosexual 

Flagellation, sadism, masochism 
Torture 

Religious persecution 
Corporal punishment 
Pseudo-psychologic 
Pseudo-anthropologic 
Love story magazines 
True confession magazines 
Physical culture magazines 
Nudist magazines 
Fetish magazines 
Scandal sheets . 

Advertising materials 
Fetishistic objects 
Materials on sex cults 


Obviously, it is impossible in any single volume to summarize the infor- 
mation contained in as diverse a body of material as is outlined above. 
Since the present volume is a taxonomic study of the frequencies and 
sources of sexual outlet among American males, we shall confine our 
review to those previous American studies which are (1) scientific, (2) 
based on more or less complete case histories, (3) based on series of at least 
some size, (4) involving a systematic coverage of approximately the same 
items on each subject, and (5) statistical in treatment. Many other studies 
which are not taxonomic have been important for comparison with our 
data, and they are cited throughout the present volume and listed in the 
Bibliography at the end of this book. There are only 19 studies of sex 
behavior (23 titles) which are in any sense taxonomic. They are: 


1. Achilles, P. S. 1923. The effectiveness of certain social hygiene literature. New York, 
Amer. Soc. Hyg. Assoc., pp. 116. 

A group-administered questionnaire study of 1449 males and 483 females, made 
by a psychologist. All of the subjects were from the New York City area, including 
high school and college students and some lower middle-class and lower level 
groups, mostly of yoynger individuals, with a few of them Negro. The study was 
primarily concerned with the effectiveness of certain social hygiene literature in 
disseminating information about venereal disease, but some of the questions con- 
cerned the sex experiences of the subjects. The population is broader than in most 
studies, but not broken up into homogeneous groups for analyses. The conclusions. 



24 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


therefore, are not applicable to the whole Ajnerican population, nor to any particular 
segment of it. The data are merely tabulated. 

2. Bromley, D. D., and Britten, F. H. 1938. Youth and sex. A study of BOO college 

students. New York and London, Harper and Bros., pp. XIII + 30:). 

A questionnaire and interview study of 1364 college students, made by two 
women with journalistic backgrounds. Forty-three per cent of the subjects were male, 
57 per cent female. Questionnaires were filled out by students attending 46 colleges, 
spread throughout the United States, and the authors visited 15 colleges to supple- 
ment their questionnaires with partial and not so complete interviews. The con- 
tributors may have represented a somewhat select portion of each student body: 
only one-fifth of the questionnaires which were distributed were filled in, and no 
device was used to assure a good sample. The statistical treatment of the data is 
scant, arriving at totals and averages, with no breakdown ol the sample for cor- 
relations. The data on petting and pre-marital intercourse agree fairly well with 
our own for college groups. On the other hand, the authors' suggestion (p. 27) that 
the conclusions may be safely transferred to the non-college segments of the pop- 
ulation, because college students are the leaders and set the behavior patterns for 
the rest of the population, is incorrect (Chapter 10 in present volume). The males 
in the study evidently balked at telling the women investigators about their mastur- 
batory and homosexual experiences, for the incidence figures on those points are 
lower than in any other published investigation. 

3. Davis, K. B. 1929. Factors in the sex life of twenty-two hundred women. New 

York and London, Harper and Bros., pp. XX -f 430. 

A questionnaire study made by an experienced social worker and her collabo- 
rators, located in the New York City area. The 2200 women subjects represented 
the select segment which answered a questionnaire that was originally mailed to 
20,000 persons. The sample was confined to “normal” women of good standing m 
their communities, most of them being college and club women. A high percentage 
of them were graduates of Eastern women’s colleges. The ages ranged mostly 
between 25 and 55. Many of the women weje teachers. The sample was, theiefoie, 
not broad, but neither was it strictly held to the group which was best represented 
in the study. Only a portion of the life history was covered. The treatment of the 
data is simple but statistical. 

4. Dickinson, R. L., and Beam, L. 1931. A thousand marriages. Baltimore, Williams 

and Wilkins Co., pp. XXV -f 482. 

Dickinson, R. L., and Beam, L. 1934. The single woman. Baltimore, Williams and 

Wilkins Co., pp. XIX + 469. 

Studies based on a gynecologist’s half century of experience in private practice, 
dealing with over 5000 female patients from the New York City area. Publications 
of the National Committee on Maternal Health. The findings of the physical e.xam- 
inations are systematic, particularly as brought together in the same author’s 
Human Sex Anatomy (1933, Baltimore, Williams and Wilkins Co., pp. XIII -f 145 
175 figs.). The data on sexual behavior vary from fairly complete life histories 
to single items. Most of the women came from middle-class or upper level homes. 
All age groups (except children) were included; and some of the cases were followed 
for scores of years. These pioneer studies have had considerable influence on later 
work, particularly among physicians, gynecologists, marriage counselors, students 
of fertility, and other climeal groups. There are only occasional tabulations of data, 
and the calculations of averages are sometimes inaccurate. There is no selection 
of the sample, and there are no analyses of the populations on which the various tab- 
ulations are based. The findings, therefore, cannot be transferred to other segments 
of the population. 

5. Exner, M. J. 1915. Problems and principles of sex education. A study of 948 college 

men. New York, Association Press, pp. 39. 

Apparently the pioneer attempt to secure statistical data on American sexual 
behavior. The author, a physician, was secretary of the Student Department of the 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 


25 


International Committee of the Y.M.C.A. A questionnaire study in which part 
of the questionnaires were group administered and part sent by mail. Answers 
were secured on a limited number of questions from 948 male students in colleges 
scattered widely over the United States. The treatment is simple tabulation. Since 
the population is homogeneously college male, the data should have been significant ; 
but because of the inadequacies of questionnaires the incidence figures for various 
sexual activities are much too low. The most notable aspect of the study was the 
‘TOO per cent” sample secured by getting records from every one of the 673 males 
in a series of groups, this series providing material for comparison with the data 
secured from those in the remainder of the sample. 

6. Finger, F. W. 1947. Sex beliefs and practices among male college students. J. Abn. 

Soc. Psych. 42:57-67. 

An interesting and profitable questionnaire study of 111 males, all of whom 
were college students in three successive classes in advanced psychology. The 
author, as the instructor in the course, had previously given lectures to the group 
on psychological processes in general and on emotional behavior and on certain 
aspects of sex behavior in particular. All but ten of the subjects were premedical 
students. The group, in consequence, was uniquely prepared for cooperating in 
such a study, and this vitiates the author’s contention that his results justify ques- 
tionnaire studies as against more costly personal interview studies. It is probable 
that the same methods applied to college students who did not have such a prepa- 
ration in biology and psychology would not have given comparable results, and 
the method would have been quite unworkable with most of the population that 
never goes to college. Of the 138 students to whom questionnaires were given, 
111 (81%) made returns. Sixty of the students were given a duplicate questionnaire 
to fill out nine days later in an attempt to test the validity of the. data. The study 
is unique among previous surveys of American sexual behavior in securing data on 
the incidence of the homosexual which are comparable to those reported in the 
present volume. Similarly, the data on masturbation and on heterosexual coitus 
compare well with those obtained by other investigators in this field. Since the 
population was homogeneous in* its sex, its racial constitution, its age, marital 
status, and educational level, the data may be taken as indicative of what can be 
expected from similar groups. 

7. Hamilton, G. V. 1929. A research in marriage. New York, A. and C. Boni, pp. 

XUI + 570. 

Hamilton, G. V., and MacGowan, K. 1929. What is wrong with marriage. New 

York, A. and C. Boni, pp. XXI + 319. 

A formalized interview study by a New York City psychiatrist whose earlier 
research experience had included studies of sexual behavior among apes. The project 
was undertaken at the suggestion and with the support of the National Research 
Council’s Committee for Research on Problems of Sex. The study was ba^ed on 
100 married females and 100 married males. Of this number 55 couples represented 
pairs of spouses, but no correlations of data were made on these pairs. The pop- 
ulation was not selected to represent any particular segment of society, and its 
constitution is not even described in the original report. Nearly all of the subjects 
were college graduates, most of them nearer 30 and under 40 years of age, most of 
them the psychiatrist’s patients or their friends, and most of them from the New 
York City area. An undue number (21%) of the subjects had been upset enough to 
have needed psychiatric help prior to their participation in the study. The data were 
obtained by personal “interviews” where the questions were typed on a card and 
handed without further discussion to the subject. The subject gave his answers orally, 
and they were recorded verbatim, without further questioning from the interviewer. 
There was considerable attention given to standardizing the questions and the inter- 
view, but none to standardizing the population, apparently on the assumption that 
if one standardizes one end of an experiment it does not matter what one throws 
in at the other end. The treatment involved simple tabulation and comparisons of 
male and female data. The small size of the sample also limits its usefulness, but 



26 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


there is a definitely systematic coverage of a large number of sexual items. The 
findings more or less match our own for white, married, college-bred, urban males 
between 30 and 35 years of age. 

8. Hohman, L. B. and SchafFner, B. 1947. The sex life of unmarried men. Anier. J. Soc. 

52:501-507. 

A study made by two psychiatrists on the basis of the interviews which they had 
with 4600 selectees at induction centers in New York City, upper New York State, 
and Baltimore during the recent war. Because of the very limited time available for 
each interview, the questions were few and, as pointed out in the report, not always 
well framed. The age, marital status, religious connections, educational level, and 
economic status of each individual were secured and the data on sexual activity 
were correlated with these background factors. A chief source of error was the fact 
that only two-way correlations were made. The data on the sexual activities of the 
several religious groups, for instance, probably would have led to different con- 
clusions if the comparisons had been confined to persons of the same educational 
level, and if the devoutness of the religious connections had been taken into account. 
In all the other comparisons the results are badly distorted by the failure to take 
educational levels into account. The statements made concerning the Negroes are 
unwarranted, for that reason. The data on pre-marital intercourse are misleading 
because they represent ogives instead of the accumulative incidence curves (Chapter 
3) that are needed to reach the conclusions which the authors are trying to make here. 
At several points in regard to the Negro histories, in regard to the high 
incidence of masturbation among adult males, etc.) the authors’ own subjective 
responses become evident. They arrive at an incidence figure for the homosexual, 
as they define it, of 0.36 per cent and express the opinion that this figure is reliable 
“because we were specifically instructed by mobilization medical standards to ex- 
clude homosexuals.” They seem to have no realization of the fact tliat the condi- 
tions under which three- to five-minute interviews were held in army induction 
centers were not conducive to winning admissions of socially taboo behavior, espe- 
cially when the men had been irformed beforehand that they would be puni.shed by 
being exduded from the Armed Forces if they did admit such behavior. For our 
own data on the incidence of the homosexual in the sort of group which was 
involved here, see Chapter 21. 

9. Hughes, Ww L. 1926. Sex experiences of boyhood. J. Soc. Hyg. 12:262-273. 

A group-administered questionnaire study of 1029 presumably unmarried males 
ranging for the most part between 15 and 20 years of age. The author w^s a biologist 
serving as a state health officer in North Carolina, and the sample represented a 
fair cross-section of both rural and urban populations of boys in high school, in 
grade school, and in employment in the mills of that state. Some comparisons of 
high school and mill boys were made with the caution that the sample was too small 
to warrant extended conclusions. Thus the set-up of the analyses was better than 
usual, but the dependence on the questionnaire was not good, and the choice of the 
questions was totally inadequate. Masturbation and nocturnal emissions were the 
only two, out of six possible sources of outlet, which were investigated ; and at every 
point the study is loaded with moral evaluations (^.^., “Has anyone ever tried to 
give you the mistaken idea that sex intercourse is necessaiy for the health of the 
young man?”). 

I'O, Landis, C., «t al. 1940. Sex in development. A study of the growth and development 
of the emotional and sexual aspects of personality together with physiological, 
anatomical, and medical information on a group of 153 normal women and 142 
female psychiatric patients. New York and London, Paul B. Hoeber,pp. XX 4- 329. 

A study of approximately 300 females, personally interviewed by Mrs. Agnes T. 
Landis, the interviews standardized with formal questions which were carefully 
worded on the interviewing sheets. The study was supported by the National 
Research Council’s Committee for Research on Problems of Sex. Approximately 
half of the sample was drawn from patients at the New York State" Psychiatnc 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 


27 


Institute and Hospital; the other half was obtained largely from a Y.W.C.A. 
and a Y.W.H.A. group. The ages lay between 18 and 25 years in 54 per cent of 
the sample; 54 per cent of the girls were Jewish, and 71 per cent had gone into high 
school but not beyond. The sample was largely from a particular sort of group but, 
unfortunately, it was not strictly confined to that group. The conclusions, conse- 
quently, are not transferable to the population as a whole, nor to a particular age, 
educational level, or religious group. After the breakdown for analyses, many of 
the ultimate groups contained only one or two to half a dozen individuals, and the 
statistical treatment is more elaborate than such small samples warrant. Practical 
experience shows that an additional three or four cases, or a dozen additional cases 
added to such small groups could have changed the conclusions which were drawn 
from such small samples. 

11. Landis, C., and Holies, M. M. 1942. Personality and sexuality of the physically 

handicapped woman. New York and London, Paul B. Hoeber, pp. XII + 171. 

An interview study of 100 handicapped females (spastics, orthopedics, cardiacs, 
and epileptics), interviewed in the same way as in the previous study (Landis 1940), 
the interviewing done by Dr. Marjorie Holies. The study was supported by the 
National Research Council’s Committee for Research on Problems of Sex. The 
sample was drawn from a group of New York City institutions; most of the girls 
were between 18 and 25 years of age; half of them were Catholic. The statistical 
treatment is too detailed and open to much the same objections as in the previous 
Landis study {q.v. above). 

12. Merrill, L. 1918. A summaiy of findings in a study of sexualism among a group of 

one hundred delinquent boys. J. Juv. Res. 3 :25 5-267. 

Data gathered by a probation officer who interviewed a hundred boys who were 
passing through a Juvenile Court in Seattle. The boys ranged in age between 8 and 
18, and most of them probably came from lower social levels, as most court cases 
do. It is unfortunate that the social backgrounds were not held constant for the 
studied group, so the conclusion% could have been utilized with more certainty. 
It is not entirely clear how systematically the sex items were covered on each history. 
The study is notable because it published the most definite record of pre-adolescent 
orgasm (for 6 boys in self masturbation), and gave the highest recorded incidence 
figures (31%) for younger boys involved with oral techniques in homosexual con- 
tacts. On both of these points, our own data substantiate those given by Merrill. 

13. Pearl, R. 1925. The biology of population growth. New York, Alfred A. Knopf, 

pp. XIV 4- 260 (especially pp. 178-207). 

A nicely analyzed study by a biostatistician using hospital data on 257 older, 
married, white males, most of them over 55 years of age. They had all undergone 
prostatic operation. Mailed questionnaires brought the information on frequencies 
of marital intercourse, which are the only sexual data actually given. The subjects 
may have come from the Baltimore area, or from elsewhere. The population is 
analyzed with a simultaneous age and occupational breakdown, which is instruc- 
tive. On the whole, the samples in each ultimate breakdown are too small, but the 
age trends seem correct. Unfortunately, educational levels were not used in the 
calculations, and our present data show that Pearl’s comparisons of rural and urban 
histories would have led to totally different conclusions if he had taken educational 
levels into account. 

14. Peck, M. W., and Wells, F. L. 1923. On the psycho-sexuality of college graduate 

men. Ment. Hyg. 7:697-714. 

Peck, M. W., and Wells, F. L. 1925. Further studies in the psycho-sexuality of 

college graduate men. Ment. Hyg. 9 : 502-520. 

A directed questionnaire study of about 550 men with college backgrounds, the 
study made by a psychiatrist and a psychologist, with support from the National 
Research Council’s Committee for Research on Problems of Sex. The study was 
made in the Boston area. Most of the subjects were between 21 and 30 years of age. 



Published Studies on Sex 




Bromley-Bntten 1938 Journalism Qst’naire, & par- U. S., wide 592 772 1364 Students m 46 Col- 40 = 7.7% Sex Male and female 

tial interview leges ( -i- 1 5 ‘^) college students 



Arranged chronologically 



30 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


The treatment was simple tabulation with no breakdown of the population; but 
since the study was confined to the one sort of group the generalizations may be 
extended to other college groups with a minimum possibility of error. The sex 
questions were limited to about 30 items, but as far as they go, the findings are 
close to those obtained in our own study. The second part of the study was expanded 
with about 80 more questions concerning the personality traits of the subjects, their 
recreational interests, etc.; but this added nothing to an understanding of the 
sexual data in the study. 

15. Peterson, K. M. 1938. Early sex information and its influence on later sex concepts. 

Unpublished manuscript in Library of University of Colo., pp. 136. 

A supervised questionnaire survey of 419 males who were still in college. The 
geographic locations of the colleges are not mentioned, but they were probably in 
Middle Western and Rocky Mountain areas. There is a simple tabulation of the 
data, most of which concern sex education, but there is some valuable information 
on adolescent developments and on later, overt experiences in sex. Some of the 
data were subsequently used in KirkendalFs volume on Sex Adjustments of Young 
Men (Kirkendall 1940). Since the population was uniformly male, of college 
level, and of college age, the data are transferable to other college male groups. 
The results, however, are not sufiScient, because of the relative inefiectiveness of 
questionnaire studies. 

16. Ramsey, G. V. 1943, The sex information of younger boys. Amer. J. Orthopsy- 

chiatry 13:347-352. 

Ramsey, G, V. 1943. The sexual development of boys. Amer. J. Psych. 56:217-23 4 

Studies of 291 pre-adolescent and younger adolescent males, representing all of 
the boys (and some others) in a seventh and eighth grade group in a medium-large 
city in^ Illinois. Based on personal interviews which were coordinated with the list 
of questions and the techniques of the present study. The study is particularly 
valuable because of the age period covered, and further significant because of its 
hundred percent sample. The conclusions are not extensible to other groups of 
particular social levels, but the unselected saftiple is probably an approximation to 
the sort of group that occurs in any middle class junior high school in a medium- 
large city of the Middle West. 

17. Strakosch, F. M. 1934. Factors in the sex life of seven himdred psychopathic 

women. Utica, N. Y., State Hospitals Press, pp. 102. 

A doctor’s thesis in psychology, based on the accumulated case history records 
in the New York State Psychiatric Institute and Hospital, in New York City. The 
data had been gathered by a number of psychiatrists, with possible variations in 
standards of recording. All of the 700 women were psychopathic. They represented 
a wide range of ages, and a variety of educational and social levels. There are tab- 
ulations and some simple but sometimes erroneous statistical treatments of the 
data, and comparisons of each item with similar calculations in the studies by 
Davis (1929), Hamilton (1929), and Dickinson and Beam (1931, 1934). These com- 
parisons are intended to show the similarities and differences between psychopathic 
and normal women. However, all of the other studies dealt with populations 
that were from more or less exclusively upper educational and social levels, and 
the Strakosch sample came from such different social levels that all comparisons in 
this study are invalid (see Chapter 10). 

18. Taylor, W. S. 1933. A critique of sublimation in males: A study of 40 superior 

single men. Genet. Psych. Monogr. 13 (1): 1-1 15. 

The sample is fairly homogeneous. It is made up of single males, mostly between 
21 and 30 years of age, who were for the most part superior graduate students 
from universities in Eastern Massachusetts. Because of the uniformity of the 
population, the results are better than such a small sample might be expected to 
give. The incidence and frequency data are tabulated and totalled, without further 
statistical analyses. 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 31 

19. Terman, L. M., et al. 1938. Psychological factors in marital happiness. New York 
and London, McGraw Hill Book Co., pp. XIV -h 474. 

A group-administered questionnaire study of 2484 subjects representing nearly 
1250 pairs of spouses. The study was made by a psychological group, supported 
by the National Research Council’s Committee for Research on Problems of Sex. 
Most of the study was concerned with questions of personality and marital happi- 
ness, but there are data on pre-marital intercourse and on marital intercourse. The 
population came from various occupational groups, but chiefly from professional 
and semi-professional classes. Seventy-one per cent of the sample had gone to college ; 
the mean age was near 39 years; the subjects came from various areas in Cahfornia. 
The statistical treatment is better than in most studies, because the data are analyzed 
by age and by occupational class, although not simultaneously for the two factors 
because the populations were not large enough to warrant such a breakdown. The 
data would have been more reliable if they had been obtained by direct interviewing, 
and the conclusions would have been totally different at certain points if the analyses 
had been confined to particular educational levels. 

In summarizing the taxonomic validity of the 19 studies reviewed above 
it is to be noted that: 

1. The previous investigators of American sexual behavior have been, 
variously: Psychologists (in 9 cases), psychiatrists (in 4 cases), journalists 
(2 persons), a gynecologist, biologists (in 2 cases), a student of education, 
a public health officer, a social worker, a probation officer, and a physician 
who was an officer in the Y.M.C.A. The statistically most useful work has 
come from the two journalists (Bromley and Britten), the Y.M.C.A.’s 
physician (Exner), a psychiatrist (Peck), four psychologists (Ramsey, 
Finger, Taylor, and Wells), and the student of education (Peterson). 
Other studies have not been as sound because of the mixed or otherwise 
inadequate nature of the populations on which the generalizations were 
based. 

2, The techniques of the studies have been questionnaire in 10 cases, and 
the examination of clinical records in 1 case. Gynecological examinations 
have been made in 2 studies. In less than half (8) of the cases has the investi- 
gator actually faced his subjects in actual interview, but in only 4 of the 
studies (Dickinson, Merrill, Ramsey, and Taylor) has the interviewer 
abandoned the limits of pre-formed questions and stilted formalities and 
talked as friends talk to friends (Chapter 2). The most serious error in these 
studies has been the wide use of questionnaires. They are used because they 
are easier to administer, and they save time. When distributed to a group 
of persons who simultaneously fill out the answers after they are brought 
together in a lecture room, a Y.M.C.A. gathering, or an Army mess hall, 
the investigator or his associates can secure a couple of hundred histories 
in the same amount of time that another person, using a personal inter- 
view technique, needs to contact, win, and secure a single history. How- 
ever, the differences in values of the two techniques, especially when applied 
to a socially taboo subject like sex, more than justify the extra time and 
expense that go into an interview (Chapter 2). 



32 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


3. More than a third (7) of the studies were based on material from the 
New York City area, and 5 others came from nearby points in the eastern 
United States. Our own data indicate that New York, in its sexual pattern, 
is one of the unique cities in the country; and, in spite of its importance, 
it does not give a picture which is typical of the United States as a whole. 
Three of the studies on college students (Bromley and Britten, Exner, and 
Peterson, all of them questionnaire studies) were the only ones which 
drew data from any wide area over the country. 

4. Males and females have figured about equally in the published 
studies. In 10 cases only males were investigated; in 5 cases, only females; 
in 4 cases, both males and females. The sex distribution is fairly good, but 
there are few comparisons of the two sexes. 

5. The previous studies were based wholly or primarily on individuals 
of college level in 10 of the 19 cases. In 6 studies most of the subjects 
belonged to groups that had not gone beyond high school, and in no study 
was the sample distributed through large portions of the population. In 
this field, as in many others, many of the studies have been “so limited by 
the campus-bound inertia of research that generalizations have tended to 
hold only for college students, rather than for man in generaf’ (McNemar 
1946). This has been particularly disastrous in considering questions of 
sexual behavior, for we now know (see later chapters) that different age 
groups, persons who stop their education at different levels, and persons 
from different social or occupational classes have very different sexual 
patterns, involving in some groups participation in certain types of activi- 
ties with frequencies that may be 10 or 20 times higher than the frequencies 
in other portions of the population. 

6. The number of questions asked in the various studies has varied from 
8 in the earliest instance (Exner 1915) to 147 in the Hamilton study (1929). 
There were 218 in the Ramsey study (1943) which was coordinated with our 
own. There are 521 items which are systematically covered on each of the 
histories in the present study (Chapter 3). In the earlier studies there is 
evidence of a considerable hesitancy to ask direct questions about sex. The 
interviewer is evidently embarrassed, substitutes euphemisms, and com- 
pletely avoids whole chapters of possible activity. In our own experience, 
direct questions on sex, asked as simply as questions about age, place of 
birth, etc., are answered as simply as they are asked, and get more honest 
answers. Either average men and women or the scientists have become 
more scientific in the thirty years that have elapsed since American sex 
surveys were first undertaken. 

7. The size of the sample necessary for statistically sound generalizations 
depends on the variability of the phenomenon under investigation and the 
homogeneity of the population in respect to the factors which effect that 
variation. We have indicated that pragmatic tests show that about 300 



HISTORICAL INTRODUCTION 


33 


cases are desirable for a sample in a group that is homogeneous for sex, 
age, educational level, and the various other factors (Chapter 3). The total 
number of cases gathered in most of the previously published studies falls 
above these minima, and seems impressive until it is realized that only 
five of the studies (Bromley and Britten, Exner, Finger, Peck and Wells, 
Peterson) were restricted to populations wliich were homogeneous for 
three or more of the factors listed above. 

8. The possibility of extending the conclusions reached in any investi- 
gation is, after all, the chief excuse for doing research. A mastery of the 
realities of the universe, or of any particular corner of it, depends upon the 
capacity to find over-all formulae, over-all descriptions which will apply 
to the whole or to some appreciable portion of the whole. There is no use 
in studying particular rats or mice or men if one has to believe that infor- 
mation gained thereby will never again be of use, because one will never 
again meet exactly the same kinds of rats or mice or men. There is no use 
in tabulating and totalling and calculating means and other fine figures 
about the sexual doings of particular people who go to a clinic or to an 
office, or who fill out a questionnaire, if such calculations and totals give 
no reliable notion of what the next group is doing, because the next group 
is too different from the sample which was studied. One may, as Hamilton 
did (1929:8-9), disclaim any interest in even trying to get a sample that 
would be representative of any other group, and insist that one is simply 
studying a particular two hundr^ persons because of a “desire to obtain 
as many comparable case records as possible and a desire to make of each 
case record a comprehensive list of significant facts about the individuals 
under examination”; but no one is fooled, not even if it is a psychiatrist 
who tries to rationalize away the objectors which his conscience anticipates 
there will be among his “survey-minded readers.” The book is entitled “A 
Research in Marriage” and there is no indication that it is supposed to end 
up as a study of the marriages of a particular group of two hundred people; 
but, rather, that it is a study of marriages in general, among people in 
general. No one would bother to study and to publish on the sexual be- 
havior of particular persons if he did not expect that his generalizations 
would have some applicability to at least some other persons in the world. 

The validity of extending generalizations derived from a study of any 
sample depends, fundamentally and unavoidably, upon the representative- 
ness of that sample. Each segment which is studied must be precisely 
delimited, and all conclusions must be confined to such precisely defined 
groups. This is the principle on which modern taxonomic method, the 
agricultural and economic surveys, and the pubhc opinion polls depend. 
Social scientists will have to pay as much attention to the way in which 
they select their subjects if they are ever to arrive at generalizations which 
are applicable to any large group of men. Unfortunately, such precise 



34 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


delimitations of the samples used have not been attempted in most of the 
previously published studies of sex. Davis, Hamilton, Dickinson, and Ter- 
man had populations which were largely college, and would have illumi- 
nated college populations very well if the samples had been strictly con- 
fined to college people; but the significance of the data in all these studies 
was impaired by the addition of cases from other social groups. 

Beyond the specific studies which have been reviewed here, there are 
several which present tabulated data on series of European histories, 
mostly of Russian factory workers and Russian college students (see 
Willoughby 1937 for a bibliography and a summary of the data). The 
Continental European patterns of sex behavior are so distinct from the 
American (as our own European sampling already indicates), that no 
additions of the European to the American data should ever be made. 

In addition, there are, of course, thousands of individual sex histories 
in the psychiatric and psychologic journals and texts, and in hundreds of 
other volumes. There are histories in the writings of Havelock Ellis, Freud, 
Stekel, Hirschfeld, Krafft-Ebing, Mantegazza, Marcuse, Moll, Block, 
Rohleder, Henry (1941), and a long list of others. In many ways these have 
been important contributions on sex. As pioneer studies they contributed 
materially to the development of a public realization that there were 
scientific aspects to human sexual behavior, and the present-day student 
finds it much simpler to undertake an investigation of sex because of the 
influence which these older studies had? But none of the authors of the 
older studies, in spite of their keen insight into the meanings of certain 
things, ever had any precise or even an approximate knowledge of what 
average people do sexually. They accumulated great bodies of sexual facts 
about particular people, but they did not know what people in general did 
sexually. They never knew what things were common and what things 
were rare, because their data came from the miscellaneous and usually 
unrepresentative persons who came to their clinics (Freud, Hirschfeld, et 
al.), or from persons from whom they happened to receive correspondence 
(Ellis), or from limited numbers of persons whom they interviewed in 
elaborate detail (as in the Henry study). None of the older authors, with 
the possible exception of Hirschfeld, attempted any systematic coverage 
of particular items in each history, and consequently there was nothing to 
be added or averaged, even for the populations with which they dealt. 

Considering the importance which sexual problems have in the practice 
of psychiatry, medicine, psychology, and counseling of every sort, it is dis- 
concerting to reahze what scant bases there have been for over-all state- 
ments that have been made in this field. The present study is designed as a 
first step in the accumulation of a body of scientific fact that may provide 
the bases for sounder generalizations about the sexual behavior of certain 
groups and, some day, even of our American population as a whole. 



Chapter 2 


INTERVIEWING 

The quality of a case history study begins with the quality of the inter- 
viewing by which the data have been obtained. If^ in lieu of direct observa- 
tion and experiment, it is necessary to depend upon verbally transmitted 
records obtained from participants in the activities that are being studied, 
then it is imperative that one become a master of every scientific device and 
of all the arts by which any man has ever persuaded any other man into 
exposing his activities and his innermost thoughts. Failing to win that much 
from the subject, no statistical accumulation, however large, can ade- 
quately portray what the human animal is doing. However satisfactory 
the standard deviations may be, no statistical treatment can put validity 
into generalizations which are based on data that were not reasonably 
accurate and complete to begin with. It is unfortunate that academic 
departments so often offer courses on the statistical manipulation of human 
material to students who have little understanding of the problems involved 
in securing the original data. Learning how to meet people of all ranks and 
levels, establishing rapport, syiripathetically comprehending the signifi- 
cances of things as others view them, learning to accept their attitudes and 
activities without moral, social, or esthetic evaluation, being interested in 
people as they are and not as someone else would have them, learning to 
see the reasonable bsises of what at first glance may appear to be most 
unreasonable behavior, developing a capacity to like all kinds of people 
and thus to win their esteem and cooperation— these are the elements to 
be mastered by one who would gather human statistics. When training in 
these things replaces or at least precedes some of the college courses on the 
mathematical treatment of data, we shall come nearer to having a science 
of human behavior. 

Problems of interviewing have been particularly important in the present 
study because of the long-standing taboos which make it bad form and, for 
most people, socially or legally dangerous to discuss one’s sexual activities 
in public or even in the presence of one’s most intimate friends. It is 
astounding that anyone should agree to expose himself by contributing his 
sex history to an interviewer whom he has never before met, and to a 
research project whose full significance he, in most instances, Cannot begin 
to understand. Still more remarkable is the fact that many of the histories 
in the present study have come from subjects who agreed to give histories 
within the first few minutes after they fiurst met the interviewer. We are not 

35 



36 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


sure that we completely comprehend why people have been willing to talk 
to us ; but there may be some value in discussing the bases on which we 
have appealed for histories, and in describing some of the devices that we 
have employed to establish the quality of the record. 

MAKING CONTACTS 

Any study which depends upon obtaining data from large numbers of 
people must have an appeal which is sufficient to win the whole-hearted 
cooperation of persons of every sort. In the present instance, the chief 
appeal has been altruistic — an invitation to contribute to basic scientific 
research, an opportunity to help others by sharing one’s experience. The 
appeal to professionally trained and other educated groups has involved a 
technical exposition of the scientific problems involved, and of the social 
significance of securing data which clinicians may utilize in their practice. 
The academic groups in psychology, biology, and sociology cooperated as 
soon as they saw broad, basic principles emerging from the study. Reli- 
gious groups saw a need for information on the early training of children, 
and have shown an outstanding willingness to cooperate in any study which 
might contribute to an understanding of problems which affect the stability 
of the home and of marriage. More poorly educated and mentally dull 
individuals have responded to the simple and brief explanation that ‘The 
doctors need to know more about these things. They need your help, so 
they can help other people.” The underworld requires only a gesture of 
honest friendship before it is ready to a^imit one as a friend, and to give 
histories “because you are my friend.” For each group the mode of the 
appeal is different, but in each case it is based on the measure of altruism 
that is to be found — if one knows how to find it — in nearly ail men. 

In answer to our request for her history, the little, gray-haired woman 
at the cabin door, out on the Western plain, epitomized what we have 
heard now from hundreds of people: “Of all things — ! In all my years I 
have never had such a question put to me! But — ^if my experience will help. 
I’ll give it to you.” This, in many forms, some of them simple, some of them 
sophisticated as scientists and scholars like them, some of them crude, 
incisive, and abrupt as the underworld makes them, is the expression of 
the altruistic bent (however philosophers and scientists may analyze it) 
which has been the chief motive leading people to cooperate in this study. 
We shall always be indebted “to the twelve thousand persons who have 
contributed to these data, and to the eighty-eight thousand more who, 
some day, will help complete this study.” However involved the reader 
may become in the statistics, the fine points of the argument, and the grand 
intricacies of the minute details, he will never understand this study until 
he comprehends the human drama that has been involved in securing the 
data. 

In an honest way, we have tried to make those who have contributed 
aware of our amazement at their willingness to help, and of our esteem 



INTERVIEWING 


37 


for them because they have helped. This appreciation has, undoubtedly, 
been a factor in winning cooperation. Evident appreciation may, therefore, 
belong in the list of devices which may be employed to secure histories; 
but appreciation must be sincere, else it will not work. 

More selfish interests have animated many of those who have contrib- 
uted. This is understandable, too. Many of the subjects have welcomed the 
opportunity to obtain information about some item affecting their personal 
lives, their marriages, their families, friends, or social relations. The more 
frequent questions have concerned: 

Possibly harmful effects from “excessive” sexual activity 
Physical harm resulting from masturbation 

Incidences of masturbation, pre-marital intercourse, extra-marital inter- 
course, mouth-genital contacts, homosexual relations, animal con- 
tacts 

Comparisons of the individual’s activities with averages for the group 
to which he belongs 
“Am I normal?” 

The physical and social significances of petting 

The relation of pre-mantal experience to subsequent adjustment in 
marriage 

Items to consider in choosing a mate for marriage 
Differences between male and female responsiveness 
Techniques conducive to mutuality of response in marital intercourse 
Medical aspects of contraception 

Data on the sexual development and education of children 
Problems arising from homosexual activity 

Information about available medical, psychiatric, or other chnics to 
which persons with special problems may be referred 
Impotency, heredity of physical defects, worries over genital characters, 
venereal disease, pregnancy (but these items only occasionally) 

As scientists, the authors of this volume have given information when it 
was available and scientifically established, while refusing to advise on any 
choice of behavior. Nevertheless, many persons have felt that the informa- 
tion obtained was sufficient repayment for their own contributions to the 
study. 

In a number of communities, public knowledge of this source of help 
has brought many histories. This does not mean that an undue number 
of neurotic or psychotic individuals has contributed. On the contrary, 
items of the sort listed above are the everyday sexual problems of the 
average individual; and the greatly disturbed type of person who goes to 
psychiatric clinics has been relatively rare in our sample. We have refused 
to take histories from recognizable psychotics who were handicapped with 
poor memories, hallucinations, or fantasies that distorted the fact. 



38 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


The psychoanalyst will incline to the view that most of those who have 
given histories have obtained some inflation of their egos by doing so. 
This is undoubtedly true, whether the record was one of unusual prowess, 
of conformance with the norm, or of low rates of activity which were the 
result of some incapacity for which the individual wanted pity. Most 
clinicians find that people like to talk about themselves. On the other hand, 
there is no evidence that this human quality has distorted the record, and 
the exaltation of one’s self has not seemed as significant as the altruistic 
motives which have animated most of the subjects — unless altruism is, of 
course, merely another means of self gratification. 

There are some who have contributed histories in order to satisfy their 
curiosity as to the nature of the questions asked, and to learn how such an 
interview is conducted. Several hundred psychoanalysts, psychiatrists, 
physicians, clinical psychologists, social workers, and other professional 
persons have had an especial interest in observing the interviewing tech- 
niques. In communities where we have worked for periods of time, persons 
of every social level, even including the lowest, have volunteered in order 
to find out what sort of thing their friends were experiencing when they 
contributed histories. 

In many instances, cooperation with the study may be made a group 
activity. To accomplish this, an interviewer must utilize the principles of 
mass psychology, mix them well with common sense, and add the skills of 
a patent medicine vendor and a Fuller brwsh man — while, withal, maintain- 
ing the community’s esteem for the dignity of a science wliich has nothing 
to sell. Members of a college fraternity, a sorority, a church organization, 
a parent-teacher group, a service club, all of the inmates of a penal institu- 
tion, the patients of a particular physician, all of the persons in some section 
of a city, all of the population in some rural community, may be persuaded 
to contribute as a matter of loyalty to an activity which is officially or 
tacitly supported by the group. In this way many persons have been reached 
who, as lone individuals, would have had little interest in the research. 
Loyalty to the group may also lead an individual to exercise more than 
usual care in providing a detailed and accurate record of his activity. 

Lectures to college, professional, church, and other community groups 
have most frequently provided the entree to the better educated portions 
of the population. Hundreds of such lectures have been given. Perhaps 
50,000 persons have heard about the research through lectures, and per- 
haps half of the histories now in hand have come in consequence of such 
contacts. 

Practically all of the contacts at lower levels, and many of those at other 
levels, have depended upon introductions made by persons who had pre- 
viously contributed their own histories. One who has not already given a 
history is not usually effective as a “contact man.” Contact men and women 



INTERVIEWING 


39 


have often spent considerable time and have gone to considerable pains to 
interest their friends and acquaintances. Many hundreds of such persons 
have helped, but a short list of those who have helped most will show 
something of the diversity of the backgrounds which have been repre- 
sented : 


Bootleggers 

Clergymen 

Clerks 

Clinical psychologists 
College professors 
College students 
Corporation officials 
Editors 
Farmers 

Female prostitutes 
Gamblers 

Headmasters of private schools 

Housewives 

Lawyers 

Male prostitutes 

Marriage counselors 


Ne’er-do-wells 

Persons in the Social Register 

Physicians 

Pimps 

Police court officials 
Prison inmates 
Prison officials 
Professional women 
Psychiatrists 
Public school teachers 
Social workers 
Thieves and hold-up men 
Y.M.C.A. secretaries 
Y.W.C.A. secretaries 
Welfare workers 
Women’s Club leaders 


In securing histories through personal introductions, it is initially most 
important to identify these key individuals, win their friendship, and 
develop their interest in the research. Days and weeks and even some years 
may be spent in acquiring the first acquaintances in a community. In a 
sober rural area, the most highly esteemed of the local clergymen may be 
the right person to sponsor the project. If it is a prison population, the 
oldest-timer, the leading wolf, the kingpin in the inmate commonwealth, 
or the girl who is the chief trouble-maker for the administration must be 
won before one can go very far in securing the histories of other inmates. 
If it is a good residential area in a large city, the quiet but steady young 
housewife with a host of friends who know they can count on her, or the 
sociable and reasonably successful middle-aged business man who is active 
in service clubs and civic projects, is the person most likely to put us across 
in that community. If it is the underworld, we may look for the man with 
the longest FBI record and the smallest number of convictions, and set out 
to win him. To get the initial introductions, it is necessary to become 
acquainted with someone who knows someone who knows the person we 
want to meet. Contacts may develop from the most unexpected sources. A 
rich man may provide the introduction to a leader in the underworld, a 
Salvation Army worker may serve as the contact for the Social Register. 
The number of persons who can provide introductions has continually 
spread until now, in the present study, we have a network of connections 
that could put us into almost any group with which we wished to work, 
anywhere in the country. 

Having met these significant persons, and gotten their histories, we take 
time to become acquainted with them and with their communities. They 



40 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


must come to like us as individuals, and the whole community must know 
about us and about the research, if we expect to secure any large number 
of histories in the area. We go with them to dinner, to concerts, to night 
clubs, to the theater; we become acquainted with them at community 
dances, in poolrooms, in taverns, and in other places which they frequent. 
They in turn invite us to meet friends in their homes, at teas, at dinners, at 
other social events. For years we have maintained a considerable corre- 
spondence with persons who are likely sources of new contacts. In many 
cases we have developed friendships which are based upon mutual respect 
and upon our common interest in the success of this project. When, in the 
course of time, we turn to securing histories from the rest of the community, 
most of the people who then contribute do so because they are friends of 
our friends, because they accept the contact man’s evaluation of the 
research, and because of their confidence that he would not involve them 
in difficulties. Among more poorly educated groups, and among such 
minority groups as rural populations, Negroes, segregated Jewish popula- 
tions, homosexual groups, penal institutional inmates, the underworld, etc., 
the community is particularly sensitive to the dangers of outside interfer- 
ence, and particularly dependent upon the advice of their leaders in decid- 
ing whether they should cooperate. 

An element of competition may be introduced by working two groups 
simultaneously or in immediate succession. College A contributes because 
College B has also contributed. College B is persuaded to cooperate 
because College A has already contributed more histories than B. The 
principle works equally well for fraternity and sorority groups, for people 
living in different houses on a city block, for the inmates of a penal institu- 
tion, for the court judge and his staff, for groups of psychiatrists, and for 
many other groups. 

There may be a certain amount of pressure employed in securing his- 
tories from the last persons in any group which is contributing a hundred 
percent to the study. Sometimes the pressure has originated from the 
investigators, more often it has been the group interest which has swayed 
the individual. There has been some constraint upon professional people, 
especially upon those who are involved in giving sexual advice in clinics, 
to contribute to a research project which will serve them in their profes- 
sional activities. Some of the histories obtained from inmates in institutions 
probably would not have been obtained except for the institutional tradi- 
tion of conformance to the administration’s program, or to the group 
activity in which all the other inmates were cooperating. Where such 
indirect or more direct pressure is employed it becomes particularly 
important to establish a satisfactory rapport with each subject after he 
has actually come into conference for a history. 

Payment for histories has been confined to the economically poorer 
elements in the population, to persons who are professionally involved in 



INTERVIEWING 


41 


sexual activities (as prostitutes, pimps, exhibitionists, etc.) or to others who 
have turned from their regular occupation and spent considerable time in 
helping make contacts. The payment has never been large, rarely amount- 
ing to more than a dollar or two for the couple of hours involved in con- 
tributing; and equivalent amounts may be paid to persons who have 
helped make the contacts. There is no evidence that such payments have 
distorted the quality of the record, although the prospect of securing double 
payment leads an occasional individual to try to duplicate his contribution. 
In the latter case, it has been necessary to keep accurate records and require 
identification; but this has presented only a minor problem. On the whole, 
payment has worked well, for it has undoubtedly made it possible to secure 
many histories which otherwise would not have been obtainable; and it 
should be realized that even in the groups which are paid, men and women 
have contributed primarily because they respect us, because they appre- 
ciate our interest in them, and because they are willing to contribute for 
the sake of helping others. Certain it is that the remarkable body of con- 
fidential information that has been secured from some of these lower level 
and underworld groups would not have been available if there had been 
no other bases than money to interest them. Twelve thousand people 
have helped in this research primarily because they have faith in scientific 
research projects. 

ESTABLISHING RAPPORT 

There are, after all, only two reasons why anyone should hesitate to 
contribute his sex history to a scientific project. He may hesitate because 
he fears that the interviewer will object to something in his history, and he 
may fear a loss of social prestige, or legal penalties, if his history were to 
become a matter of public knowledge. An occasional individual has 
hesitated, in addition, because he did not want to stir up memories of old 
fears, old hurts, or old losses that were associated with his or her sexual 
life; occasionally a psychotic — or simply a contrary individual — has 
blocked at cooperating; but most persons who have hesitated have done 
so because they feared embarrassment before the interviewer, or feared 
public disclosure of their activities. 

It is imperative, therefore, that the investigator be able to convince the 
subject: 

1. That he, as a scientist, offers no objection to any type of sexual 
behavior in which the subject could possibly have been involved. 

2. That the confidences of the record wiU be kept without question. 

A scientist studying sex should be able to accept any type of sexual 
behavior objectively, listen to the record without adverse reaction, and 
record without social or moral evaluation. That much is expected of the 
student measuring the lengths of insect wings, recording the chemical 
changes that occur in a test tube, or observing the colors of the stars. It is 



42 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


not too much to expect similar objectivity of the student, of human 
behavior. 

But something more than cold objectivity is needed in dealing with 
human subjects. One is not likely to win the sort of rapport which brings 
a full and frank confession from a human subject unless he can convince 
the subject that he is desperately anxious to comprehend what his experi- 
ence has meant to him. Sexual histories often involve a record of things 
that have hurt, of frustrations, of pain, of unsatisfied longings, of dis- 
appointments, of desperately tragic situations, and of complete catas- 
trophe, The subject feels that the investigator who asks merely routine 
questions has no right to know about such things in another’s history. The 
interviewer who senses what these things can mean, who at least momen- 
tarily shares something of the satisfaction, pain, or bewilderment which 
Was the subject’s, who shares something of the subject's hope that things 
will, somehow, work out right, is more effective, though he may not be 
altogether neutral. 

The sympathetic interviewer records his reactions in ways that may not 
involve spoken words but which are, nonetheless, readily comprehended 
by most people. A minute change of a facial expression, a slight tensing of 
a muscle, the flick of an eye, a trace of a change in one’s voice, a slight 
inflection or change in emphasis, slight changes in one’s rate of speaking, 
slight hesitancies in putting a question or in following up with the next 
question, one’s choice of words, one’s spontaneity in inquiring about items 
that are off the usual routine, or any of a dozen and one other involuntary 
reactions betray the interviewer’s emotions, and most subjects quickly 
understand them. Unlettered persons and persons of mentally lower levels 
are often particularly keen in sensing the true nature of another person’s 
reactions. 

If the interviewer’s manner spells surprise, disapproval, condemnation, 
or even cold disinterest, he will not get the whole of the record. If his 
reactions add up right, then the subject is willing to tell his story. The 
interview has become an opportunity for him to develop his own thinking, 
to express to himself his own disappointments and hopes, to bring into the 
open things that he has previously been afraid to admit to himself, to work 
out solutions to his difficulties. He quickly comes to realize that a full and 
complete confession will serve his own interests. It becomes unthinkable 
that he should cover up, deny, or fail to relate anything that has happened. 

^ These are the things that can be done in a person-to-person, guided inter- 
view that represents a communion between two deeply human individuals, 
the subject and the interviewer (McNemar 1946). These are the things that 
can never be done through a written questionnaire, or even through a 
directed interview in which the questions are formalized and the confines 
of the investigation strictly limited. In the present study, the number of 



INTERVIEWING 


43 


persons who have admitted involvement in every type of sexual activity, 
and particularly in socially taboo types of activity, is much greater 
than has ever been disclosed in any questionnaire study; and com- 
parisons of the data in this and in previously published studies should 
provide some measure of the possibilities of personal interviewing as a 
technique in case history studies. 

It has been asked how it is possible for an interviewer to know whether 
people are telling the truth, when they are boasting, when they are covering 
up, or when they are otherwise distorting the record. As well ask a horse 
trader how he knows when to close a bargain! The experienced interviewer 
knows when he has established a sufficient rapport to obtain an honest 
record, in the same way that the subject knows that he can give that honest 
record to the interviewer. Learning to recognize these indicators, intangible 
as they may be, is the most important thing in controlling the accuracy of 
an interview. Beyond that there are cross-checks among the questions, 
inconsistencies to watch for, questions which demand proof, and other 
devices for testing the validity of the data (all of which are discussed in the 
last section of the present chapter). 

At the beginning of an interview the subject must be assured that he can 
tell all, but it is not always possible to win complete rapport at the very 
start. The subject will need to be reassured many times in the course of the 
interview, and continually convinced by the evident sympathy of the inter- 
viewer. Often the subject begins >by admitting only a small part of his 
activity, and adds more only gradually as he becomes more certain that he 
can do so without disapproval. — “Yes, I have been approached for such 
relations, but I did not pay attention.”— “Yes, there were physical contacts, 
but they did not interest me.”— “Yes, there were complete contacts— 
when I was asleep,” — “Yes, there was one affair in which I responded, in 
a mild way.”— “Yes, I liked it well enough, but I didn’t think I wanted any 
more of it.” — “Well, yes, I did try it again.” — “Yes, since then I have 
become interested, and I have had a good deal of it lately.” — So the history 
builds up. At each step the subject intended to stop with the minimum of 
information, and would have stopped completely if there had been any 
indication that the interviewer was surprised, was offended, or disapproved. 
After each essay, the additional bit of information was added because the 
subject discovered that he could tell more. If, at any point, the interviewer 
had failed, the story would have stopped there. 

Sometimes the capacity of the investigator is severely tested. Whatever 
his sexual background, each person reaches the limit of things he can 
understand because of his own previous experience, the limit of things he 
can appear to understand because he has wanted them and would have had 
them if it had been socially expedient, and the limit of things he can sym- 
pathetically admit because he has glimpsed what they have meant to some 



44 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Other people. Beyond that there are always things which seem esthetically 
repulsive, provokingly petty, foolish, unprofitable, senseless, unintelligent, 
dishonorable, contemptible, or socially destructive. Gradually one learns 
to forego judgments on these things, and to accept them merely as facts 
for the record. If one fails in his acceptance, he will know of it by the 
sudden confusion or sudden tenseness of his subject, and the quick con- 
clusion of the story. If the interviewer masters his own confusion, the sub- 
ject may tell him about it, and congratulate him to boot for being able 
and willing to “take it.” 

The many persons who have contributed to this study have done so 
voluntarily and with a full understanding of what we were trying to learn 
through our questioning. To have used any sort of devious device would 
have ruined the subject’s confidence in everything we were doing. It has 
repeatedly been suggested that we try narcosynthesis, lie detectors, or 
other such means for testing the reliability of at least some of the answers 
of some of the subjects; but if we had coerced a single person by any such 
means, we would have lost our capacity to win things from anyone else. In 
any study which needs to secure quantities of data from human subjects, 
there is no way except to win their voluntary cooperation through the 
establishment of that intangible thing known as rapport. 

THE CONFIDENCE OF THE RECORD 

Our laws and customs are so far removed from the actual behavior of 
the human animal that there are few persons who can afford to let their 
full histories be known to the courts or even to their neighbors and their 
best friends ; and persons who are expected to disclose their sex histories 
must be assured that the record will never become known in connection 
with them as individuals. Each subject in this study has contributed only 
because he has been thus assured by a friend whom he trusts, or by the 
investigator at the beginning of the interview. It is important to note, how- 
ever, that assuring one of the confidence of the record can be effective only 
when that assurance is honest in its intent and never, under any circum- 
stance, betrayed in its execution. If there were ever a single failure to main- 
tain such confidence, then others would learn of it and refuse to contribute 
histories. The care with which confidences have been guarded in the present 
study has probably never been surpassed in any other project dealing with 
human material. 

Keeping confidence in this study has involved the development of a 
cryptic code in which all of the data have been recorded (Chapter 3, 
Figure 2). The code is never translated into words at any stage in the 
analyses of the data. Each interviewer has memorized the code, and there 
is no key to the code in existence. Only the six persons who have actually 
taken histories have ever known any part of the code, and only four 
persons are, at the present writing, acquainted with the whole code. None 



INTERVIEWING 


45 


of the other persons who have helped in the technical work in our labora- 
tory knows the code. A few routine tabulations of non-sexual items have 
been made by the technical assistants ; but practically all of the handling 
of the data, including the punching of the Hollerith cards and their manipu- 
lation in the IBM statistical machines, has been done by those of us who 
have taken the histories. Never in the nine years of this research has any 
other person had access to the information available on the histories of 
particular individuals. 

It has been necessary to preserve the identity of each history in order to 
make subsequent additions, in order to compare re-takes of histories 
(Chapter 4), and in order to coordinate data coming froin two or more 
persons involved in the same sexual activities (as spouses in a marriage or 
heterosexual or homosexual companions in common sexual activities). 
This identification has been accomplished by the use of a coded set of 
symbols for which, again, there is no key in existence. The code was 
developed with the help of an experienced cryptographer and involves, 
simultaneously, the use of several devices designed to complicate possible 
decoding. It is the judgment of the cryptographer who tried to break the 
final form that decoding would be impossible unless one had access to all 
of the histories and all of the files for a considerable period of time; and 
that after identification the data would be practically unintelligible because 
of the difficulty of deciphering such a position code as the one used here. 
It should be added that the histories are kept behind locked doors and in 
fireproof files with locks that are inique for this project. 

To a very large degree, analyzing this material has involved additions 
of data, summations of the numbers of persons engaged in particular 
activities, tabulations of ages, tabulations of frequencies, totals of other 
data, and correlations of facts and factors ; and, as taxonomists interested 
in the behavior of whole segments of the population, we do not foresee 
that we will ever be concerned with the publication of the particular his- 
tories of particular individuals. It has, therefore, been possible to guaran- 
tee that no history will ever be published in a form which would identify it 
as an individual history. It has been possible to explain the safety of this 
mode of publication even to uneducated and mentally duller individuals, 
and thus to persuade them that they can safely contribute histories. 

Individual histories in this project have been discussed only among the 
research associates on this staff. They have not been discussed even with 
professional friends outside of the staff. Particular histories have not been 
used as illustrations in public lectures or in group conversation, although 
examples have sometimes been synthesized from real cases. They have not 
been discussed even when the individuals involved were geographically so 
remote as to seem unidentifiable; for people travel about over the country, 
and one often meets persons who are acquainted with one’s subjects in 
some distant town. 



46 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Few professional people seem to know what it means to preserve the 
absolute confidence of a record. Professional confidence too often refers 
to the discussion of individual cases with anyone in the professional 
fraternity. Such discussions, often in the hearing of secretaries or nurses, 
soon spread the information abroad, whence it returns to confound the 
subject who gave his history only after he was guaranteed strict confidence. 
Professional people connected with courts too often obtain confessions by 
promising the confidence of the record, which they promptly betray by 
carrying the data to the court. Academic persons doing research on human 
case histories regularly turn them over to graduate students to be studied, 
and on occasion exhibit them, with names attached, to whole classes of 
students for examination. Few clinical records are ever in code, and in 
very few cases is there any attempt to separate the name of the subject 
from the plainly written record. In many a social welfare agency there are 
more non-professional persons who see the confidential records than there 
are professional people in the organization. In penal institutions there are 
always inmates who are employed in clerical positions, where they have 
access to the “confidential” records; and information spreads through 
them to the whole of the inmate body. In some institutions the inmate 
population is better acquainted with the content of some of these records 
than the officials themselves. Persons who have been betrayed through 
such sources become, naturally enough, skeptical about contributing fur- 
ther data to any professional person, and it has often been difficult to 
convince them that our own records wduld be kept inviolate. 

We have been pressed by many people for information about particular 
persons who have contributed histories. Husbands and wives often want 
data about their spouses, and in many cases such information would help 
them make better marital adjustments; but if such information were ever 
given, other husbands and wives would not be willing to give their histories. 
Parents ask about their children, and partners in common sexual activities 
often want advice which cannot be given without drawing upon the con- 
fidential record. While it has disappointed many persons not to secure 
such information, their esteem for the integrity of the records has inevi- 
tably increased and, in consequence, they are then willing to contribute 
their own histories, and to interest their friends in the project. 

In penal and other institutions, we have maintained an invariable rule 
that no confidence given by an inmate would ever be passed on to the 
administration of the institution. We have worked only in institutions 
which have accepted us on these terms. No administration has ever asked 
us to break the rule. In a few cases where we have felt that some inmate 
would find it to his advantage to have the administration know more of 
his history, we have advised the subject to that effect and, if he has agreed, 
we have helped make such contacts — but only when he has voluntarily 
agreed to such a procedure. 



INTERVIEWING 


47 


There is probably no legal right for anyone to preserve the confidence 
of any information which has been given him. By custom the courts 
ordinarily recognize the rights of a priest or of a physician to preserve 
confidences obtained in the performance of their professional duties, but 
there is no statute law establishing such a right. If we were brought before 
a court we would have to hope that such precedents would be extended to 
scientists involved in the investigation of such a subject as human sex 
behavior. If the courts of all levels were to refuse to recognize such a 
privilege, there would be no alternative but to destroy our complete body 
of records and accept the consequences of such defiance of the courts. If 
law enforcement officials, students of law, and persons interested in social 
problems want scientific assistance in understanding such problems, they 
will have to recognize a scientist’s right to maintain the absolute confidence 
of his records ; for without that it would be impossible to persuade persons 
to contribute to this sort of study. 

TECHNICAL DEVICES IN INTERVIEWING 

The skillful interviewer will develop particular techniques which work 
for him, though they may not serve another investigator so well. But even 
though there are these differences in the applicability of particular methods, 
it may be of value to other persons who are interested in interviewing to 
know something of the technical devices that have proved effective in the 
present research. ^ 

1. Putting the subject at ease. Many of the persons who contribute to 
a sex study manifest some uneasiness at the beginning of an interview, and 
from the start particular attention needs to be given to putting the subject 
at ease. Interviews are held in places that are as attractive and comfortable 
as the subject’s social background may demand. Many persons are con- 
siderably helped if they can smoke during the interview. Conversation is 
initiated as casually as possible, first about everyday affairs that may be 
remote from anything that is sexual. If the subject knows someone whom 
we have previously known, or has recreational interests which are in any 
way connected with our own, that provides a basis for conversation. One 
does the sort of things that a thoughtful host would do to make his guests 
comfortable, but always easily so that the subject is not aware that they 
are designed to put him at ease. 

2. Assuring privacy. Places where the interviews are held should be 
reasonably soundproof, and there should be no unexpected interruptions 
from other persons entering the room.* From the very set-up of the inter- 
view, the subject must be reassured of confidence. 

* In contrast, we have observed a psychiatrist interviewing a subject, who was under 
criminal indictment, in a small room in which a half dozen persons were continually 
moving about and listening to the whole of the conversation. 

3 



48 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


3, Establishing rapport. The subject should be treated as a friend or 
a guest in one’s own home. The tottering old man who is a victim of his 
first penal conviction, appreciates the interviewer’s solicitation about his 
health and his interest in seeing that he is provided with tobacco, candy, 
or the other things that the institution allows one who has sufficient funds. 
The inmate in a woman’s penal institution particularly appreciates those 
courtesies which a male would extend to a woman of his own social rank, 
in his own home. The interviewer should be as interested in the subject as 
he is in recording the subject’s history. It is important to look the subject 
squarely in the eye, while giving only a minimum of attention to the record 
that is being made. People understand each other when they look directly 
at each other. 

4. Sequence of topics. Since, as already indicated, it is often necessary 
to build up rapport after an interview has actually started, it is advisable 
to begin a conference with the items that are non-sexual and least likely to 
disturb the subject, and to follow with a sequence which leads gradually 
into things that the subject may consider more difficult to discuss. Those 
things on which one may expect the maximum cover-up and blockage 
should be left until the end of the interview. By then the subject has 
acquired confidence, and it is possible to secure a record of things that 
could never have been secured at the beginning of the interview. In the 
present study we usually begin with a discussion of the subject’s age, place 
of birth, educational history, recreational interests, physical health, paren- 
tal background, brothers, sisters, and other non-sexual data. The first 
sexual items are those for which the subject is least responsible, namely the 
sources of his sex education. The record of overt sexual activities begins 
with the things that are most remote, such as the pre-adolescent sex play. 
From there on the sequence of topics is varied in accordance with the 
subject’s social background, his age, and his educational level. For unmar- 
ried college males the sequence is nocturnal emissions, masturbation, 
pre-marital petting, pre-marital intercourse with companions, intercourse 
with prostitutes, animal contacts, and the homosexual. For males who 
have never gone beyond the tenth grade in school, pre-marital intercourse 
can be discussed much earUer in the interview, because it is generally 
accepted at that social level; but masturbation needs to be approached 
more carefully if one is to get the truth from that group. At that level, 
petting is secondary to intercourse in interest and in acceptance, and it is 
brought into an interview only after the discussion of intercourse. 

With many females it is simpler to get a record of the homosexual than 
a record of masturbatory activity. For the older generation of males of 
every social level it is simpler to get a record of pre-marital intercourse than 
to get a record of masturbation. With persons who have publicly known 
homosexual histories, extensive masochistic or sadistic experience, his- 
tories as prostitutes, or other special sorts of sex experience, we get better 



INTERVIEWING 


49 


cooperation when we take the record of the special experience before trying 
to get details on the more usual activities. It is often easier to get the pro- 
fessional record from a female prostitute than it is to get the record of her 
personal sex life with her boy friend or with her husband. In dealing with 
an uneducated and timid older woman from a remote farm area or moun- 
tain country, the sequence has to become most desultory, including only 
the simplest questions about each type of sexual experience, with no details 
on any point until the whole of the history has been covered in a prelimi- 
nary way. By then the subject should have become more confident, and it 
will be possible to ask her such details about each type of activity as would 
have shocked her at the beginning of the interview. A good interviewer 
becomes very sensitive to the reactions of his subjects, immediately drops 
any line of inquiry which causes embarrassment, and stays with simpler 
matters until the subject is ready to talk in more detail. This technique, 
more than anything else, probably accounts for the fact that among the 
12,000 persons who have been interviewed in the present study, all but 
three or four have completed their histories ; and those few would not have 
been lost if we had known as much at the beginning of this study as we 
now know about a good sequence of questions in an interview. 

5. Recognizing the subject’s mental status. One should not attempt 
to take a history from a subject who is mentally incapacitated, whether 
permanently or temporarily so. Persons who are badly intoxicated cannot 
give reliable histories ; and while it is impossible to rule out all who have 
been drinking, since that would^'ule out a high proportion of all persons 
in certain social groups, one must learn to identify the level of intoxication 
of his subjects and avoid taking a record that is below standard. Some 
individuals, of course, are more cooperative when they have had a small 
amount of alcohol. Persons who are under the immediate influence of 
drugs, particularly of some narcotic that induces sleep, are impossible as 
subjects. A person who is heavily intoxicated with marihuana (which is 
not a drug) is similarly unrehable. Benzedrine and some other drugs are 
not so hkely to interfere with the individual’s capacity to give an accurate 
record. Persons who are physically exhausted or mentally fatigued are 
difficult, and some older persons who are badly senile are hopeless. 

Feeble-minded individuals vary considerably in their capacities to 
remember things. There are some whose memories are accurate on details, 
and this is also true of many uneducated persons who are not feeble- 
minded. Consequently, the interviewer must learn to identify a feeble- 
minded case and must preface the interview with such ordinary, everyday 
conversation as will allow him to determine the capacities of such an 
individual. It is possible to get a fair record from most feeble-minded 
individuals whose IQ’s are not below 50, although interviewing any person 
with a rating below 70 becomes a slow process in which each idea must be 
given plenty of time to penetrate, with endless repetition, and with a 



50 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


vocabulary which is confined to the simplest of words, both in the sexual 
vernacular and in the references to commonplace activities. 

6. Recording at time of interview. In the literature on interviewing, it 
is customary to advise that records should not be made in the presence of a 
subject, but that they should be made after the subject has left at the close 
of an interview. This is the commonest procedure among many psychia- 
trists, clinical psychologists, and among social workers. It is supposed that 
a subject is embarrassed at seeing his statements put on paper, and that 
he will talk more freely if he feels that he can say some things that are not 
recorded. It has been said that there is a loss of rapport when the inter- 
viewer records during the interview. For these reasons we attempted to 
follow standard practice early in this study and found that it introduced a 
tremendous error into the records. Much of the specific quotation which 
appears in psychiatric literature evidently represents the interview'er’s 
notion of how the subject talked rather than a record of what the subject 
actually said. After the first few months of this study, we began to record 
all of the data directly in the presence of the subject, and there has been 
no indication that this has been responsible for any loss of rapport or 
interference with the subject’s free exposure of confidences. We have 
become convinced that any loss of rapport which comes when data are 
recorded directly has been consequent upon the longhand method of 
writing out answers while the subject sits in silence waiting for the next 
question. This is the thing that is destructive to rapport. By using a code 
for recording, it has been possible in the present study to record as rapidly 
as one can carry on a conversation, without loss of rapport or blockage on 
the subject’s part. 

7. Systematic coverage. On each history in the present study there has 
been a systematic coverage of a basic minimum of about 300 items. This 
minimum is expanded for persons who have extended experience in pre- 
marital or extra-marital intercourse, who have extended homosexual his- 
tories, who have had experience as prostitutes or pimps, or who have had 
multiple marriages. The maximum history covers 521 items. One of the 
most fundamental aspects of the present survey is its systematic coverage 
of a uniform list of items on each history. Such coverage is not possible in 
a free association procedure where the subject records things as they 
happen to come to his mind and where, in consequence, each person may 
provide information on some items that are not covered by the next persons 
in the study, and where, in consequence, it is impossible to add together 
and secure incidence or frequency figures that would be applicable even to 
the sample population as a whole. The use of a standard form in coding 
the data (Figure 2) makes it possible to look over the history sheet at the 
end of an interview, and to make sure that every block in which there 
should be some record has been satisfactorily accounted for. 



INTERVIEWING 


51 


8. Supplementary exploration. While there be a basic minimum of 
material that is covered on each history, the interviewer should not hesitate 
to secure additional data on special situations that are outside of the 
routine. For instance, about twenty questions are routinely asked about 
masturbation, but there are males who have developed elaborate tech- 
niques about which scores of additional questions should be put. Concern- 
ing the average individual’s relations with his parents, the routine questions 
may give all the necessary information on most individuals, but an occa- 
sional individual may have had some complex relation with his parents 
which makes it valuable to get the record in more detail. In taking the 
histories of identical twins, especial inquiry is made about their emotional 
relations to each other, the extent to which they share common social 
activities, and other items which are not touched in the usual history. A 
highly intelligent individual who has had considerable experience in a 
socially taboo type of behavior may help analyze the situation in such detail 
as is never investigated in the average history. In the routine, there are 
only minor questions on masochism and sadism, but if there is any indica- 
tion that a subject has been consciously and deliberately involved in such 
behavior, he should be questioned on scores of items which are not in the 
basic interview. Persons who are blind, deaf, crippled, or otherwise handi- 
capped, persons who have lived in foreign countries, persons who have 
had experience in military groups or who have Jived in other special 
situations, similarly become sources of special information. As scientific 
explorers, we, in the present stildy, have been unlimited in our search to 
find out what people do sexually. These, again, are the things that can be 
done in a guided interview and which cannot be touched in a questionnaire 
study or even in a directed interview. 

9. Standardizing the point of the question. In the present study, the 
questions asked in the interviews have never been standardized in form, 
but the points which they cover have been strictly defined. When the sub- 
ject is asked about his relations to his parents, there is a strict definition 
of the period to which the information should apply. When the question 
concerns the subject’s experience in petting, petting is precisely defined so 
there is no confusion about the sort of experience which may be included 
imder that head. When the subject’s relations with prostitutes are the issue, 
a clear distinction is made between a prostitute and a girl who is merely 
promiscuous. Data about the health of an individual are designed to catch 
those illnesses which interfere with an individual’s social adjustment. Each 
other question has had its point precisely defined, in order that the data 
secured from the many different subjects and by the several interviewers 
may fairly be added together. It is unfortunate that the limitations of space 
make it impossible to give the whole list of questions, with their precise 
definitions, anywhere in the present volume, although a list of items covered 
in each interview is shown in Chapter 3. 



52 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


10. Adapting the form of the question. While the point of each question 
has been standardized, the form of each question has varied for the 
various social levels and for the various types of persons with whom the 
study has dealt. Standardized questions do not bring standardized answers, 
for the same question means different things to different people. In order 
to have questions mean the same thing to different people, they must be 
modified to fit the vocabulary, the educational background, and the com- 
prehension of each subject. It is especially important to use a vocabulary 
with which the subject will feel at home, and which he will understand. 
The college-bred interviewer needs to go to considerable pains to limit his 
vocabulary to the relatively few words that are employed by persons in 
lower educational levels. Everyday terms, as well as sexual vernaculars, 
are involved: a lower level individual, for instance, is never ill or injured, 
though he may be sick or hurt. He does not wish to do a thing, though he 
wants to do it. He does not perceive, though he sees. He is not acquainted 
with a person, though he may know him. One needs a certain sensitivity 
to adapt his vocabulary to the limited usages of such subjects. Except 
among college graduates, there is little knowledge of the clinical terms that 
concern sex, and sexual vernaculars must be used in interviewing lower 
level individuals. Such vernaculars vary considerably among different 
groups. 

One must know and use the vernacular terms with a fine sense of their 
proprieties and their exact meanings in ^.ach group. Their awkward use 
may damage instead of building rapport. Sexual vernaculars differ con- 
siderably in different sections of the country. One should take considerable 
pains to determine the precise meanings of the variant terms as soon as he 
starts work in a community. Negro and white groups differ in their usages 
even in the same city. There are differences between the vocabularies of 
older and younger generations in any social level. A volume could be 
written on the vernaculars that should be known by anyone attempting to 
deal with people outside of his own social level, and the training of inter- 
viewers for the present study has involved a considerable amount of work 
on that point. Everywhere questions must be varied so they will bring 
replies that pertain to exactly the same thing. In many instances, questions 
must be freely expanded in order to make their meanings clear to the sub- 
ject. This again is the sort of thing which cannot be done with a question- 
naire or with a directed interview in which the questions are standardized 
as to form. 

11. Avoiding bias. In a study in which the forms of the questions are 
not standardized, there is a considerable responsibility on the interviewer 
to see to it that his spontaneous questions are not so phrased as to bias 
the subject’s reply (McNemar 1946). In his tone of voice and in his choice 
of words, the interviewer must avoid giving the subject any clue as to the 



INTERVIEWING 


53 


answers he expects. For instance, when the subject is at a loss to know how 
to estimate the frequencies with which he has engaged in a particular sort 
of activity, the interviewer can explain what sorts of frequencies are pos- 
sible, provided he is careful not to give any idea what frequencies are com- 
mon in the population, or what frequencies he, the subject, might be ex- 
pected to have. What is actually done is to suggest to the subject that his 
activity might average once a week, three or four times a week, once a 
month, every day, or more often, or less often. The interviewer avoids 
suggesting an answer by avoiding any sequence in the illustrative list which 
he gives, and is careful not to attach particular importance to the last 
item in the list, which is the one that many persons will accept as their 
answer if the interviewer is not on his guard. Feeble-minded individuals 
and occasionally some other persons are highly suggestible, and then it 
becomes particularly important to avoid suggesting answers and important 
to test all answers for consistency. 

12. Direct questions. When one is deahng with such a socially involved 
question as sex it becomes particularly important to ask direct questions, 
without hesitancy and without apology. If the interviewer shows any un- 
certainty or embarrassment, it is not to be expected that the subject will do 
better in his answers. Euphemisms should not be used as substitutes for 
franker terms. In some of the previous studies, many sexual terms are 
avoided: masturbation becomes “touching yourself”; a climax in mas- 
turbation becomes “securing a* thrill through touching yourself”; and 
sexual intercourse becomes “relations with other persons,” or “sex delin- 
quency” (Ackerson 1931, 1942). With such questions the subject cannot 
help but sense the fact that the interviewer is not sure that sex is an honor- 
able thing, and a thing that can be frankly talked about. Evasive terms 
invite dishonest answers. In one of the previous studies there was a long 
list of questions concerning the homosexual, but the approach was so 
incfirect that a person who had had an abundance of such experience could 
have answered every one of the questions honestly, and still never have 
admitted that he had ever had an overt experience. 

13. Placing the burden of denial on the subject. The interviewer should 
not make it easy for a subject to deny his participation in any form of 
sexual activity. It is too easy to say no if he is simply asked whether he has 
ever engaged in a particular activity. We always assume that everyone has 
engaged in every type of activity. Consequently we always begin by asking 
when they first engaged in such activity. This places a heavier burden on 
the individual who is inchned to deny his experience ; and since it becomes 
apparent from the form of our question that we would not be surprised if 
he had had such experience, there seems to be less reason for denying it. 
It might be thought that this approach would bias the answer, but there 
is no indication that we get false admissions of participation in forms of 



54 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


sexual behavior in which the subject was not actually involved. Other 
techniques of modifying questions, particularly if they concern socially 
taboo behavior, may bring a considerable increase in the number of posi- 
tive answers. 

14. Avoiding multiple questions. Anyone experienced in teaching 
should have learned to avoid multiple questions. Multiple questions usually 
bring replies that are ambiguous, and their avoidance in a sex study is 
particularly important because they provide an opportunity for the subject 
to dodge one of the questions by giving his attention entirely to the other. 
For instance, the interviewer who asks the subject if he is erotically aroused 
“by seeing nude males or females,” may get as an answer that he is always 
aroused by seeing females. Thereby the subject manages to evade the fact 
that he is to some degree aroused by seeing males. 

15. Rapid-fire questioning. In order to cover the maximum amount of 
material in a single interview, it is necessary to ask questions as rapidly as 
the subject can possibly comprehend and reply. This method has the fur- 
ther advantage of forcing the subject to answer spontaneously without too 
much premeditation. Such a rapid fire of questions provides one of the 
most effective checks on fabrication, as detectives and other law-enforce- 
ment oflScials well know. It would be practically impossible for a person 
who was deliberately falsifying to answer the many questions that are 
asked concerning the details of his activity, when the questions come as 
rapidly as they do in our interviewing. Looking an individual squarely in 
the eye, and firing questions at him with maximum speed, are two of the 
best guarantees against exaggeration. 

16. Cross-checks on accuracy. Cover-up is more easily accomplished 
than exaggeration in giving a history. The best protection against cover-up 
lies in the use of a considerable list of interlocking questions which provide 
cross-checks throughout the history, and particularly in regard to socially 
taboo items. There are, for instance, twelve questions concerning homo- 
sexual experience that appear in each interview before direct questions on 
that point are ever asked. The significance of some of these preliminary 
questions would not be recognized by anyone except a skilled psychiatrist. 
It would be difiicult for most persons who had had anything more than 
incidental homosexual experience to deny that fact after they had answered 
the questions which provide the cross-checks on this point. There are 
similar cross-checks on various other aspects of a history. Such devices 
should be confined, however, to honest inquiries concerning items which 
are an integral part of the individuaFs history; and one should avoid setting 
traps that put the subject on the defensive because they are obvious de- 
vices for forcing him into an admission. 

Probably the most effective system of cross-checks has been the use of 
vocabularies that are peculiar to persons with particular sorts of experience, 



INTERVIEWING 


55 


and which are quite unknown to persons without such experience. For 
instance, when one asks a female subject "‘how many years she has been 
in the life,” she must betray an honest confusion and inability to under- 
stand, or else she identifies herself as a prostitute. There are special argots 
for practically all of the socially taboo activities; and they may provide 
checks on many of the persons who must be included in a human case his- 
tory study. Nevertheless, in spite of all that may be done, a certain amount 
of deliberate cover-up may slip by, and the investigator must find some 
means of measuring the extent of that cover-up in each part of his data. 

17. Proving the answer. If it becomes apparent that the subject’s first 
answer is not correct or sufficient, one should ask for additional informa- 
tion, and re-phrase the original question in a way that will make him 
prove his answer or expose the falsity of his reply. In a rapid fire of addi- 
tional questions, it is difficult for a dishonest subject to be consistent. With 
uneducated persons, and particularly with feeble-minded individuals, it is 
sometimes effective to pretend that one has misunderstood the negative 
repHes and ask additional questions, just as though the original answers 
were affirmatives; whereupon the subject may then expose the truth by 
answering as though he had never given a negative reply. “Yes, I know 
you have never done that, but how old were you the first time that you 
did it?” is a question which, amazingly enough, may break down the cover- 
up of a feeble-minded individual. With such a technique, on the other 
hand, it is especially important to make sure that the subject’s final 
admissions are not fictions which the interviewer has suggested to him. 

If the subject corrects his original answers, it should be made easy for 
him by ignoring the contradictions and receiving the new information as 
easily as though it were his first reply to the question. On a few occasions 
we have taken a complete history after we were convinced that it was a 
fraud, then laid it aside and suggested to the subject that he “now give it to 
us straight.” If the interviewer is sure enough of his interpretation of the 
situation, the protests of the subject can be quickly silenced and he will 
proceed to give a full and correct history. These falsified histories, in con- 
junction with the corrected records, are especially valuable documents 
because of the insight they give into an individual’s public acknowledg- 
ments, in contrast to his actual behavior. 

18. Forcing a subject. There are some persons who offer to contribute 
histories in order to satisfy their curiosity, although they have no inten- 
tions of giving an honest record of their sexual activities. As soon as one 
recognizes such a case, he should denounce the subject with considerable 
severity, and the interviewer should refuse to proceed with the interview. 
Such an attack on a dishonest subject is quite contrary to the usual rules 
for interviewing, and a procedure which we at first hesitated to employ in 
the present study. We have, however, decided that it is a necessary tech- 



56 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


nique in dealing with some individuals, particularly some older teen-age 
males and some females in underworld groups. Failure to command the 
situation in these cases would lower the community’s respect for the investi- 
gator and make it impossible for him to secure honest histories from others 
in the group. It must be understood by all concerned that giving a history 
is a voluntary matter, but that as soon as an individual agrees to contribute 
he assumes the responsibility of serving scientific accuracy. If the falsifica- 
tion is not recognized until after the interview, and if the individual is of 
importance in his community, the interviewer may well return to him and 
demand that he correct the record. The list of persons who have been 
forced in this fashion, in the present study, has included individuals in the 
underworld, feeble-minded subjects, prison inmates, and one clergyman. 
No history has ever been lost as a result of such action, and the study 
has won a number of staunch friends because of our insistence on 
scientific honesty. 

19. Limits of the interview. In spite of the long list of items included 
in the present study (Chapter 3) and in spite of the fact that each history 
has covered five times as much material as in any previous study, numerous 
students have suggested, and undoubtedly will continue to suggest after 
the publication of the present volume, that we should have secured more 
data in the fields of their special interests. Specifically it has been suggested 
that the following matters should have had more thorough investigation: 

Anthropology: Racial ancestry for several generations. A companion study on some 
culture other than American. The correlation of somatotypes and behav ior 

Endocrinology: Hormonal assays of at least some series of homosexual cases 

Gynecology: Physical examinations of the genitalia of each female subject 

Marriage Counseling: Non-sexual factors in marital adjustment 

Medicine: More complete histories of health and disease, and genital examinations 
of all male subjects 

Psychoanalysis: More data on early childhood, parental relations, etc. 

Psychology: More data on motivations and attitudes, and complete personality, 
intelligence, and masculinity-femininity tests 

Sociology : More detailed studies of cultural and community backgrounds ; a precise 
economic rating 

Urology: Sperm counts, more detailed genital measurements and records of defects 

We are quite conscious of the limitations of the data which we have 
secured, and would like to see intensive studies of all of the above subjects 
in their relations to sex. It is, however, physically impossible to undertake 
all collateral investigations while making a preliminary survey in any field. 
This is a taxonomic survey of the sexual behavior of a whole great section 
of mankind, and it has been necessary to limit the immediate study to 
those things which can be covered in the twenty-eight years assigned to 
the project. Speciahsts in psychoanalysis, in mental measurements, in 
gynecology, and in various other fields, are the ones best equipped to 
undertake intensive studies; but when we have made such suggestions, they 



58 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


each subject’s attitudes toward his parents, toward masturbation, pre- 
marital intercourse, sexual relations with prostitutes, and homosexual 
experience; but we do not have much confidence in verbalizations of 
attitudes which each subject thinks are his own, when they are, in actuality, 
little more than reflections of the attitudes which prevail in the particular 
culture in which he was raised. Often the expressed attitudes are in striking 
contradiction to the actual behavior, and then they are significant because 
they indicate the existence of psychic conflict and they throw light on the 
extent to which community attitudes may influence an individual. 

23. Interviewing young children. For children who are twelve or older, 
it is usually feasible to adapt the regular interview to their vocabulary and 
experience, securing quite satisfactory answers. For younger children, 
especially for those under eight years of age, it is necessary to use a totally 
different approach. An interview then becomes a social session involving 
participation in the child’s ordinary activities. One of the parents has been 
present in all of our interviews with these younger children. The technique 
is one in which the interviewer looks at dolls, at toys of other sorts, joins 
in games, builds picture puzzles, romps and does acrobatics with the 
vigorous small boy, tells stories, reads stories, gets the child to tell stories, 
draws pictures, gets the child to draw pictures, shares candies and cookies, 
and withal makes himself an agreeable guest. Tucked into these activities 
are questions that give information on the child’s sexual background. If 
the picture book shows kittens putting on nightgowns for bed, the child 
may be asked whether she wears nightgowns when she goes to bed. When 
the interviewer tussles with the four-year old boy, he may ask him whether 
he similarly tussles with the other boys in the neighborhood, and rapidly 
follows up with questions concerning tussling with the girls, whether he 
plays with any girls, whether he likes girls, whether he kisses girls. 

There is no sequence of questions and one depends upon opportunities 
that the play activities create for leading off into particular questions. The 
child’s drawings are highly significant, as psychologists will understand. 
Many a small child who cannot possibly describe the anatomical differences 
between males and females will draw pictures of boys and girls which make 
the distinction. An interview with a young child becomes an information 
test rather than an examination of the child’s overt activity; and the reac- 
tions of the child to the questions are more important than the specific 
information which he supplies. The above-mentioned four-year old boy 
may talk freely and spontaneously about the other boys that he plays with. 
He may or may not so freely admit that there are girls in the neighborhood 
with whom he also plays, and his embarrassment, his hesitancy, his dis- 
turbed giggling, or his calm acceptance of the fact are the most important 
things for the student of sexual behavior to note. Many of the adult 
attitudes toward various items of sex are already discernible in the three- 



INTERVIEWING 


59 


or four-year old’s history, and often the differences in attitudes of different 
social levels are already reflected in the reactions of these young children. 
A later volume will cover this aspect of the study. 

THE INTERVIEWER’S BACKGROUND OF KNOWLEDGE 

In general, it is difficult to explore effectively unless one has some under- 
standing of the sort of thing that he is likely to find. One cannot intelli- 
gently push questions on sexual behavior if he does not comprehend what 
the possibilities of behavior may be. Inevitably these possibilities are 
beyond the personal experience that any interviewer is likely to have had, 
and consequently the prospective student of sexual behavior needs to learn 
a great deal from the very considerable literature on sex, and a great deal 
more from the experiences of the persons from whom he takes histories. 

Specifically, one needs to comprehend the whole range of possible 
techniques in each possible type of sexual behavior, including masturba- 
tion, petting, intercourse, homosexual activities, animal contacts, relations 
with prostitutes, etc. ; and one needs to understand the variety of psychic 
problems that may be involved and the considerable social complications 
that may develop in connection with each type of activity. There are 
hundreds of possible positions in intercourse, and although the original 
interview may be confined to questions concerning the six major possibili- 
ties, the interviewer should be prepared to investigate the full diversity of 
positions which an experimentally-inclined subject may use. There are 
scores of variant techniques in the homosexual which should be investi- 
gated when the opportunity comes to get such information. There are 
hundreds of things that need to be known about prostitution before one is 
ready to secure an adequate history from a prostitute. Lacking a knowledge 
of the possibilities, the interviewer will get only the most routine record 
from a person who in actuality could give a wealth of information. 

Many of these variant and relatively rare situations provide most signifi- 
cant data on the backgrounds of human sexual behavior. In many instances 
variant types of behavior represent the basic mammalian patterns which 
have been so effectively suppressed by human culture that they persist and 
reappear only among those few individuals who ignore custom and deliber- 
ately follow their preferences in sexual techniques. In some instances sexual 
behavior which is outside the socially accepted pattern is the more natural 
behavior (Chapter 6) because it is less affected by social restraints. The 
clearest picture of learning in sexual behavior is to be found in the homo- 
sexual; and if the homosexual had been ignored in the present study, we 
should not have realized that similar learning processes are involved in the 
development of the heterosexual. Histories of extra-marital intercourse 
and the whole story of prostitution provide the best possible data on 
factors affecting orgasm in the female, and they are replete with striking 



60 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


instances of conditioning for particular situations. The interviewer who is 
satisfied with covering the routine, and who is not prepared to explore into 
the abundance of divergent fields, loses some of the scientifically richest 
material that can be obtained in a case history study. 

The background of knowledge which the interviewer has is of great 
importance in establishing rapport with his subjects. The importance of 
this cannot be overemphasized A subject is inevitably hesitant to discuss 
things which seem to be both outside of the experience of the interviewer, 
and beyond his knowledge. The narcissistic and masochistic male wdo has 
greatly elaborated his masturbatory techniques realizes that he is so unu- 
sual that he does not intend to admit anything beyond the simple fact of 
masturbation, when he first comes into the interview; but the experienced 
interviewer will discern that this is a special case when he asks the routine 
questions concerning the subject’s interest in looking at his own genitalia 
during masturbation, his interest in observing himself in a mirror, his 
custom of nibbling or biting a partner during a sexual relation, his reac- 
tions to being similarly bitten, his reactions to stories of torture, and his 
reactions to being hurt himself. Although these questions do not come in a 
block, but are spread throughout the length of the interview, the investi- 
gator should be capable of putting the answers together and understanding 
what they mean. Ninety per cent of all masturbation in the male is by a 
single technique, but for this case that is now before him, the intcrview'cr 
should know enough about other possiblp techniques to be able to con- 
vince the subject that he is talking with a person who understands. The 
subject must feel that the situation is not so new or so strange that the 
interviewer will be startled at these things. He must be convinced that the 
interviewer can take them as he did the routine material. The interviewer’s 
background of knowledge is the key to obtaining a wealth of special 
material that a routine schedule of questions may completely miss. 

It is particularly important that the interviewer understand socially 
taboo and illegal sexual activities, because these are the most dilficult 
items on which to secure honest records. He needs to understand the sexual 
viewpoint of the culture to which each of his subjects belongs. For instance, 
it is impossible to get any number of histories from prostitutes, female or 
male, unless they realize that the interviewer understands both the sexual 
situations involved in prostitution, and the social organization of a prosti- 
tute s life. A single phrase from an understanding interviewer is often 
sufficient to make the subject understand this, and such an interviewer wins 
a record where none would have been disclosed to the uneducated investi- 
gator. A specific illustration will make this more apparent. 

This is the case of the older Negro male whose first answers were wary 
and evasive. When questioned concerning his occupation, he listed a variety 
of minor jobs which, taken in connection with his manner of response, 



INTERVIEWING 


61 


seemed to spell underworld activities. We followed up our clue by im- 
mediately asking the subject whether he had ever been married. We were 
not satisfied with his denial of marriage, and followed with a question as 
to whether he had ever lived common law. The easy use of a vernacular 
term made him feel freer to talk, and when he admitted that he had so 
lived, we asked how old he was when he first lived common law. When he 
said that he was then fourteen, our first suspicion concerning his under- 
world activity was confirmed, and we immediately followed up by asking 
how old the woman was. At this, he smiled and admitted that she was 
thirty-five. Then we remarked, easily and without surprise: “She was a 
hustler, wasn’t she?” This was the final step necessary for winning com- 
plete confidence. The subject stopped short in his reply, opened his eyes 
wide, smiled in a friendly fashion, and said, “Well, sir, since you appear to 
know something about these things, I’ll tell you straight.” The extraordi- 
nary record that we then got of his history as a pimp could not have been 
obtained if the subject had not comprehended that we understood the 
world in which he lived. 

Very often the interviewer’s capacity to secure an accurate history 
depends upon his knowledge of the correlations that usually exist between 
certain items, and his readiness to demand an explanation of any incon- 
sistency that appears in a particular history. To illustrate again: one starts 
by asking the girl how old she was when she turned her first trick (but one 
does not ask how old she was vdien she was first paid as a prostitute). She 
is then asked how many of the tricks return after their first contacts with 
her. Considerably later in the interview there is a question concerning the 
frequency with which she rolls her tricks (robs her customers). The girl 
who reports that few of the men ever return, and who subsequently says 
that she never robs any of the men, needs to be caught up abruptly and 
assured that you know that it doesn’t work that way. If she doesn’t roll 
any of the men, why don’t they return to her? This question is likely to 
bring a smile from the girl and an admission that since you appear to know 
how these things work, she will tell you the whole story, which means that 
she robs every time there is any possibility of successfully doing so. 

The development of an interviewer is a long and slow process. In the 
present study, for instance, it has involved a full year of training for each 
interviewer before he was ready to go very far in taking histories. The code 
had to be learned, and experience in its use was acquired by reading and 
by re-recording histories that were already in the file. Further experience 
was obtained by observing other interviewers in action and recording 
simultaneously while they did the questioning. Conversely, the trainee was 
given an opportunity to interview in the presence of the more experienced 
members of the staff. Then there was an opportunity to re-take histories 
which others had previously taken, and the trainee’s own subjects were 



62 


SEXUAL BERAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


given re-takes by a more experienced interviewer. When the new inter- 
viewer is able to secure a record that is practically a perfect duplicate of 
that obtained by the experienced person, he is in a position to take his- 
tories that can be added to those obtained by the older inter\iewers. 
Finally, each new interviewer has had to acquire a considerable kno\\ ledge 
of what people can do sexually and demonstrate that he can draw on that 
knowledge in the case of an unusual sort of history. After such a training 
program, one may, or may not, be ready to face the variety of situations 
that an investigator of human sexual behavior may meet. 

The effectiveness of any interviewing technique is, in the last analysis, 
to be determined by the quality of the data that are obtained. Wc have 
described the techniques by means of which we have secured the data 
which are presented in the remainder of this volume. Whether the tech- 
niques which have been used in the present study would be equally effective 
with other persons engaged in studying other problems, is a question v’hich 
must be answered empirically by each investigator in connection with his 
own special problems. 



Chapter 3 


STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


Students who are interested in population analyses will want to examine 
the technical procedures on which the present study has been based. 
Because of the scope of the project, it has been necessary to work out some 
original techniques in recording the material, in testing the validity of the 
record, and in analyzing the data statistically. These matters will be of less 
interest to those who are primarily concerned with the actual behavior of 
the human male, and such readers may prefer to pass over this and the 
next chapter and turn directly to the consideration of the sexual data which 
begins with Chapter 5. 

NATURE OF THE DATA 


It has already been explained (Chapter 1) that the data in the present 
study have all been gathered through personal interviews. In each history, 
521 items have been explored; but since a subject is questioned only about 
those things in which he has had specific experience, the actual number of 
items covered in each case is usually nearer 300, and the number involved 
in the histories of younger and less experienced individuals is often less 
than that. The maximum list is shown in the following table. A few of the 
items (those marked with asterisks) call for information which is procurable 
only through physical examination or other special tests, and such items 
are being investigated only on certain individuals who are available for 
special study. 

Items Covered on Sex Histories 


I. Social and Economic Data 

1. Sex 

2. Age 

3. Date of birth 

4. Race 

5. Geographic origin 

State of subject's birth 
Countries, states of residence 
for a year or more 
Parents’ place of birth 

6. Rural-urban background 

7. Religious background 

Denominations involved 
Degree of adherence 

8. Occupational history 

9. Economic status 


10. Educational history 
Years of schooling 
Colleges attended 
College majors 
Age upon leaving school 
Age while in high school 
*11. Psychological test ratings 
12. Recreational interests 

Extracurricular activities in 
school and college 
Moving pictures 
Dancing 
Cards 
Gambling 
Smoking 
Use of alcohol 


* Items marked with asterisks (*) are checked only for selected series of individuals 
who are available for special study. 


63 



64 

I. Social and Economic Data {Confd) 

12. Recreational interests {Confd) 

Use of narcotics 

Use of marihuana 

Hunting 

Fishing 

Reading 

Sewing 

Cooking 

Housework 

Special interest in music 
Special interest in sports 
Other special interests and 
sources of recreation 

13. Athletic experience 

On high school and college or- 
ganized teams 

14. Fraternity or sorority member- 

ship in college 

15. Home background 

Parents’ occupational status 
Parents’ economic status 
Parents’ educational back- 
ground 

Parental marital history 
Plappiness of adjustment 
Separation or divorce 
Parent-child relationships 
Attachment to father 
Attachment to mother 
Brothers, sisters 
Number 
Ages 

Companions at 10 and at 16 
Number 

Relative numbers, male and 
female 

16. Institutional history 

Prison, orphanage, etc. 

Army or Navy experience 

17. Personality traits 

II. Marital Histories (For Each Mar- 

riage) 

1. Marital status 

2. Spouse’s history 

Age 

Length of previous acquaint- 
ance 

Length of engagement 
Religious affiliations 
Educational history 

3. Age of each spouse at marriage 

4. Years married, divorced, sepa- 

rated, or widowed 

5. Common law marriages 

6. Offspring 

Sex 

Ages 

Mother’s age at first childbirth 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 

7. Abortions 

Spontaneous 

Induced 

8. Marital adjustment 

Rating 

Sources of conflict 

III. Sex Education 

1. Sources of knowledge, ages when 

learned 
Pregnancy 
Coitus 
Fertilization 
Menstruation 
Venereal diseases 
Prostitution 
Contraception 
Abortions 

Male erection (in female his- 
tories) 

2. Parental contribution to sex edu- 

cation 

3. Experience in observing sex be- 

havior 

4. Experience with graphic depic- 

tions of sexual activity 

5. Formal sex education in school 

and college 

6. Attitudes on nudity 

Of parents 
Of subject 

IV. Physical and Piiysioiogic Data 

^ 1. General development and health 

Height 

Weight, and maximum ever 
reached 
*Pulse rate 

*Blood pressure and BMR 
Thickness of lips 
Handedness 

History of chronic illnesses and 
handicaps 

History of venereal disease 

2. Adolescence: ages at onset of 

Erotic responsiveness 
First orgasm (and its source) 
Pubic hair growth 
Breast development (in females) 
Breast knots (in adolescent 
males) 

Menstruation 
Voice change 
Onset of rapid growth 
Completion of growth 

3. Genital characters: male 

Testes 

Descent 

Position (of right and left) 
*Size 

History of injury 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


65 


IV. Physical and Physiologic Data 
(Confd) 

3. Geaital characters : male (Confd) 
Penis (subject’s self measure- 
ment) 

Length and circumference, 
normal and erect 
Angle, erect 
Curvature, erect 
Direction of carriage, erect 
History of circumcision 
Age involved 
Presence of frenulum 
Extent of foreskin 
Phimosis 
Hypospadia 

Pre-coital mucous secretion 
* Sperm examination 
Erection 
Speed 

Presence of pulsation 
Potency in coitus 
Duration 

Mormng erections, frequency 

4. Genital characters: female 

Clitoris 

*Size 

^Adherence of foreskin 
Hymen 
Status 

History of rupture 
Vaginal mucous secretions * 
Amount 

Variation in menstrual cycle 

5. Menstruation 

Age at menarche 
Length and regularity of men- 
strual cycle 
Duration of flow 
Pains 

History of menopause 

6. Erotic responsiveness 

Auto-erotic 

Observing self in mirror 
Observing genitalia 
Exhibitionism 
Homo-erotic 
Thinking of own sex 
Observing own sex 
Observing erect genitalia 
Observing buttocks 
Burlesque shows 
Nude art 
Obscene stories 
Erotic literature 
Erotic moving pictures 
Erotic photographs and 
drawings 
Dancing 


Hetero-erotic 
Thinking of other sex 
Observing other sex 
Nude art 
Burlesque shows 
Erotic pictures 
Obscene stories 
Erotic literature 
Moving pictures 
Dancing 

Physical contacts 
Biting 
Being bitten 
Zoo-erotic 

Observing animal coitus 
Physical contacts with ani- 
mals 

Non-sexual stimuli 
Music 
Alcohol 
Motion 
Pain 

Sadistic situations 
Masochistic situations 
Other emotional situation 
(especially in children) 

V. Nocturnal Sex Dreams 

1. Ages involved 

2. Frequencies of dreams with or- 

gasm 

3. Frequencies of dreams without 

orgasm 

4. Content of dreams 

Homo-, hetero-, or zod-erotic 
Other 

VI. Masturbation 

1. Ages involved, pre- and post- 

adolescent 

2. Sources of learning 

Conversation and reading 
Observation 

Participation, heterosexual or 
homosexual 
Self discovery 

3. Frequencies 

Maximum per week 
Means at each age 

4. Techniques 

For male 
Manual 
Frictional 
Oral 

Special devices 
Urethral insertions 
For female 
Breast 
Clitoral 

Vaginal insertion 
Frictional 



SEXUAL BEELWIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


66 


VI. Masturbation {Confd) 

4. Techniques {Confd) 

Thigh pressure 
Ure'thral insertions 
With devices 

5. Time required for orgasm 

6. Accompanying imagery 

Self 

Homosexual 

Heterosexual 

Zoo-erotic 

Sado-masochistic 

7. Subject’s evaluation 

Period involving fear or con- 
flict 

Sources of resolution of conflict 
Rejection; period involved, 
reasons for 

Estimate of moral, psychic, 
physical consequences 

VII. Heterosexual History 

1. Pre-adolcscent play 

Ages involved, frequencies 
Companions: ages and number 
Techniques 
Exhibition 
Physical exploration 
Vaginal insertion 
Urethral insertion 
Mouth-genital contact 
Coitus 

2. Pre-marital petting 

Ages involved 

Frequencies 

Companions 

Number in grade and high 
school 

Number between high school 
and marriage 
Techm’ques 
General body contact 
Lip kissing 
Tongue kissing 
Breast manipulation, manual 
Breast manipulation, oral 
Manual manipulation, male 
genitalia 

Manual manipulation, fe- 
male genitalia 

Mouth-genital contact on 
male, on female 
Genital apposition without 
entry 

Orgasm without intercourse, 
in male, in female 
Frequencies 
Ages involved 
After-effects 
Nervous disturbance 


Genital cramps 
Masturbation 

3. Attitudes on pre-marital coitus 

Souices of restraint 
Moral 

Lack of opportunity 
Lack of interest 
Fear of pregnancy 
Fear of venereai disease 
Fear of social disapproval 
Desire for \irginitv in hancee 
Desire for marriage 
Desire for children, number 
desired 

Intention to have, or to con- 
tinue coitus 

Evaluation of ovvm coital ex- 
perience 

4. Experience in pre-marital coitus 

Ages involved 
First experience 
Age and nature of partner 
Virginity of partner 
Speed of orgasm 
Physical satisfaction 
Frequencies in coitus 
Partners 
Total number 

Prostitutes or companions 
Age range 

Youngest since subject was 
eighteen 
Age preference 
Marital status 
Consanguinity 
Virginity 

Resulting pregnancies, births, 
abortions 
Ages involved 
Legal aspects 
Financial aspects 
Arrangements 
Places utilized for coitus 
Opportunity and desire for 
nudity 

5. Marital intercourse (separate rec- 

ords for each marriage) 

First experience 
Age of each spouse 
Virginity of partner 
Speed of orgasm 
Physical satisfaction 
Lapse between marriage and 
first coitus 
Frequencies 
Maximum ever 
Means at various periods 
Relation of sexual and marital 
adjustments 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


67 


VIL Heterosexual History (Confd) 

6. Extra-marital relations 

Ages involved 
Partners 
Number 
Age range 
Marital status 
Companions or prostitutes 
Frequencies 
Extra-marital coitus 
Extra-marital petting with- 
out intercourse 

Spouse’s knowledge of the 
intercourse 
Effect on marriage 
Desire for further experience 

7. Post-marital intercourse 
- Ages involved 

Partners 
Number 
Age range 
Marital status 
Companions or prostitutes 
Frequencies 

8. Intercourse with prostitutes 

Ages involved 
Number of prostitutes 
Frequencies 

Mouth-genital techniques 
Comparisons with non-prosti- 
tutes 

9. Coital techniques * 

Pre-coital play 
Duration 
Lip kissing 
Tongue kissing 
Breast manipulation: man- 
ual 

Breast manipulation : oral 
Genital manipulation: man- 
ual, by male and female 
Genital manipulation: oral, 
by male and female 
Frequency of orgasm 
Coital positions: relative fre- 
quencies and preferences 
Male superior 
Female superior 
Side 
Sitting 
Standing 
Rear entry 
Anal 

Other variations 
Male orgasm 
Duration of intromission 
Multiple climaces 
Female orgasm 
Frequency 


Multiple climaces 
Date of first orgasm in coitus 
Relation to coital techniques 
Nudity 
Frequency 
Attitudes 

Preference for light or dark 
Fantasies during coitus 

10. Contraceptive history: pre-mari- 

tal, marital, extra-marital ; 
techniques employed, satis- 
faction and effectiveness 
Condom 
Source 
Testing 
Lubrication 
Breakage 
Diaphragm or cap 
Source 
Type 

Withdrawal 
Douche alone 
Materials employed 
Safe period 
Jelly alone 
Other techniques 

11. Group heterosexual activities 

Circumstances, frequency 
Number and nature of partners 
Participation in strip poker 
Fraternal and other group ini- 
tiation activities 
Observation of coitus 
Of parents 
Of friends 

Of professional exhibition- 
ists 

12. Heterosexual prostitution (the 

subject as prostitute; males 
or females as heterosexual 
prostitutes) 

Ages involved 
First experience 
Occasion 
'Partner 
Pay involved 
Frequencies per week 
During first year 
During subsequent periods 
Maximum number of partners 
per day, per week 
Average number of partners 
per day and per week, at 
various periods 
Nature of partners 
Total number 
Age: range, average 
Number who return 
Longest affair 



SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


68 

VIL Heterosexual History (Confd) 

12. Heterosexual prostitution (the 
subject as prostitute; males 
or females as heterosexual 
prostitutes) {Confd) 

Love affairs 
Percent married 
Number of virgins 
Occupations 
Racial groups 
Sources of contacts 
Personal introductions by 
friends 

In established houses of 
prostitution 
Ages of subject 
Number of houses involved 
Size of houses 
Geographic location 
Physical or other force 
used to hold prostitutes 
In own established house 
Time involved 
Geographic location 
Number of persons em- 
ployed 

Street solicitation 
Percent of rejects 
Percent causing trouble 
Petting techniques 
Partners active in petting (in 
9 techniques shown in 
VII-2) 

Subject active (in same 9 
techniques) 

Coital positions (as shown in 
¥11-9) 

Orgasm, frequencies for sub- 
ject 

Prophylaxis and contraception 
Examination of partners 
Condoms 
Antiseptic douche 
Other prophylactics 
Variant techniques 
Anal coitus 
Anilinctus 
Flagellation 
Other sadism 
Masochism 
Scatology 
Voyeurism 
Fetishism 
Group activities 
Age of subject 
* Frequency 

Number of persons in group 
involved 

Psychologic reactions 


Exhibitionistic activity 
Ages involved 
Frequency 
Sizes of audiences 
Character of audiences 
Psychologic reaction of sub- 
ject 

On first occasion 
On subsequent occasions 
Pay: range, average 
Techniques of the exhibition 
Income from prostitution 
Range of payment 
Average payment 
Average earned per week, 
per month 

Percent paid to houses 
Percent paid to procurer 
Rolling 
Frequency 
Range of intake 
Average intake 
Social involvements 
With friends 
With families 
With police 

Frequency of arrest and 
conviction 
For prostitution 
For rolling 

For other concomitants 
* Legal penalties 

Bases of dismissal 
Amounts paid for protec- 
tion 

Socio-sexual background 
Factors involved in begin- 
ning prostitution 
Chief factors for continu- 
ing prostitution 
Extent of pleasure derived 
from sexual relation 
Plan for continuance of pros- 
titution 

Psychologic conflicts of pros- 
titute 

Effects on marriage 
Willingness to recommend 
prostitution to others 
VIII. Homosexual History 
1. Pre-adolescent play 

Ages involved, frequencies 
Companions 
Ages 
Number 
Techniques 
Exhibition 

Manual manipulation 
Vaginal or urethral insertion 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


69 


VIII. Homosexual History {Confd) 

1. Pre-adolescent play (Co/irW) 

Mouth-genital contact 
Anal 

2. Post-adolescent experience 

Ages involved 
First experience 
Age 
Partner 
Age 
Race 

Relation to subject 
Circumstances 
Place of contact 
Initiation of approach 
Techniques employed, 
passive, active, or mu- 
tual 

Financial arrangements 
Satisfaction for subject 
Age of first experience with 
each technique, passive 
and active 
Manual 
Oral 
Anal 

Breast (for female) 

Femoral 

Full body contact 
Frequency 
During first year 
Maximum, ever, per day ^ 
Maximum, ever, per week 
Average per week during 
each year 

Total number of contacts 
Partners 
Total number 
Age range 

Comparisons with age of 
subject 

Age preferences 
Reasons for age preferences 
Social position 
Students in grade school 
Students in high school 
Students in college 
Clergy 
Teachers 
Art groups 
Professional persons 
Business groups 
Armed forces 
Laboring groups 
Law enforcement officers 
Highest position held 
Number married 
Number without previous 
homosexual experience 


Number of oncers 
Duration of longest affairs 
Relations involving love and 
affection 

Percentage of approaches 
which are rejected 
Races involved: white, Ne- 
gro, others 

Techniques 

Petting, passive and active 
Lip kissing 
Tongue kissing 
Body kissing 

Breast manipulation, ma- 
ual 

Breast manipulation, oral 
Genital mampulation, 
manual 

Genital manipulation, oral 
Flagellation on back, but- 
tocks, genitalia 
Urethral insertions 
Anilinctus 
Nudity 

Positions involved (includ- 
ing 69) 

Preference for light or dark 
Places involved 

Subject’s orgasm 
Frequency by each technique 
or by spontaneous ejac- 
ulation 

Partner’s orgasm 
Frequency by each technique 
or by spontaneous ejac- 
ulation 

3. Psychic reactions 

Preferences for 
Masculine or feminine type 
of partner 

Partner of particular height 
Partner of particular weight 
Partner of particular com- 
plexion 

Particular amount of body 
hair 

Particular genital charac- 
teristics 

Particular breast character- 
istics 

Circumcised partner 
Other physical qualities of 
partner 

Reaction to odor and taste, 
genitalia and semen 

4. Sources of contacts 

Personal friends 

Pick-ups 

Street 



SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


70 

VIII. Homosexual History {Confd) 

4. Sources of contacts {Confd) 

Park 
Hotel 
Theatre 
Tavern 
Night club , 

Restaurant 

Beach 

Transportation terminal 
Public bath 
Hitch hiking 
Other places 

5. Social conflicts 

Difficulties met in home, school, 
community, business 

Arrests, court action, penal 
history 

Blackmail, active and passive 

Robbery, active and passive 

Restriction to homosexual asso- 
ciates 

6. Homosexual prostitution 

Subject as prostitute 
Frequency 
Situations 
Amounts involved 
Long-time maintenance as 
prostitute 

Subject paying prostitutes 
Frequency 
Situations 
Amounts involved 
Long-time maintenance of 
prostitutes 


7. Subject’s self analysis 

Recognition of physical stig- 
mata 

Carnage and mo\ements 
Voice 

Hip mo\ements 

Walk 

Dress 

Make-up 

Interest in trans\estism 
Other qualities 
Conflicts and regrets 
Expectancy for continuation 
Expectancy for transfer to 
heterosexual 

Recommendation of the homo- 
sexual for others 
Subject’s analysis of factors 
involved 

Subject's estimate of extent of 
homosexuality 
Among males, females 
Among Negroes, whites 
IX. Animal Contaci's 

1. Ages involved 

2. Frequencies 

With orgasm 
Without orgasm 

3. Animal species involved, with 

pieferences 

4. Techniques 

Masturbation of animal 
Vaginal coitus 
Mouth-genital contact 
Passive 
Active 


Each item in the above list has been strictly defined in order to standard- 
ize the data used in the study; but the body of definitions is, unfortunately, 
too large to include in the present volume. As previously indicated (Chap- 
ter 2), the sequence of topics actually used in an interview is varied in 
accordance with the age, social background, and experience of the partic- 
ular subject, and the sequence shown in the above list has never been used. 
Neither does the sequence in the list correspond with the one shown in the 
coded form of Figure 2. Although many of the items in the list are covered 
by single questions, and although an occasional question may elicit infor- 
mation on two or more points, it often takes more extended inquiry to 
secure particular answers. Consequently, the number of questions asked 
may considerably exceed the number of items which are covered. It has 
already been noted (Chapter 2) that additional questions may be asked of 
subjects who have been involved in activities which are not covered in the 
routine interviews. Persons with experience in particular situations, as in 
the armed forces, in prisons, CCC camps, and other institutions, are 
questioned in particular detail concerning those periods. 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


71 


CODING 

The data obtained in each interview are recorded directly in the sort of 
code which is shown in Figure 2. No record is kept in any other form, and 
the coded data have never been translated into any longhand or typewritten 
account. Coding at the time of the interview serves several functions: (1) 
It facilitates recording, making it possible to secure a complete history 
without slowing up an interview, and without losing rapport with the 
subject. (2) It preserves the confidence of the record, and this is particularly 
important in a sex study. (3) It facilitates the transference of the data from 
the original record sheet to punched cards for statistical analyses. (4) It 
increases the accuracy of the coding because the subject is present at the 
time of the operation. Where there is uncertainty about the classification 
of the data, additional questions may be asked and final determinations 
may be made on the spot. When coding is delayed until after a longhand 
record is carried back to the laboratory, the investigator too often finds 
that there are insufficient data to determine what classifications are 
involved. (5) Coding is of supreme importance in conserving space, making 
it possible to put the whole of the basic history on a single sheet, or on two 
sheets in the case of individuals who have especial experience in pre- 
marital intercourse, in extra-marital intercourse, in the homosexual, or in 
prostitution. Where the original record is made in longhand which is 
subsequently copied onto typewritten sheets, or even where the basic 
record is made in a standard or special system of shorthand, each history 
may extend over twenty, or thirt;^, or in some studies over a hundred or 
more pages. Coding and punching cards from such data are slow and some- 
times well-nigh impossible procedures. Moreover, it is always difficult for 
the investigator to comprehend the whole of such a history when it is 
spread over so many pages. If the record is confined to a single sheet, it is 
possible to correlate any item with any and every other item by a rapid 
sweep of one’s eye over the page of simple and precisely placed 
symbols. (6) It facilitates and encourages the systematic coverage of the 
same items on each and every history. At the end of an interview, a rapid 
examination of the page will show what items have been missed, and the 
blank places can be filled before the subject has departed. 

The specific code used in the present study cannot be explained because 
of the necessity for maintaining the confidence of the record. However, 
certain of the principles involved can be described for the benefit of those 
who are interested in developing coding devices in other studies : 

1. The record is made on a ruled form which provides a number of 
blocks somewhat in excess of the number of items on the basic history 
(Figure 2). The form used in the present study is a standard Keuffel and 
Esser product (General Data Sheet No. 358-230), with over-printing done 
on our especial order. 



72 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


2. Each aspect of the sex history is recorded in a particular block or 
portion of a particular block. The significance of any symbol depends, 
consequently, upon its position in a particular block on the page. 

3. Each block has its own system of symbols, its own independent code. 


o 

o 


o 


o 


4ia 

N\ 

lU- S 


IX 

n 

6 

5^4 3 

w 

X - 3/^ 

V 

5 

1c> ctw.fc> X 


f V5\ 

o <>,»>< X.C 

*tl 9©)3 v^ ' 

X 


7 + 




X 




y 




X 

V 

.L- . _ . 



s/ *t-4 






— 

! 


X 








✓ 

X X” 







-^4 

--- ”-j 

y 

X 



1*1 

< 





o 





di 

'f -/po 


a 

o 





X 


'x 



T- IT- 

Vc. 

1 

A.-clA 

— 


X 


__ — 

— 



jC-- TvV 

X \ \ d'ac 

X tr X 



X 1 



VC 

T 

K 


jS^rr 




< 


— 

X 



, 

X 

v/ 

X 


3 ^ 


3/ f V 

X» H 

'7 - 



o 


M 



X 

X 



2_ 


5-W 

Mr! 

v/»/ 

- 

I 

» 






✓ 


X ' " 1 

X 

X “7 





— 

X 

X i 

X 

X v" 

X 

1 



O 

y~ 

v/— * 

✓ 

(O 

M 

X 



Vv/ y 


I 




X 

2x 

a 

X 

X 

V. ' 

o 

C -d 


t/ 




✓ " - 


X 

TP 

(O 

v' 


4 




(73-. PCO 




1 



V 

Xn/ 

P J 





— 

y- - ( 

dL 

•i. TA 

X 

1 


— 

y 

— 


R 

v/ 

(7-ViS->d 





^ 1 



X 

v/ 

✓ 

•7 ! 


X 

1 f- sT” 1 


\ ! 

— y 

a-s 1 

✓ 



1- 

v' iw -d ; 




X 

y 

— 



r " 

1 

y 


X 


X 


>/ - ! 

' 

— 



9 

— 

0)1 i 

1 


1 



X ‘ 

X 


a. 

X 


x\ 





X 

~^7 




[ 

’ 


X 


-la =7/ 




"1 

-h3 

11 N\ ^ 

-1? = t-V 

X 



n 


A r 

1 L.^T 




X 1 1 

4..y- 

X 


X 

I 


X i 1 

fA 

o 


X 



X :ir“T 1 




2< X 

ZxT 



X y/ \ 1 

X X V 

SAJ 

X V" 

c.\jif Hvj ^ 

— — 5, 

] 



■21 C H5 1 j 


CL f >; V*- ^;vr MJr 

•. IS- X X. 

6 15. Ini 


\vs5 


o 


Figure 2. Sample history in code 

4. In each block, the available symbols are sufficient in number to 
designate all of the categories into which the particular data will be classi- 
fied during subsequent analyses. It is necessary to anticipate the whole 
array of possible classifications, including those that lie beyond usual 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


73 


experience. The code used in the present study is so flexible that it has been 
possible to handle every type of overt activity and every sort of attitudinal 
situation which we have attempted to record in the 12,000 histories now at 
hand. 

5. Since this study has been primarily concerned with percentages of 
incidence, with frequency distributions, graded scales of attitudes, intensi- 
ties of response, and other questions of degree, the code provides for a 
considerable series of classifications of each item. Rarely is it a matter of 
alternative possibilities — of a yes or a no. It is the usual statistical experi- 
ence that six to a dozen or twenty categories, and occasionally a few more, 
provide the number of points best designed to establish a curve; and in 
most instances there should be that many possibilities in the code for each 
item, in each block. 

6. Where the nature of the code allows, as in using numbers to desig- 
nate the ages or years involved, there is no objection to recording more 
detail than is used in the subsequent analyses. It not infrequently happens 
that such detail proves useful in making finer calculations than were 
originally planned. 

7. The symbols used in coding data in the present study have included 
various mathematical signs (±, — , X, V? 0, V V) and numbers for 
recording ages, the years involved, frequencies, and still other items. In 
addition, we have used numbers, letters, symbols derived from standard 
practice in biology, chemistry, physics, and the other sciences, and some 
unique symbols developed especially for this study. 

8. There is no written key to the code that has been used in the present 
study, and the strict maintenance of that rule has been necessary for pre- 
serving the confidence of the record. 

9. In consequence, each interviewer has had to memorize the code and 
learn, through considerable drilling, the significance of each block, and 
the symbols pertaining thereto. 

10. In coding items that are not discrete in nature, it is necessary to 
make precise definitions to which each coded symbol should apply. The 
coding done by different interviewers on a project *must constantly be 
checked to provide strict standardization. This is especially important in 
coding attitudes, intensities of response, and other non-discrete materials. 
For this reason in the present study a minimum of such items has been 
employed, and for these items the judgments made by the several inter- 
viewers have been repeatedly analyzed and coordinated. 

SUPPLEMENTARY DATA 

While most of the material in the present volume is based upon the data 
which have been routinely secured in the interviews, considerable attention 
has been given to securing supplementary information by other techmques. 



74 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


These additional data have come from a considerable list of subjects with 
whom lona-time social contacts have been maintained, in some cases for 

o 

as long as seven and eight years. Time has been spent in their homes, and 
visits have been made with them to the homes of their friends, to theatres 
and to concerts, to night-clubs and to taverns, and to their oilier places ot 
recreation. During these contacts, there have been abundant opportunities 
to observe how these individuals react to a variety of social, professional, 
academic, and other situations. With many of them there has been a con- 
siderable correspondence which now supplements their original histories 
with extensive day by day records of their activities, and of their thinking 
on various aspects of sex. In a number of cases there are se\ cral hundred 
pages, and in each of two cases there are over a thousand pages of such 
supplementary material. These latter constitute more extensive sexual his- 
tories than any which have yet been published on particular individuals. 
For some subjects there are photographic collections of their imaginative 
drawings, and scrapbooks; there are photographic collections of the com- 
plete artistic output of some of the artists who have contributed to the 
study, and complete collections of the books written by certain other 
subjects. For others who have had contact with public agencies, we ha\e 
transcripts of the court records, institutional records, data from social 
agencies, and other material. While these supplementary records have con- 
tributed little to the statistical tabulations of data, they have provided a 
considerable portion of the detail which is given in this volume on the 
physical nature of sexual arousal and 6f orgasm, and on the psychologic 
and social concomitants of sexual behavior, particularly in relation to the 
factors which motivate and control the activities. 

A number of persons have turned in sexual calendars and diaries 
showing their day to day activities over some period of time. The calendars 
now at hand cover periods which range from six months to more than 
thirty-five years in length. They admirably supplement the information 
routinely obtained on the standard histories. They provide data on the 
weekly periodicity which a seven-day calendar and the consequent social 
organization impose upon many human activities, as Havelock Ellis 
pointed out for a. group of diaries which he studied (Ellis 1897, 1936 
Edit.) ; and they clearly demonstrate the monthly periodicity of sexual re- 
sponsiveness in the female, and the lack of any such periodicity in the male. 
As soon as there are enough of these calendars, it will be possible to run 
correlations between the precise records they supply and the estimated 
frequencies of activities obtained in the regular interviews; and it is un- 
fortunate that there are not enough of the calendars yet available to make 
the analyses in the present volume. Persons who have kept records or who 
are willing to begin keeping day by day calendars showing the frequencies 
and the sources of their sexual outlet, are urged to place the accumulated 
data at our disposal. 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


75 


Throughout this study, especial attention has been given to the com- 
munities in which the subjects of this study have hved. Contacts have been 
maintained with some of the communities for months or even several 
years. In that way it has been possible to win confidences from persons 
who hesitated to contribute their histories when we first arrived. In time 
one becomes accepted in the homes of a community and becomes ac- 
quainted with the daily lives of its individual members. One becomes 
acquainted with the general attitudes of the community on matters of sex 
and learns something about the community backgrounds which determine 
the early development of individual patterns of sexual behavior, and their 
fixation in the adult histories. One begins to understand how the church, 
the schools, political leaders, social agencies, and other groups affect the 
community’s thinking on these matters. One learns how far each commun- 
ity goes in controlling the sexual activities of its individual members, and 
how its law enforcement officials act when sexual situations are involved. 
The communities with which we have maintained such long-time contacts 
include: 

College communities connected with a variety of institutions 
Several upper middle-class groups 
Several professional groups 
A remote and isolated rural community 

A concentrated and rather large homosexual community in a large city 
A Negro underworld community in a large city 
Several penal institutional groups 
A white male underworld group in 9 . large city 

THE TWELVE-WAY BREAKDOWN 

It has previously been pointed out that the analyses in the present study 
have depended upon successive breakdowns of the total population on the 
basis of twelve biologic and socio-economic factors. Each of the ultimate 
groups resulting from these breakdowns is, in consequence, homogeneous 
in respect to these twelve items. The exact nature of each item involved is 
shown in the following tabulation. 

1. Sex. A 2-way breakdown into male and female populations. 

2. Race-cultural Group. An 11 -way or further breakdown into groups 
which are : 

(1) American and Canadian White 

(2) American and Canadian Neg^ro 

(3) British (Great Britain) 

(4) Western and Northern European 

(5) Mediterranean European 

(6) Latin American 

(7) Slavic 

(8) Oriental (Asia) 



16 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


(9) Filipino 

(10) Polynesian 

(11) American Indian 

There are still other groups to be considered and further breakdowns of 
the above which can be made whenever the material becomes available. 
The question is one of race-cultural backgrounds, rather than racial back- 
ground in the exclusively biologic sense; and the subject’s place of birth, 
his place of residence during childhood and adolescent years, and the 
ancestral home of the parents decide the race-cultural group to which he 
belongs. An individual may be placed in two or more of these groups if 
he has lived for appreciable periods of time in two or more of the areas, 
particularly if he has ever had an adolescent background which is definitely 
different from that of the United States in which, he is now living. 

The present volume is confined to a record on American and Canadian 
whites, but we have begun accumulating material which wall make it 
possible to include the American and Canadian Negro groups in later 
publications. Several hundred histories from still other race-cultural groups 
begin to show the fundamental differences which exist betw^een American 
and other patterns of sexual behavior, but the material is not yet sufficient 
for publication. 

3. Marital Status. A 3-way breakdown into single, married, and post- 
marital groups. The single persons have never been married. The married 
persons were living, at the time they contributed their histories, either in 
formally consummated legal marriages or in common-law relations that 
had lasted for a year or more. The post-marital cases w'ere widow'ed, 
divorced, or permanently separated from their former spouses. 

4. Age. An 18~way breakdown by five-year periods, ranging from a 
group which has its maximum age at 5, to a group wath a maximum age 
of 90. The chief difficulty involved here is the existence of three systems for 
designating age. Each system is more or less confined to a particular social 
level, persons of lower levels usually calculating in terms of their forth- 
coming birthdays, while better educated persons are in the habit of express- 
ing their ages in terms of past birthdays. Some persons (perhaps most 
commonly in the middle classes) express their ages in terms of their nearest 
birthdays, and this is the system used by the insurance companies and by 
many government agencies. The error introduced by these diverse systems 
is rarely compensated for in the literature of the social sciences (Pearl 
1940: 74). Ages given in institutional records are likely to be in error by a 
year, especially where the data apply to lower level inmates. Throughout 
the present study an attempt has been made to determine the precise year 
of birth and to calculate all ages as from the past birthday; but this has 
not always been possible and, consequently, differences in mean ages of 
two groups that are not more than a year apart are never to be taken as 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


77 


significant, because of the uncertainty involved in the original record 
(also cf, U. S. Census 1940: Populat. 4 (l):2-4). 

5. Age at Adolescence. A 6-way or further breakdown based on the age 
of the subject at the time of the onset of adolescence. The determination 
of the year involved in the onset of adolescence is described in Chapters 
5 and 9. The breakdown is as follows: Those who start adolescence at 10 
or earlier, at 1 1, at 12, at 13, at 14, and at 15 and later. 

6. Educational Level. A 9-way breakdown on the basis of the number of 
years in a completed educational history, by two-year periods. This classi- 
fication can be made for those who have permanently stopped their school- 
ing before contributing a history, but it cannot be made for those who are 
still in school. Specifically, the groups have had the following years of 
schooling: 0-2, 3-4, 5-6, 7-8, 9-10, 11-12, 13-14, 15-16, 17 plus. The last 
group includes ail those who have done any graduate work. The classifica- 
tion depends upon the educational level attained by the individual, rather 
than upon the number of years required to reach that level. On the other 
hand, ever since state laws have required a minimum number of years of 
school attendance, there have been school systems which pass pupils 
through the grades and even into high school without respect to their 
actual achievements, and it is occasionally possible to find an iUiterate or 
even a feeble-minded child who has been in the eighth or ninth grade in 
school. In such cases, the educational rating of the individual should be 
lowered to a grade approximating the one in which he could perform 
satisfactory work. In cases of persons who have acquired their education 
through private tutoring or through their own independent reading and 
travel, as sometimes happens in families of upper social levels, the educa- 
tional rating should approximate the level to which the individual’s 
achievements would have carried him in a formal school system. There are, 
however, few instances where it is necessary to make such arbitrary adjust- 
ments and, on the whole, the raw rating of an educational level is the best 
single indicator of the social stratum to which an individual belongs 
(Chapter 10). 

7. Occupational Class of Subject. A 10-way breakdown based upon the 
classes developed by Chapin (1933) and W. Lloyd Warner (Warner and 
Lunt 1941, 1942, Warner and Srole 1945), and modified by other workers 
(Hollingshead 1939). This is an attempt to designate the .social status of an 
individual by measuring the prestige of the work in which he is engaged. 
Persons within each occupational class not only work together, but carry 
on their social activities together. They are less often involved in social 
activities with persons of other occupational classes. The classification 
does not depend upon the individual’s income. Warner’s original classifi- 
cation has been adapted to the needs of the present study in the following 
manner: 



78 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


(0) DEPENDENTS. If the Subject is an adult who is dependent upon the 
State or upon a person other than a spouse for his or her support, the 
classification is 0. If the individual is a minor dependent upon his parents 
or other guardians, the classification is shown as a 0, with the classification 
of the parents shown in parenthesis, e.g., 0 (5) for a minor from a home 
which belongs to class 5. The classification of a dependent wife is that of 
her husband. 

(1) UNDERWORLD. Deriving a significant portion of the income from 
illicit activities: e.g., bootleggers, con men, dope peddlers, gamblers, 
hold-up men, pimps, prostitutes, etc. 

(2) DAY LABOR. Persons employed by the hour for labor which does not 
require special training: e.g., construction labor, domestic help, factory 
labor, farm hands, junk and trash collectors, laundry help, maids, messenger 
boys, porters, railroad section hands, stevedores, WPA labor, etc. 

(3) SEMI-SKILLED LABOR. Persons employed by the hour or on other 
temporary bases for tasks involving some minimum of training: e.g., semi- 
skilled labor in factories or on construction jobs, bartenders, bell hops, 
blacksmiths, cooks (some), elevator operators, filling station attendants, 
firemen on railroads, firemen in cities, marines, miners, policemen, prize 
fighters, sailors, showmen, soldiers, stationary engineers, street car con- 
ductors, taxi drivers, truck drivers, ushers, etc. 

(4) SKILLED LABOR. Persons involved in manual activities which require 
training and experience. Employed either by the hour or more often for 
piece work, or on salary: e.g., skilled workmen as defined by labor unions, 
in factories or on construction jobs, athletes (professional), bakers, barbers, 
bricklayers (skilled), carpenters (skilled), cooks (skilled), dressmakers 
(skilled), electricians, farm owners (some^ foremen in factories, linemen, 
machinists, masons, mechanics (skilled), plumbers, printers, radio techni- 
cians, tool and die makers, welders, etc. 

(5) LOWER WHITE COLLAR GROUP. Persons involved in work which is not 
primarily manual but which more particularly depends upon their educa- 
tional background and mental capacity: e.g,, army officers (some), bank 
clerks, bookkeepers, clergymen (in smaller churches), clerks in offices, 
clerks in better stores, express and postal agents, salesmen (some), secre- 
taries, small store owners, small business operators, stenographers, farmers 
(some), insurance agents, musicians (some), nurses, navy officers (some), 
political officers (some), railroad conductors, teachers in grade schools, 
laboratory technicians, etc, 

(6) UPPER WHITE COLLAR GROUP. Including persons of some importance 
in the business group, army officers (some), bank officials, certified public 
accountants, clergymen (most), better store owners, better actors, artists, 
and musicians, navy officers (some), school teachers in high schools. 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


79 


school principals, farm and ranch owners (of better rank), management in 
construction and other businesses, higher political officers, some lawyers, 
some dentists, most salesmen, welfare workers, etc. 

(7) PROFESSIONAL GROUP. Persons holding positions that depend upon 
professional training which is usually beyond the college level: e.g., college 
professors, trained lawyers, physicians, dentists (with better training), 
trained engineers ; some actors, artists, musicians, and writers; some clergy- 
men, etc. 

(8) BUSINESS EXECUTIVE GROUP. Primarily executive officers in larger 
businesses, and persons holding high social rank because of financial 
status or because of hereditary family position, including persons in the 
Social Register. 

(9) EXTREMELY WEALTHY GROUP. Living primarily on income and 
occupying high social status because of their monied position and/or their 
family backgrounds. 

8. Occupational Class of Parent. A 10-way breakdown on the same basis 
as that for the occupational class of the subject. Significant as a measure of 
the childhood and educational backgrounds of the subject. 

9. Rural-Urban Background. A 5-way breakdown as follows : 

(0) Never lived on an operating farm 

(1) Incidental residence of at least a year, but not for any long period 

of years in rural areas* 

(2) Primarily rural, up to 11 years of age 

(3) Primarily rural, between 12 and 18 years of age 

(4) Primarily rural, after 18 years of age 

The classification gives an opportunity to measure the effect of rural 
backgrounds during those periods in childhood and adolescence which are 
most important in the development of sexual patterns. A single individual 
may fall into more than one of these classes. Town farmers who live on 
farms which are operated for them by other persons, while they maintain 
businesses and social interests in the city, are not treated as rural. 

10. Religious Groups. A 3-way or further breakdown into Protestant, 
Catholic, Jewish and other groups. Based upon membership, attendance, 
or any degree of activity or nominal connection with a religious group, in 
any period of the subject’s life. A particular subject may belong to more 
than one such group within his lifetime. 

11. Religious Adherence. A 4-way breakdown showing the degree of 
active connection with a particular religious group, as follows : 

(1) Actively concerned in a religious group, either as a regular attendant 
or as an active participant in organized church activities. For devout 
Cathohcs, frequency of attendance at confession, and for Orthodox Jews, 


4 




Figure 3. Principle involved in a twelve-way breakdown 
Showing items used in the analyses in the present study. 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


81 


frequency of attendance at the Synagogue and the extent to which the 
Orthodox observances are followed, provide measures of the individual’s 
concern with his religion. 

(2) Fairly frequent church attendance or activity. 

(3) Infrequent church attendance or activity. 

(4) Practically no church attendance or activity, although the individ- 
ual’s background may still be classifiable as Protestant, Catholic, or Jewish. 

12. Geographic Origin, A breakdown which will be made as soon as 
the sample in the present study is sufficiently large. Residence is defined as 
continuous living in a given place for a period of at least twelve months. 
A single individual may, therefore, claim several places of residence in a 
lifetime. The state of residence for the most continuous period of time, 
and the place of residence during the childhood and adolescent years, will 
probably represent the most significant part of the data. 

Successive breakdowns on these twelve items give a geometrically 
expanding array (Figure 3) which terminates in a great series of popula- 
tions, each of which is homogeneous for all of the items involved in the 
breakdown. With only 12,000 cases now in hand, it has not been possible 
to make more than a 6- or 7-way breakdown at any point in the analysis, 
and there are some places at which it is impossible to make anything 
beyond a 5-way breakdown. As the study progresses, it should be possible 
and will be desirable to make th^ 12-way breakdowns outlined above. 

Psychologic measurements of mental capacity have been available on 
two or three thousand of the persons who have contributed to this study. 
Unfortunately, the tests used have been so diverse, and administered by 
such a diversity of testers (in schools, colleges, and mental and penal 
institutions) that it has proved difficult to coordinate the measurements. 
An investigation of the possibility of a correlation between mental level 
and patterns of sexual behavior should be undertaken in the further 
development of the present research program. 

The number of groups in the theoretic 12-way breakdown outlined 
above is nearly two billion, and a complete survey of the whole population 
would, obviously, be impossible if there were no means of reducing the 
problem. Fortunately, many of the theoretic groups are non-existent, or 
so rare in the American population that they are unimportant for study. 
For instance, it would never be possible to secure a statistically good sample 
of Orthodox Jewish males who were Negro, single, between the ages of 
eighty-five and ninety, illiterate, living in rural areas, and belonging to the 
Social Register. Again, the problem may be reduced by confining the study 
for the time being to American and Canadian white and Negro groups, and 
the theoretic eleven race-cultural groups are thus reduced to two. Since the 
age groups between 10 and 60 are the ones most frequently met with, the 



82 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


age breakdown may be limited to 1 1 instead of the theoretic 1 8 groups. 
In some other classifications, the problem can similarly be confined. Pre- 
liminary experience indicates that some groups are so similar that they 
may be thrown together for analyses. Thus the theoretic 9-way breakdown 
on the basis of the subject’s educational history can become a 6-way break- 
down into groups having 0-4, 5-8, 9-10, 11-12, 13-16, and 17 plus years 
of schooling; and for most purposes it can become a 3-way breakdown 
into groups having 0-8, 9-12, and 13 plus years of schooling. Finally, the 
problem is tremendously reduced because the history of each older person 
covers data for all the earlier 5-year age periods and, therefore, supplies 
cases for several of the breakdowns. This reduces the total problem to a 
small fraction of its theoretic magnitude, and brings it within the range 
of possible study, 

SIZE OF SAMPLE 

The number of factors which actually affect human sexual behavior 
must far exceed the twelve listed above. In spite of the degree of homo- 
geneity which such a breakdown brings, there is a considerable amount of 
variation still to be found in each ultimate group. In order to understand 
any group it is necessary, therefore, to secure a sample of such size as will 
show the full range of the variation in the group, and show the frequency 
with which each type of variant occurs in the group. This is possible only 
with samples of some size. 

In studies where an over-all picture c^f a total, undivided population is 
a chief objective, as in some of the Department of Agriculture surveys, and 
in most of the problems with which the Census Bureau has been concerned 
(Stephan, Deming, and Hansen 1940), the sampling has been considered 
sufficient when a small group of individuals represents each ultimate cell 
in the population. The validity of such a procedure may, however, be 
debatable even for over-all surveys, and it is certainly inadequate when an 
understanding of the variability within any sub-group is the prime concern. 
Persons who have recommended that we use pin-point sampling, and those 
who have urged that an elaboration of the techniques of factor analysis 
could accomplish the ends of this research with a sample of much smaller 
size, have failed to comprehend that the chief concern of the present study 
is an understanding of the sexual behavior of each segment of the popula- 
tion, and that it is only secondarily concerned with generalizations for the 
population as a whole. As subsequent chapters will indicate, there are 
segments of the population which engage in particular kinds of sexual 
activities with frequencies that average 10 or 20 times as high as the fre- 
quencies in other segments of the population. Scientifically and socially it 
is of the greatest importance to understand why populations differ as much 
as that. Pin-point sampling which is designed to secure an over-all picture 
of a total population provides no basis for analyzing factors which account 
for differences between groups, and it even obscures such differences, 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


83 


reducing all measures to the sort of mediocrity which a combination of high 
and low scores always gives. 

It has, then, been of prime importance in the present study to determine 
the size of the sample needed in each ultimate group. Such a determination 
has been attempted through the strictly pragmatic procedure of making 
calculations on series of samples of different sizes. Means and medians* 
for both total and active populations, the incidences of active cases, the 
range, the height of the mode, and the locus of the mode have been calcu- 
lated for each of the 698 samples which have been used in this study. 
Systematic comparisons of the results obtained from the populations of 
various sizes have provided information on the size of sample necessary 
for securing relatively stable results. The detailed data are shown in Tables 
155 to 162, which form an Appendix in the present volume. A summary of 
the material shown in those tables is presented here as Table 2. 

In every instance, the samples used in the present study have represented 
populations that were made homogeneous for sex, race, marital status, age, 
educational level, and either the rural-urban background or the religious 
background of the individual. The samples of various sizes have all been 
selected by a randomization performed on IBM machines. The successive 
samples in each problem contained 50, 100, 200, 300, 400, and (wherever 
the material was available) 600, 1000, and 1500 cases. Where there was still 
additional material, calculations were made for the total number of cases. 
In some instances more than 2'^0 histories were available for the final 
calculations. 

The samples of successive size were all selected directly out of the total 
population, i.e., the original sample of 50 cases was turned back into the 
total population after calculation, and the sample of 100 cases was then 
chosen from the total population. This process was repeated for each 
sample of subsequent size. In no instance was the larger sample obtained 
by adding cases to the smaller sample. If the latter procedure had been used, 
the results might have been confused because of variation in the increments 
which were added. The present method of selecting each sample has strictly 
confined the study to the problem of sample size. 

Table 2 shows how many of the samples of various sizes (50, 100, 200, 
etc.) gave statistical calculations that were close enough to calculations 
derived from the largest samples to have been acceptable without the fur- 
ther accumulation of cases. The statistics derived from the largest sample 
in each series have been the bases for measuring the adequacy of the results 
obtained from the smaller samples. The smaller samples were identified as 
adequate whenever the statistics calculated from them came within 5 per 
cent, plus or minus (f.e., within a total range of 10 per cent), of the corres- 

* For definitions and explanations of the statistical terms used here, see later sections 
of the present chapter. 



SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUIvlAN MALE 


84 . 


Statistic 


Percent of Samples Which Prove Adequate 
When Size of Sample is: 



50 

100 

200 

1 300 

400 

i 600 

1000 

’ 1500 

mean FREQ., TOTAL POPULA- 

0/ 

/o 

O/ 

/ o 

/o 

O " 

/o 

0 ^ 

/ 0 

0 

0 

'*() 

O 

/o 

TION 








i 

All outlets 

23 

36 

54 

61 

72 

67 

77 

1 67 

Masturbation 

16 

53 

58 

74 

88 

67 

100 

j 100 

Nocturnal emissions 

41 

50 

59 

77 

84 

100 

100 

67 

Pre-marital intercourse 

16 

16 

37 

21 

65 

50 

50 

33 

Homosexual 

MEAN FREQ., ACTIVE POPU- 

5 

11 

37 

32 

12 

17 

33 

33 

LATION 









All outlets 

21 

31 

50 

63 

69 

67 

79 

73 

Masturbation 

21 

47 

58 

63 

88 

50 

100 

! 100 

Nocturnal emissions 

23 

45 

50 

68 

84 

100 

100 

i 67 

Pre-marital intercourse 

26 

16 

26 

37 

53 

50 

50 

33 

Homosexual 

MEDIAN FREQ., TOTAL POPU- 

0 

0 

32 

47 

24 

33 

67 

67 

LATION 

All outlets 

14 

34 

49 

64 

67 

75 

90 

50 

Masturbation 

5 

42 

47 

63 

65 

67 

100 

33 

Nocturnal emissions 

18 

22 

33 

61 

63 

100 

100 

67 

Pre-marital intercourse 

MEDIAN FREQ., ACTIVE POPU- 

0 

13 

25 

43 

29 

0 

50 

0 

LATION 









All outlets 

21 

31 

48 

52 

71 

67 

73 

67 

Masturbation 

11 

32 

37 

58 

71 

67 

100 

67 

Nocturnal emissions 

32 

36 

5^ 

50 

89 

100 

100 

100 

Pre-marital intercourse 

21 

16 

32 

37 

53 

67 

33 

67 

Homosexual 

16 

21 

32 

37 

41 

17 

50 

33 

INCIDENCE 









All outlets 

28 

39 

47 

58 

67 

63 

73 

67 

Masturbation 

21 

32 

47 

84 

88 

83 

100 

100 

Nocturnal emissions 

18 

36 

41 

59 

68 

67 

83 

100 

Pre-marital intercourse 

11 

11 

26 

26 

41 

33 

67 


Homosexual 

RANGE (MINUS 1) 

5 

11 

21 

16 

29 

33 

17 

*33 

AH outlets 

0 

8 

17 

37 

51 

23 

67 

67 

Masturbation 

0 

11 

26 

32 

47 

50 

83 

67 

Nocturnal emissions 

0 

5 

14 

45 

63 

17 

50 

33 

Pre-marital intercourse 

0 

11 

11 

26 

35 

33 

67 

67 

Homosexual 

0 

11 

16 

47 

41 

17 

83 

100 

MODE : HEIGHT 









All outlets 

35 

43 

50 

73 

85 

80 

93 

80 

Masturbation 

5 

37 

42 

58 

82 

67 

100 

67 

Nocturnal emissions 

36 

50 

45 

91 

79 

100 

83 

100 

Pre-marital intercourse 

42 

53 

53 

74 

100 

83 

100 

100 

Homosexual 

68 

58 

84 

95 

100 

100 

100 

100 

All statistics 

23% 

35% 

47% 

60% 

69% 

63% 

79% 

69% 

Number of samples studied 

769 

768 

768 

769 

668 

215 

214 

108 


Table 2. Size of sample versus adequacy of sample 
For an explanation, see the accompanying text. 




STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


85 


ponding statistics derived from the largest samples. All comparisons shown 
in the table have been on this 5 per cent basis, except the comparisons of 
incidence data, for which a range of error of only 2 per cent was allowed. 
These definitions of adequacy have been, of course, quite arbitrary. It is 
obviously possible to calculate the adequacy of each sample when a larger 
range of error, or when only a smaller range of error, is allowed. A whole 
series of such calculations should be made before this sample study is 
completed ; but such an extended statistical survey must be pursued else- 
where, rather than in the present volume. 

An examination of Table 2 warrants a number of conclusions concerning 
the size of the sample that is needed for each of the ultimate cells in the 
present study. These generalizations will need to be modified before they 
are extended to problems in other fields; but they have served as guides in 
the set-up of the immediate problem, and should provide some help to 
others who are interested in setting up similar surveys. 

1. Samples of 50 cases chosen at random after a 6-way breakdown of the total popu- 
lation occasionally give results which are within 5 per cent of those obtained from 
samples of 1000, 1500, or more cases. This happens in something between 5 and 60 per 
cent of all the problems which we have worked; but in most categories hardly 20 per cent 
of the samples of 50 prove adequate. If a sample of 50 is all that is used, the calculations 
of various statistics for various types of sexual outlet could not be depended upon in 
more than 1 in 5 cases. 

2. A sample of 1 00 proves adequate, by the above definition, in a much larger number 
of cases. 1 in 3 or even 1 in 2 of the samples of 100 give results which are within 5 per 
cent, plus or minus, of those obtained from the largest samples. 

3. There has been a corresponding increase in the quality of these samples when the 
cases were increased to 200. 

4. There is a still more marked increase in adequacy when 300 cases are used. On 
most of the statistics calculated on populations of 300, two-thirds to three-fourths or 
more of all the samples give results which are nearly identical to those obtained from 
the largest samples. 

5. Samples of 400 show still improved quality in regard to nearly all the calculated 
statistics except the frequency data; but the improvement is hardly enough to warrant 
the time and effort involved in gathering the last 100 cases — ^unless it is important to 
obtain greater precision than 300 cases afford. 

6. Samples of various sizes between 400 and 1500, or even 2700 cases, fail to show 
any consistent improvement. By standard statistical theory, a steady albeit slow im- 
provement in the quality of the calculations might have been expected as the size of the 
sample was increased. We fail to find that this is so in the present problem. On the con- 
trary, the statistics calculated on the larger samples vary erratically from sample to 
sample, almost as much as they do between populations of two, three, and four hundred 
cases. 

7. The incidence data are the most stable, and samples of 50 or 100 give results 
which, in many cases, are comparable to those obtained from the largest samples 
(Figures 4, 5). If samples of 200 or 300 are used, in half or more of these small samples 
the incidence data fall within 2 per cent of those obtained from the larger samples. 

8. The locus of the mode is adequately determined in two-thirds of the samples of 
50 cases, and in 80 or 90 per cent of the samples of 100 cases. This statement would be 
modified, of course, if the categories used in the frequency distributions were more or 
less extended than those which have been used in the present study (Figures 4, 5). 



SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 



o S S S 55 

o o o o 

S r5 to xj* 


S 8 § s 8 Si 

K> Tj- Tt 


o o o o o 
Cn o s o 

— «N< rt 


PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS- SINCLE MALES • EDUC LEVEL 0-8 * PROTESTANT INACTIVE 

AGE I6-Z0 


o o 
o o 

K> ^ ^ 


O O O o O 

^ 2 S ^ 5 




o o o 
o ^ 


HOMOSEXUAL CONTACTS - SINGLE MALES -AGE 16-ZOEOUC LEVEL 0-8* PROTESTANT INACTIVE 

Figure 4, Relation of size of sample to statistical values 

Size of each sample is shown in the figures at the base of each bar. Means and medians 
in each series are on the same scale, and therefore directly comparable. 


495 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


8 : 


NECESSARY SAMPLE SIZE 

MEAN MEDIAN INCIDENCE RANGE 



TOTAL OUTLET- SINOLE MALES • AOE 21-25 EDUC LEVEL 13+ * PROTESTANT INACTIVE 



MASTURBATION - SINGLE MALES AGE 21-25 • EDUC LEVEL 13+* PROTESTANT INACTIVE 



NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS - SINGLE MALES* AGE 21-ZS • EDUC LEVEL 13+ PROTESTANT INACTIVE 



PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS -SINGLE MALES -AGE 21-25 ■ EDUC LEVEL • PROTEST INACTIVE 


Figure 5. Relation of size of sample to statistical values 

Size of each sample is shown in the figures at the base of each bar. Means and mediani: 
in each series are on the same scale, and therefore directly comparable. 




88 


SEXUAL BERAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


9. For most of the other statistics, samples of 300 are markedly better than samples 
of 200, except for the frequency data where samples of 400 are necessaiy’ to obtain con- 
sistent results (Figures 4, 5). 

10. The range of variation w'hich actually exists in a population is not adequately 
shown by any small sample. There is a steady extension of the range of variation through 
samples of 300 or 400, and m some cases the range is materially increased by still larger 
samples (Figures 4-7). 

11. Frequency curves become increasingly smooth as the samples increase in size, at 
least up to 200 or 300 cases (Figures 6, 7). On some problems, they do not reach the 
ultimate degree of smoothness until 400 or 500 cases are used; but it is a waste of time 
and effort to secure a larger series of cases. Frequency curves never do reach the ideal 
in smoothness, at least with any large sample of the size (2700) which we have had for 
testing. 

12. It is well known statistically that the adequacy of a sample depends upon its range 
of variation as well as upon its size. There are, therefore, some phenomena that may be 
sufficiently illustrated by samples that are inadequate for measuring other phenomena. 
The frequencies of masturbation, for instance, show a wider range of variation than the 
frequencies of nocturnal emissions, and the latter are sufficiently explored (Table 2) 
with a much smaller sample than would serve for describing masturbation in the same 
population. The size of a sample in a case history study, however, must be adequate 
for the examination of the most variable phenomenon which is to be studied. 

13. Balancing the diverse considerations outlined above, we reach the conclusion 
that samples of 300 are desirable in each of the ultimate cells of the present study. 
Samples of 400 are enough better to warrant gathering that many histones when they 
are available. Samples of still larger size do not add enough information to warrant 
their use, and we have avoided going after such samples. The larger samples which are 
shown in a few places in the present volume have been obtained for the sake of an ulti- 
mate 7-, 8-, or even 12-way breakdown of the data. 

14. While samples of 300 are more depend^jble than smaller samples, calculations 
based on samples of 100 or 200 have considerable significance, and calculations made 
in the present volume on samples of that size need not be dismissed as inadequate 
(Table 2, Figures 4, 5). 

15. In a few cases, samples of 50 give a good indication of the results that a large 
sample would give. However, such small samples have been used in the present volume 
only when they belong to series for which most of the points are established 'yv relatively 
large samples. Samples of 50 are used, for instance, to place older groups in age series 
for which larger samples of younger males have already establishedlhe trends. 

16. Samples of less than fifty cases have not been used for any of the calculations in 
this volume. On occasion, incidental references have been made to such small groups. 

17. All of the above conclusions apply to populations w'hich are homogeneous for 
six of the factors which are used in the basic breakdown of the present problem. Pre- 
liminary calculations indicate that when seven or more breakdowns are made, the increas- 
ing homogeneity of each cell makes it possible to base analyses on something less than 
the three hundred cases called for above. Pragmatic tests of the size of sample necessary 
for these more complex breakdowns will have to be made as this research progresses. 

It is customary in statistics to measure the accuracy of a calculated 
mean by computing its “standard deviation” (represented by the symbol 
or by some similar measure of significance. This defines the limits on 
either side of the calculated mean, within which there is a 2 to 1 chance 
that the actual mean, the reality, may fall. Unfortunately, standard devia- 
tions of means are sometimes misinterpreted as measures of the adequacy 
of the samples on which they are based. In Tables 155-156 in the Appen- 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


89 


dix, standard deviations are attached to all of the means calculated for the 
samples of various sizes. Some idea of the eifect of adding cases to origi- 
nally smaller samples may be obtained from an examination of these 
standard deviations. More extensive comparisons of the significance of 
these statistical measures, in contrast to the results obtained by the prag- 
matic testing of sample size, will need to be made elsewhere at some later 
date. 

Where the distribution of the variants in a population is fairly homo- 
geneous (as in some physical universes), and where the range of variation 
is within limits which can be fairly well anticipated (again, as in some 
physical universes), a relatively small sample may be representative of the 
whole. But in the living world the distribution of the variants in any 
population is usually more irregular, and it is less often possible to antici- 
pate the full range of variation. The number of factors affecting living 
protoplasm, and particularly the number of factors affecting the behavior 
of whole organisms, is infinitely greater than the number affecting most 
physical phenomena. There is, in consequence, much greater variation 
among living structures and biological phenomena. Behavior characters 
vary even more than physiologic characters, and these in turn vary more 
than morphologic characters (Pearl 1946:43 ff.). Frequency distributions of 
physical phenomena often follow standard curves or simple permutations 
thereof ; but frequency distributions in the living world are rarely normal, 
and usually fall into irregular ci^rves that are sometimes not even smooth 
curves, as our own work on insect measurements has shown (preliminarily 
reported in Kinsey 1942), and as the frequency curves in the present volume 
will also demonstrate. 

In such non-homogeneous populations, it is quite possible to collect a 
few individuals so nearly alike that the standard deviation of the mean is 
small Unfortunately, in too many biologic, psychologic, sociologic, and 
anthropologic studies, including some of the published sex studies, such 
small standard deviations are taken as indicators of the adequacy of a 
sample, even though it may have only a half dozen or a dozen or a score 
or two individuals in it. Such a use of standard deviations or probable 
errors as measures of validity involves a misunderstanding of their real 
nature and function. The student with practical experience in taxonomy 
or in human surveying soon learns that the addition of a few more cases 
to such small samples may introduce data that are outside of the range of 
variation covered by the original specimens, and that such additions may 
alter the original calculations to an extent which would never have been 
anticipated through an examination of the standard deviations of the 
means. Each investigator must know the general order of the variation 
that may occur in the material with which he works, see to it that the 
sample is well spread through the whole range of variation, and learn 



90 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


NECESSARY SAMPLE SIZE 



Figure 6. Relation of size of sample to form of frequency curve 

Showing frequency distributions for total outlet. Based on single males, belonging to 
the age group 16-20^ of grade school level (0-8), and inactive Protestant. 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


91 




MODe=lO 


FREaUENCY PER WEEK 


Figure 7. Relation of size of sample to form of frequency curve 
Showing frequency distributions for masturbation. Based on single males belonging 
to the age group 16-20, of college level (13+), and inactive Protestant. 






92 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


through some pragmatic means the general order of the sample size that 
will begin to represent the whole of the universe that is being sampled. At 
that point, and not before, standard deviations serve to indicate the range 
within which the calculated means may match reality. It is for that purpose, 
and not as measures of the adequacy of the samples, that standard devia- 
tions have been calculated and attached to the means shown in the tables 
throughout this volume. 

It is important to understand that the sampling techniques used in the 
present study call for more or less equal samples from each of the ultimate 
groups, irrespective of the relative size of each of those groups in the 
population as a whole. This has been called “stratified sampling” (Snede- 
cor 1946. See Whelpton and Kiser 1943-1945 for an instance of its use). 
On the other hand, many persons think of sampling as a technique that 
draws from each group in proportion to the size of that group in the total 
population. This is “representative sampling.” Such samples may, in 
actuality, serve when the objective is a single set of figures which will 
describe the entire population. But whenever one attempts to understand 
the particular groups of which a population is composed, such a course 
is unacceptable because data so obtained are of variable reliability, due to 
the differences in the sizes of the samples which represent the several seg- 
ments of the population. For instance, Negroes constitute less than 10 
per cent of the total population of the United States (U. S. Census 1940); 
but a Negro sample that was only a tSnth as large as the white sample 
would be much less adequate than the white sample. If one is to study 
Negroes as a group, one should have as many Negro cases as white. 
Similarly, the samples for each of the other cells in the present study should 
be more or less equal in size. This is a principle on which the public opinion 
surveys depend, and the principle about which the present study has been 
organized. 

DIVERSIFICATION OF SAMPLE 

In a physical universe, or even in measuring dead insects, it is possible 
to choose the cases which enter into any sample by some carefully planned 
system of randomization which avoids bias on the part of the investigator, 
and minimizes those fortuitous circumstances which account for the irreg- 
ular distribution of particular kinds of individuals within a population. 
By the same token, the ideal set-up in a human study would involve a pre- 
liminary survey in which every person in the total population, or a random- 
ized percentage of all persons, would be required to provide the informa- 
tion which would allow him to be classified on the basis of the items in- 
volved in the analysis of the problem {e.g., the six-way or twelve-way 
breakdown in the present study). From the persons that fall into each 
ultimate cell, the necessary number of cases would then be selected by some 
thorough scheme of randomization, and persuaded or commanded to con- 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


93 


tribute the full and complete data necessary in the survey. A recent survey 
of factors affecting fertility, sponsored by the Milbank Foundation Fund, 
chose its sample in this way from white couples in the city of Indianapolis 
(Whelpton and Kiser 1943-1945). 

Unfortunately, human subjects cannot be regimented as easily as cards 
in a deck, and the investigator of human behavior faces sampling problems 
which are not sufficiently allowed for by pencil and paper statisticians. In 
a nation-wide survey, it would be impossible to make the preliminary 
investigation necessary for classifying the population on a twelve-way, or 
even a six-way breakdown. Neither is it feasible to stand on a street corner, 
tap every tenth individual on the shoulder, and command him to contrib- 
ute a full and frankly honest sex history. Theoretically less satisfactory 
but more practical means of sampling human material must be accepted 
as the best that can be done. 

The first principle to observe in securing histories is that of diversifying 
each collection which enters into the sample. Even after a twelve-way 
breakdown, the population in each ultimate cell is still affected by a multi- 
plicity of factors which cause variation in the group. Even after a twelve- 
way breakdown, a sample from one city cannot be taken as representative 
of cities in general. A study based on New York City (as nearly half of the 
previous sex studies have been) cannot be taken as representative of all 
other cities. The population in one city block differs from the population 
in the next block in the same city? A group from one church is not a dupli- 
cate of a group from the next church. The factory workers in one plant do 
not duplicate the factory workers in the next plant. Skilled carpenters must 
not be taken as representative of all skilled craftsmen. The students in 
one girls’ college must not be depended upon for the total sample from 
exclusively girls’ schools. The cases that are used to represent each ultimate 
cell in a human population should be drawn from a number of groups, 
widely distributed geographically, and including as great a diversity as is 
possible within the limits of the group. 

HUNDRED PERCENT SAMPLES 

Since it is impossible to secure a strictly randomized sample, the best 
substitute is to secure one hundred percent of the persons in each social 
unit from which the sample is drawn. One hundred percent of the members 
of a family group, all the persons living in a particular apartment house, all 
the members of a college sorority or fraternity, all the persons in some 
service club, all the members of some Sunday School class or some other 
church organization, all the persons in a city block, all the persons in a 
rural township, all the inmates of some penal or other institution, all the 
persons in some other unit, provided that unit has not been brought to- 
gether by a common sexual interest. 



94 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Hundred Percent versus Partial Samples 
Single White Males of College Level 


GROUP 

sample 

total population 

active population 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

age: adol~15 

Total Outlet 

100% 

Partial 

2.62 ± 0.11 
2.89 0.06 

1.96 

2.28 

95,6 

95.8 

2.74 ± 0.11 
3.02 ± 0.06 

2.07 

2.40 

Masturbation 

100% 

Partial 

2.12 =i= 0.09 
2.25 =fc 0.06 

1.52 

1.64 

82.7 

82.2 

2.56 ± 0.10 
2.74 ± 0.06 

1.94 

2.11 

Nocturnal 

Emissions 

100% 

Partial 

0.30 0.02 

0.35 =1= 0.02 

0.08 

0.11 

67.2 

70.7 

0.45 ± 0.03 
0.49 ± 0.02 

0.26 

0.28 

Pre-marital 

Coitus 

100% 

Partial 

0.05 ± 0.02 

0.08 i 0.01 

0.00 

0.00 

9.5 

9.3 

0.56 ± 0.24 
0.90 ± 0.11 

0.17 

0.30 

Homosexual 

100% 

Partial 

0.06 =i= 0.01 
0.10 0.01 

0.00 

0.00 

17.9 

22.6 

0.32 ± 0.05 
0.43 ± 0.04 

0.08 

0.09 

age: 16-20 

Total Outlet 

100% 

Partial 

2.38 =fc 0.08 

2.80 0.05 

1.85 

2.20 

99.7 

99.8 

2.38 ± 0.08 
2.81 ± 0.05 

1.85 

2.21 

Masturbation 

100% 

Partial 

1.61 ± 0.07 
1.84 0.05 

1.03 

1.24 

88.9 

88.5 

1.81 ± 0.07 
2.08 ± 0.05 

1.29 

1.56 

Nocturnal 

Emissions 

100% 

Partial 

0.42 ± 0.02 
0.42 0.01 

0.24 

^0.25 

91.3 

91.3 

0.46 ± 0 03 
0.47 ± 0.01 

0.28 

0.29 

Pre-marital 

Coitus 

100% 

Partial 

0.18 i 0.02 

0.27 =1= 0.02 

0.00 

0.00 

36.1 

39.6 

0.50 ± 0.05 
0.67 ± 0.04 

0.23 

0.21 

Homosexual 

100% 

Partial 

0.03 =fc 0,01 
0.08 ± 0 01 

0.00 

0.00 

12.3 

17.1 

0.25 ± 0.04 
0.44 ± 0.05 

0.08 

0.09 

age: 21-25 

Total Outlet 

100% 

Partial 

2.14 ± 0.09 
2.57 ± 0.06 

1.64 

1.93 

100.0 

99.8 

2.14 ± 0.09 
2.58 ± 0.06 

1.64 

1.94 

Masturbation 

100% 

Partial 

1.15 ± 0.07 
1.34 ±0.05 

0.66 

0.69 

88.8 

86.5 

1.30 ± 0.07 
1.55 ± 0.05 

0.80 

0.88 

Nocturnal 

Emissions 

100% 

Partial 

0.40 ± 0.03 
0.38 ± 0.02 

0.23 

0.22 

86.4 

87.2 

0.47 ± 0.04 
0.43 ± 0.02 

0.30 

0.28 

1 

Pre-marital 

Coitus 

100% 

Partial 

0,36 ± 0.05 
0.47 ± 0.03 

0.01 

0.03 

52.3 

55.2 

0.68 ± 0.08 
0.86 ± 0.05 

0.30 

0.31 

Homosexual 

100% 

Partial 

0.03 ± 0.01 
0.10 ± 0.02 

0.00 

0.00 

5.7 

10.4 

0.57 ± 0.17 
1.01 ± 0.12 

0.23 

0.32 


Table 3. Comparisons of hundred percent and partial samples 

The “partial samples” include both the hundred percent groups and the volunteers obtained 
outside of the hundred percent groups. Comparisons have been made on this basis in order 
that these “partial samples” should correspond with the samples on which calculations have 
been made throughout the present volume. Populations for the hundred percent samples in 
the three age groups are 655, 664, and 367, respectively; and for the partial samples, 2144, 
2197, and 1531, respectively. 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


95 


Securing a hundred percent of any group is, in actuality, more feasible 
than securing a good random sample of the same group ; for, as already 
noted (Chapter 2), it is possible to develop a community interest in a group 
project, and this puts considerable pressure on each individual to contrib- 
ute as a matter of loyalty or obligation to the group of which he is a part. 
It is, of course, easier to secure a hundred percent of a smaller group, unless 
it be a group of inmates in an institution, and it is ordinarily impossible to 
secure a hundred percent of any group unless the investigators can work 
with it for a period of weeks or months. Ordinarily it is not profitable to try 
to secure a complete sample until an appreciable portion (perhaps a half 
or more) of a group has contributed. Then the first persons who have given 
histories can help develop a group project by enlisting whatever organiza- 
tion there is to make it an official project. The time required to secure such 
a sample is costly, as calculated per history, and that is one reason why 
a larger number of hundred percent groups has not yet been secured for 
the present study. In some cases it has been necessary to work with the last 
few individuals in a group for as long as a year or two before they agree to 
contribute. 

Of the 12,000 histories now at hand in the present study, 3104 (=26%) 
have come from hundred percent groups. These groups have come from 
the following sources: 

Hundred Percent Groups 


% . — . — 

Type of Group 

Number of Groups 

College sororities 

2 

College fraternities 

9 

College student groups 

6 

College classes 

7 

CoUege rooming houses, for unorganized 

5 

Professional groups 

13 

Conscientious objectors 

2 

NYA project 

1 

Junior high school classes 

3 

Speech clinic groups 

3 

Rooming houses, in town 

3 

Hitch-hikers (over a 3-year period) 

1 

Delinquent institutional groups 

4 

Penal institutional groups 

2 

Mental institutional group 

1 

Total 

62 


These hundred percent groups have come from some variety of sources, 
but only the college groups are well enough represented (by series of at 
least 300 cases) to allow their use in testing the validity of the partial sample 
in this study. The accumulation of many more histories in these complete 



96 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Hundred Percent versus Partial Samples: Accumulative Incidence 

Data 

Educational Level 13-|- 


AGE 

masturbation 

nocturnal emissions 

1 

Partial Sample 

100% Sample 

i 

Partial Sample 

100% Sample 

Cases 

1 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases i 

. 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

2815 

0.0 

656 

0.0 

2811 

0.0 

656 

0.0 

9 

2815 

0.3 

656 

0.3 

2811 

0.0 

656 

0.0 

10 

2815 

2.3 

656 

2.1 

2811 

0.5 

656 

0.6 

11 

2815 

8.9 

656 

8.2 

2811 

3.2 

656 

3.2 

12 

2815 

27.9 

656 

26.2 

2811 

11.6 

656 

10.4 

13 

2815 

52.9 

656 

52.3 

2811 

29.1 

656 

25.3 =■ 

14 

2815 

72.2 

656 

72.0 

2811 

52.2 

656 

47.1 

15 

2815 

80.2 

656 

81.3 

2811 

68.9 

656 

67.2 

16 

2814 

84.3 

655 

85.2 

2810 

80.7 

655 

79.8 

17 

2812 

87.0 

655 

88.2 

2808 

87.0 

655 1 

86.9 

18 

2736 

88.9 

611 

90.2 

2732 

91.0 

611 

92.0 

19 

2572 

90.0 

539 

91.8 

f 2568 

92.6 

539 

93.9 

20 

2337 

91.1 

457 

92.3 

2333 

93.6 

457 

95.4 

21 

2031 

92.0 

383 

92.4 

2027 

94.5 

383 

95.0 

22 

1670 

92.8 

312 

93.3 

1668 ! 

94.7 

312 ! 

95.2 

23 

1396 

93.3 

269 

93.3 

1395 

94.8 

269 I 

95.2 

24 

1151 

93.1 

242 

93.8 

1150 

95.7 

242 i 

95.9 

25 

1002 

93.9 

203 

94.6 

1001 ' 

96.2 

203 

97.0 

26 

884 

94.9 

163 

95.7 

883 

96.5 

163 

97.5 

27 

774 

95.3 

124 

96.0 

773 

96.2 

124 

97.6 

28 

699 j 

95.3 

no 

95.5 i 

698 

96.6 

no 

97.3 

29 

634 

95.0 

94 

94.7 ! 

633 

96.8 

94 

97.9 

30 

573 

95.6 

79 

96.2 

572 

97.2 

79 

97.5 

31 

529 

95.3 

73 

95.9 

528 I 

97.3 

73 

97.3 

32 

492 

95.5 

67 

98.5 

491 

97.6 

67 

98.5 

33 

448 

95.3 

58 

98.3 

447 

97.5 

58 

98.3 

34 

412 

95.9 

51 

98.0 

411 

98.1 

51 

98.0 

35 

382 

95.8 



381 

98.4 


36 

356 

95.8 



355 

98.6 


j 

37 

323 

95.7 



322 

98.4 



38 

307 

95.8 



306 

98.7 



39 

280 

95.4 



279 

98.6 



40 

257 

96.1 

! 


256 

99.2 




Table 4. Comparisons of data obtained from partial and hundred percent samples 
Based on males of the college level. 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


97 


HUNDRED PERCENT VERSUS PARTIAL SAMPLES 




AGE 

)00 I 1 1 ] 



AGE 

Figures 8-10. Comparisons of accumulative incidence curves based on hundred 
percent and partial samples 


For males of college level (13+). 





98 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Hundred Percent versus Partial Samples: Accumulative Incidence 

Data 

Educational Level 134- 


AGE 

petting to climax 

TOTAL intercourse 


Partial Sample 

100% Sample 

Partial Sample 

100% Sample 


Cases 

%with 

Cases 

% with 

Cases 

% with 

Cases 

% with 


Exper. 

Exper. 

Exper. 

Exper. 

8 

1596 

0.0 

276 

0.0 

2817 

0.0 

656 

0.0 

9 

1596 

0.0 

276 

0.0 1 

2817 

0.0 

656 

0.0 

10 

1596 

0.0 

276 

0.0 

2817 

0.0 

656 

0.0 

11 

1596 

0.0 

276 

0.0 

2817 

0.2 

656 

0.2 

12 

1596 

0.3 

276 

0.4 

2817 

1.0 

656 

1.1 

13 

1596 

1.6 

276 

1.8 

2817 

3.1 

656 

3.0 

14 

1596 

4.1 

276 

3.6 

2817 

6.0 

656 

5.2 

15 

1596 

7.6 

276 

8.0 

2817 

9.5 

656 

9.5 

16 

1596 

15.5 

276 

16.3 

2816 

15.5 

655 

15.3 

17 

1593 

23.0 

275 

26.2 

2814 

23.1 

655 

24.4 

18 

1534 

31.2 

245 

30.6 

c2738 

30.9 

611 

31.6 

19 

1474 

38.4 

232 

40.1 

2574 

38.3 

539 

37.5 

20 

1389 

46.0 

227 

47.6 

2339 

45.7 

457 

44.0 

21 

1240 

50.1 

214 

50.9 

2033 

50.9 

383 

47.0 

22 

1047 

52.8 

193 

54.4 

1672 

58.3 

312 

56.1 

23 

872 

54.6 

181 

58.0 

1397 

63.0 

269 

61.0 

24 

688 

56.3 

164 

61.6 

1152 

68.8 

242 

68.6 

25 

543 

58.2 

123 

64.2 

1002 

75.0 

203 

72.9 

26 

437 

60.6 

97 

68.0 

884 

80.1 

163 

77.3 

27 

321 

56.7 i 

60 

65.0 

774 

82.6 

124 

79.8 

28 

255 

56.5 j 



699 

85.1 

110 

81.8 

29 

204 

59,3 1 



634 

87.1 ' 

94 

85.1 

30 1 

161 

61.5 



573 

89.5 

79 

87.3 

31 

122 

60.7 



529 

90.9 

73 

89.0 

32 

100 

57.0 



492 

91.3 

67 

89.6 

33 

89 

56.2 



448 

91.1 

58 

89.7 

34 

72 

55.6 



412 

92.5 

51 

94.1 

35 

61 

55.7 



382 

93.2 




Table 5. Comparisons of data obtained from partial and hundred percent samples 


Based on males of the college level. Petting is pre-marital. Total intercourse includes 
pre-marital, marital, extra-marital, and post-marital relations with both companions 
and prostitutes. 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


99 


samples is one of the important things to be followed through in the future 
development of this project. 

By means of Table 3 it is possible to compare the frequency and inci- 
dence figures for the 15 groups on which there are sufficient cases in the 
hundred percent sample. It will be seen that the active incidence figures 
(recording the number of persons who are involved in any particular period 
of time) show a remarkable conformance between the partial sample and 
the hundred percent portion of that sample. The same is true of the accu- 
mulative incidence figures (recording the number of persons who have ever 
been involved) (Tables 4-6, Figures 8-13). The differences usually involve 
1 per cent to 5 per cent of the population. Throughout this study it may, 
therefore, be accepted that both the active and accumulative incidence 
data and curves show the general locus of the reality, though the curves 
may need correction of a few percent one way or the other. For instance, 
the actual, accumulative incidence figure for masturbation in the college 
segment of the population must lie within a few degrees of the 96 per cent 
figure given by the data; and whether it is in actuality 94 per cent or 98 
per cent is not of much moment ; but it is certain that it is not the 85 per cent 
nor 90 per cent figure given by some studies, nor the 100 per cent figure often 
guessed at, nor the 7 per cent figure found in one study (Bromley and 
Britten 1938). Similarly, there can be no question that the actual accumu- 
lative incidence figure for the homosexual in the college-bred group lies 
somewhere between the 28 per ^ent figure derived from the hundred per- 
cent sample and the 34 per cent figure derived from the partial sample of 
college histories, and that it is nowhere near the 1 per cent to 2 per cent 
figure which has been commonly published, nor even the 10 per cent figure 
which has been the maximum previously suggested. 

There are greater discrepancies between the frequency figures (the 
number of times per week each type of activity is engaged in), as calculated 
from the hundred percent samples and from the partial samples. The 
figures derived from the partial samples are consistently higher for the 
total sexual outlet and for all the individual outlets except nocturnal 
emissions. For this, there are a number of possible explanations, and it 
seems impossible to identify the primary factors until we can secure more 
material for analysis. It is quite probable that a number of factors are 
really involved. The following considerations should be kept in mind : 

1. The volunteers who make up the partial sample may represent a 
more active group of individuals, of the type which is aggressive, responds 
to a call for cooperation in a survey, and is more responsive and less 
inhibited sexually. It is true that the last persons to contribute in a hundred 
percent sample are sometimes the more prudish, restrained, apathetic, and 
sexually less active individuals. If this is often true, then the frequency 
figures throughout this volume should be reduced by some percentage, and 



100 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Hundred Percent versus Partial Samples: Accumulative Incidence 

Data 

Educational Level 13+ 


intercourse with prostitutes homosexual outlets 

age 



Partial Sample 

100% Sample 

Partial Sample 

100% Sample 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

! Cases 

! 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

2816 

0.0 

656 

0.0 

2817 

0, 

.0 

656 

0.0 

9 

2816 

0.0 

656 

0.0 

2817 

0, 

.1 

656 

0.0 

10 

2816 

0.0 

656 

0.0 

2817 

0, 

.5 

656 

0.2 

11 

2816 

0.0 

656 

0.0 

2817 

1, 

.8 

656 

1.4 

12 

2816 

0.0 

656 

0.0 

2817 

6, 

.2 

656 

5.0 

13 

2816 

0.1 

656 

0.0 

2817 

11, 

.6 

656 

8.4 

14 

2816 

0.8 

656 

0.2 

2817 

18, 

.0 

656 

14.5 

15 

2816 

2.3 

656 

1.8 

2817 ! 

21. 

.1 

656 

16.9 

16 

2815 

4.8 

655 

4.4 

2816 

23, 

.0 

655 

19.4 

17 

2813 

9.1 

655 

9.8 

2814 

24, 

.1 

655 

20.0 

18 

2737 

13.6 

611 

13.1 

2738 

25, 

.6 

611 

20.8 

19 

2573 

17.4 

539 

15.4 

2574 

26, 

,7 

539 

21.3 

20 1 

2338 

20.6 

457 

19.7 

*2339 

27. 

,6 

457 

23.2 

21 

2032 

22.2 

383 

24.8 

2033 

28. 

,6 

383 

23.5 

22 

1672 

24.9 

312 

26.3 

1672 

29. 

,8 

312 

24.4 

23 1 

1397 

25.7 

269 

21.9 

1397 

31. 

,5 

269 

23.4 

24 ! 

1152 

26.6 

242 

23.6 

1152 

32. 

,1 

242 

25.6 

25 

1002 

28.6 

203 

25.1 

1002 

33. 

.0 

203 

24.1 

26 

884 

29.4 

163 

28.8 

884 

32. 

.9 

163 

23.9 

27 

774 

30.5 

124 

21.8 

774 

33. 

,7 

124 

23.4 

28 

699 

32.0 

110 

23.6 

699 

33, 

.9 

110 

22.7 

29 

634 

32.0 

94 

24.5 

634 1 

33. 

.6 

1 94 

21.3 

30 

573 

33.2 

79 

24.1 

573 

33. 

.7 

79 

22.8 

31 

529 

34.0 

73 

26.0 

529 

34. 

.2 

73 

24.7 

32 

492 

33.7 

67 

23.9 

492 

32. 

.9 

1 67 

23.9 

33 

448 

33.3 

58 

22.4 

448 

33, 

.9 

58 

22.4 

34 

412 

33.7 

51 

21.6 

412 

34. 

.7 

51 

23.5 

35 

382 

34.6 



382 

34. 

.0 



36 

356 

35.7 



356 

33. 

.7 



37 

323 

35.6 



323 

33. 

.4 



38 

307 

36.2 



307 

33. 

.2 



39 

280 

36.4 



280 

33, 

.6 



40 

257 

36.6 



257 

32. 

.7 




Table 6. Comparisons of data obtained from partial and hundred percent samples 


Based on males of the college level. 




PERCENT OF TOTAL POPULATION PERCENT OF TOTAL POPULATION PERCENT OF TOTAL POPULATION 


STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


101 





Figures 1 1-13. Comparisons of accumulative incidence curves based on hundred 
percent and partial samples 


For males of college level (134-). 





102 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


an increasing proportion of the future intake should be secured from hun- 
dred percent groups. However, there are other factors (given below) which 
are undoubtedly involved, and the discount made on the frequency data 
for the partial sample should not be more than some undetermined fraction 
of the difference between the figures for the partial sample and the figures 
for the hundred percent groups. 

2. The hundred percent samples are not entirely representative, for they 
are not as well distributed as the partial sample is through the whole of the 
population, even in the college group from which the largest hundred per- 
cent samples have come. 

3. The hundred percent samples from college groups include an undue 
number of sexually less experienced freshmen, because the freshmen 
groups were large in the particular fraternities which contributed most 
heavily to these samples. Moreover, 28 per cent of the hundred percent 
sample is Jewish, while only 10 per cent of the partial sample is Jewish. 
The Jewish histories (Chapter 13) are less active than the histories of some 
other groups, and this will to some extent account for the lower figures in 
the present hundred percent sample. 

4. The persons contributing to the hundred percent samples may have 
covered up more of the fact, because they did not contribute as willingly 
as the volunteers who made up the partial sample. 

5. Persons with socially taboo items (e.^., pre-marital intercourse, extra- 
marital intercourse, homosexual activity, animal contacts) in their histories 
are often among the last to contribute to a hundred percent sample, and 
in a number of instances complete collections may have been forestalled 
by such persons. On the other hand, these special histories can be secured 
in a partial sample by making contacts through the friends of these persons. 
There is no doubt that the more extreme histories will always have to be 
obtained in some way other than through hundred percent samples. 

6. The hundred percent samples are of smaller size than the partial 
samples, and therefore less reliable. The partial samples show wider ranges 
of variation, and this raises the values of the means. With larger series, the 
means in the hundred percent samples might be raised. 

CONTROLLING PARTIAL SAMPLES 

The above comparisons indicate that there is considerable merit to 
samples obtained from volunteers who respond to a general appeal for 
histories at a lecture, or through some organizational agency, or who 
respond to a more individual appeal. But such volunteer samples can be 
quite inadequate, if they are not safeguarded at every step in a study. 

1. All general appeals for histories have emphasized the importance of 
securing every kind of history — “histories that have everything in them 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


103 


and histories that are complete blanks” — “big histories and little histories 
and every other kind of history” — “histories that are quite usual and his- 
tories that have things in them that some people consider wrong or abnor- 
mal, but which we accept as objectively as any other kind of history.” 
The restrained histories have, on the whole, been the more difficult to get, 
and it has been constantly necessary to reassure individuals with relatively 
inactive histories that they were contributing to the study in as important a 
way as the persons with more active histories. 

2. Contact persons have had to be educated to understand that “a good 
history” is a history that accurately reports everything, rather than a his- 
tory that has some special element in it. Especially at lower levels, where 
the contact men have been paid, it was difficult at first for them to under- 
stand that the forty-minute history of an inexperienced teen-ager is as 
important as the two- or three-hour history of an older person who has 
been involved in every conceivable sort of sexual activity. 

3. Experience indicates that the first volunteers from any group are 
likely to be the extrovert, aggressive, sexually less inhibited, and often 
more active individuals ; but if a group is worked with over a longer period 
of time the sample becomes more diversified. For that reason, we have, in 
general, avoided working with groups where only a single appeal could be 
made, or where the time for taking histories was limited to a few days or 
even a week or two. Also for that reason, we have concentrated on securing 
samples from a more limited number of cities and towns, and from partic- 
ular groups to whom we might return over periods of months and even 
years. Some of these groups have been contributing throughout the eight 
or nine years of the research. In such places, some persons contribute even 
after two or three years of refusing — ^finally convinced by the reaction of 
the community that their socially irregular or utterly blank histories can 
be reported without embarrassment, and that the project is, after all, 
worth while. The partial sample employed in this study would never have 
been as representative as it is if we had not had such long-time contacts 
with most of the groups. 

It is unfortunate that we do not yet have large enough populations to 
measure the differences between first samples and subsequent samples 
from the same community. It is possible, however, to report measurements 
on one college group where Maslow’s dominance and security ratings were 
available on some of the females who contributed histories to the present 
study of sex behavior (see Maslow 1940, 1942a, 1942b; Maslow, 
Hirsh, Stein, and Honigmann 1945, for a detailed description of the tests). 
The first volunteers seemed to be more extrovert and assured individuals 
(though how that affects a sexual history is not yet clear). By staying nearly 
a month in the community, a sample was obtained from about 400 students, 
on 92 of whom dominance and/or security scores were available for com- 



104 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


parison with about 80 students who were in the same psychology classes 
but who failed to volunteer for histories. The volunteer group showed the 
full range of variation in dominance and security ratings, from the most 
aggressive to the most timid levels. The mean dominance rating for the 
group that had volunteered for histories was about 10 per cent higher than 
for those who had not volunteered; the mean security score was about 3 
per cent lower. We are indebted to Dr. A. H. Maslow for the data which 
allow this analysis. 

4. Considerable attention must be given to securing an appreciable 
portion of each group from which histories are taken, even when it is not 
possible to secure a hundred percent sample. In many instances half to 
three-quarters or more of each group has been secured. We have an impres- 
sion (but as yet insufficient data to test it) that such a sample is not so 
different from a complete sample. There is one statistical study (Shuttle- 
worth 1941) that suggests that a sixty per cent sample is still insufficient to 
represent the whole. 

Whenever, as in the present survey, it is not feasible to secure a strictly 
randomized sample, a combination of hundred percent sampling and con- 
trolled partial sampling seems the best that can be done. To attempt to 
base the entire study on hundred percent sampling would not be satis- 
factory, for it would be impossible to secure such complete samples in 
sufficient number from all of the diverse groups in a population. Suffi- 
ciently controlled partial samples seem •to have considerable value, espe- 
cially when they are offset by an even greater proportion of hundred 
percent samples than we have, as yet, utilized. 

ORDER OF SAMPLING 

The present study has been very much speeded up while the cost has been 
kept at a phenomenally low minimum — actually between 2 per cent and 
4 per cent of the cost per history of the previous personal interview studies 
in this field. This has been primarily because of a policy of accepting what- 
ever histories were immediately available, rather than going after particu- 
lar sorts of histories in particular sequence. After securing the histories, 
they have been placed in the classificatory cells to which they belong. The 
value of such a policy was learned through our experience with insect 
sampling. The customary procedure of searching for particular persons to 
represent particular segments of the population is expensive because of 
the work involved in locating those particular cases. If one is satisfied to 
accept material in the order in which it appears, one sooner or later finds 
the particular cases which are necessary for the completion of a study. 
While we have always endeavored to secure some degree of diversity in our 
sample, we have not failed to seize the opportunity to take histories from 
the immediately available groups, until enough histories had been secured 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


105 


to satisfy the demands in those groups. At the present writing there are 
only two cells from which we have enough histories, and it is now a matter 
of avoiding cases that belong to those particular groups. In the course of 
time one has to go further out of his way to secure histories from certain 
other groups, and that will increase the cost; but the cost can always be 
kept relatively low if one bides his time and takes the material that is most 
available. 

SYNTHESIZING A U. S. SAMPLE 

While, as just indicated, data on each of the ultimate groups in the popu- 
lation are the first objectives of the present study, it has been desirable at 
certain points to calculate statistics which would be applicable to some 
larger group, as, for instance, all single white males in the U. S. population, 
or all married white males, or all white males of all sorts in the total 
American population. This has been accomplished by weighting the raw 
data from each of the ultimate groups in the study, in proportion to the 
size of that group in the U. S. Census, and totalling the weighted results for 
all the groups. The Census of 1940 shows the distribution of the total 
population by all the items which are involved in the six-way breakdown 
employed in the present volume: sex, race, marital status, age, number of 
years of schooling (without a clear distinction between current and com- 
pleted educational histories), and the rural-urban background (on a slightly 
different basis than the one employed in the present study). At a few points 
where the Census breakdowns do not exactly match our own (e.g., in their 
failure to indicate what proportion of the population is pre-adolescent, and 
in regard to the educational record as noted above), it has been possible 
to make estimates which cannot have introduced an error of more than a 
fraction of one per cent into the calculations. Tables 7 to 1 1 show the con- 
stants thus derived from the 1940 Census figures. They are the bases of the 
calculations which appear throughout the present volume as “U. S. Cor- 
rections” of the raw data. To make any correction from these tables, each 
item in the raw data should be multiplied by the figure shown at the appro- 
priate point in the table. The products of all the items in any particular age 
group are then totalled and divided by the “age weight” figure (the second 
column in each table). 

An examination of the tables and charts throughout this volume will 
show how far apart raw data and “U. S. Corrections” may be. Since the 
smaller groups in stratified sampling should be represented by samples of 
the same size as those used for the larger groups, they unduly affect the 
calculations made for a total population. Therefore, in the case of phenom- 
ena which occur most frequently in groups which constitute only a small 
proportion of the population (e.g., masturbation, nocturnal dreams, and 
petting in the college population), the raw data for the total population 
give higher averages than the U. S. corrected data (e.g., Figures 38-42, 
53-57, 59-63). Conversely, in the case of phenomena which are more 



106 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Table for U. S. Correction: Males, White 


AGE 

GROUP 

EDUCATIONAL 

LEVEL 

0-8 

9-12 

13 + 

No 

Educ 

Rpt. 

MARITAL 

STATUS 

S 

M 

W&D 

S 

M 

W&D 

S 

M 

W&D 


URBAN AND RURAL NON-FARM 


Age Weight 











Adol.-14 

(4.59) 

(1.03) 



(1.83) 



(0.48) 




15-19 

12.33 

2.46 

0.03 

0.00 

6.01 

0.08 

0.00 

0.35 

0.00 

0.00 

0.06 

20-24 

11.43 

1.58 

0.56 

0.01 

3.79 

1.36 

0.02 

1.07 

0.38 

0.01 

0.07 

25-29 

10.93 

1.05 

1.82 

0.03 

1.58 

2.74 

0.05 

0.54 

0.95 

0,02 

0.08 

30-34 

10.24 

0.69 

2.64 

0.06 

0.70 

2.74 

0.07 

0.28 

1.10 

0.04 

0.09 

35-39 

09.51 

0.57 

3.21 

0.12 

0.39 

2.18 

0.08 

0.17 

0.95 

0.03 

0.10 

40-44 

08.92 

0.50 

3.39 

0.17 

0.27 

1.81 

0.08 

0.12 

0.79 

0.03 

0.11 

45-49 

08.57 

0.48 

3.58 

0.21 

0.19 

1.45 

0.08 

0.09 

0.66 

0.03 

0.12 

50-54 

07.72 

0.46 

3.32 

0.28 

0.14 

1.07 

0.10 

0.07 

0.51 

0.04 

0.11 

55-59 

06.23 

0.36 

2.65 

0.31 

0.10 

0.76 

0.09 

0.05 

0.36 

0.04 

0.09 

60 + 

14.12 

0.82 

5.40 

1.74 

0.16 

1.08 

0.35 

0.09 

0.58 

0.19 

0.26 

Total 

100.00 

j 8.97 

j26.60 

2.93 

13.33 

o 

15.27 

0.92 

2.83 

6.28 

0.43 

1.09 


FARM 


Adol.-14 

15-19 

(4.59) 

12.33 

(0.38) 

1.68 

0.03 

0.00 

(0.69), 

1.53 

0.03 

0.00 

(0.18) 

0.05 

0.00 

0.00 

0.02 

20-24 

11.43 

0.98 

0.34' 

0.01 

0.79 1 

0.27 

0.01 

0.12 

0.04 

0.00 

0.02 

25-29 

10.93 

0.48 

0.73 

0.02 

0.29 

0.43 

0.01 

0.04 

0.06 

0.00 

0 01 

30-34 

10.24 

0.28 

0.89 

0.02 

0.13 

0.39 

0 01 

0.02 

0.07 

0.00 

0.02 

35-39 

' 09.51 

0.21 

1.01 

0.03 

0.06 

0.30 

0.01 

0.01 

0.06 

0.00 

0.02 

40-44 

08.92 

0.17 

1.04 

0.03 

0.04 

0.27 

0.01 

0.01 

0.06 

0.00 

0.02 

45-49 

08.57 

0.15 

1.12 

0.05 

0.03 

0.23 

0.01 

0.01 

0.06 

0.00 

0.02 

50-54 

07.72 

0.15 

1.09 

0.07 

0.02 

0.19 

0.01 1 

0.01 

0.06 

0.00 

0.02 

55-59 

06.23 

0.13 

0.96 

0.09 

0.02 

0.14, 

0.01 

0.01 

0.04 

0.00 

0.02 

60+ 

14.12 

0.30 

2.07 

0.58 

0.03 

0.22! 

0.06 

0.01 

0.09 

0.02 

0.07 

Total 

100.00 

4.53 

9.28 

0,90 

2.94 

2.47 

0.14 

0.29 

0.54 

0.02 

0.24 


Table 7. Six-way breakdown, U. S. Census, 1940 

Weights to be used for correcting raw data on populations resulting from a 6-way successive 
breakdown on males where race, rural-urban background, educational level, marital 
STATUS, and age are known. Classification based on 44,743,534 white males aged 15 and over. 
Estimated number of single white adolescent males through 14 years of age == 2,052,793. 
These are not included in totals because the data are not segregated in the U. S. Census ; but 
estimates are shown in parentheses on the first line of figures in the table. 



Tables for U. S. Correction: Males, White 


age 

GROUP 

educa- 

tional 

LEVEL 


0—8 



9-12 

j 


13 + 


MARITAL 
STATUS ! 

! 

M 

W&D 

S 

M 

1 

W&D 

S 

M 

W&D 

Adol.-14 

Age 

Weight 

(4.59) 

! 

(1.41) 

0,00 

0.00 

(2.52) 

0.00 

0.00 

(0.66) 

0.00 

0.00 

15-19 

12.25 

3.70 

0.06 

0.00 

6.64 

0.10 

0.00 

i 1.75 

0.00 

0.00 

20-24 

11.34 

2.56 

0.90 

0.02 

4.58 

1.63 

0.03 

1.19 

0.42 

0.01 

25-29 

10.84 

1.53 

2.55 

1 0.05 

1.87 

3.17 

0.06 

0.58 

1.01 

0.02 

30-34 

10.13 

0.97 

3.53 

0.08 

0.83 

3.13 

0.08 

0.30 

1.17 

0.04 

35-39 

9.39 

0.78 

4.22 

0.15 

0.45 

2.48 

0.09 

0.18 

1.01 

0.03 

40-44 

8.79 

0.67 

4.43 

0.20 

0.31 

2.08 

0.09 

0.13 

0.85 

0.03 

45-49 

8.43 

0.63 

4.70 

0,26 

0.22 

1.68 

0.09 

0.10 

0.72 

0.03 

50-54 

7.59 

0.61 

4.41 

0.35 

0.16 

1.26 

0.11 

0.08 

0.57 

0.04 

55-59 

6.13 

0.49 

3,61 

0.40 

0.12 

0.91 

0.10 

0.06 

0.40 

0.04 

60-1- 

13.79 

1.12 

7 47 

1 2.32 

0.19 

1.30 

0.41 

0.10 

0.67 

0.21 

Total 

98,68 

13.06 

35.88 

3.83 

15.37 

17.74 

1.06 

4.47 

6.82 

0.45 


Table 8. Five-way breakdown, U. S. Census, 1940 


Weights to be used for correcting raw data on populations resulting from a 5-way successive 
breakdown on males where race, educational level, marital status, and age are the items 
involved in the analyses. For males who are under 19 years of age and still in grade or high 
school, estimates have been made of the educational levels which they will ultimately attain. 
Persons who did not report their education in the Census are eliminated from this calculation. 
C/. legend on Table 7. 



RURAL-URBAN 

• 

Urban and Rural Non-Farm 


Farm 


AGE 

GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

0-8 

9-12 

13+ 

No 

Educ. 

Rpt. 

0-8 

9-12 

13+ 

I 

No 

Educ. 

Rpt. 

15-19 

Age Weight 
12.33 

2.49 

6.09 

0,35 

0.06 

... 

1.56 

0.05 

0.02 

20-24 

11.43 

2.15 

5.17 

1.46 

0.07 

1.33 

1.07 

0.16 

0.02 

25-29 

10.93 

2.90 

4.37 

1.51 

0.08 

1.23 

0.73 

0.10 

0.01 

30-34 

10.22 

3.39 

3.51 

1.42 

0.09 

1.19 

0.53 

0.09 

0.02 

35-39 

9.51 

3.90 

2.65 

1.15 

0.10 

1.25 

0.37 

0.07 

0.02 

40-44 

8.93 

4.06 

2.16 

0.94 

0.11 

1.24 

0.32 

0.07 

0.02 

45-49 

8.59 

4.27 

1.72 

0.78 

0.12 

1.32 

0.27 

0.07 

0.02 

50-54 

7.71 

4.06 

1.31 

0.62 

0.11 

1.31 

0.22 i 

0.07 

0.02 

55-59 

6.24 

3.32 

0.95 

0.45 

0.09 

1.18 

0.17 

0.05 

0.02 

60+ 

14.11 

7.96 

1.59 

0.86 

0.26 

2.95 

0.31 

0.12 

0.07 

Total 

100.00 

38.50 

29.52 

9.54 

1.09 

14.71 

5.55 

0.85 

0.24 


Table 9. Five-way breakdown, U. S. Census, 1940 


Weights to be used for correcting raw data on populations resulting from a 5-way 
successive breakdown on males where race, rural-urban background, educational 
LEVEL, and AGE are the items involved in the analyses. 

107 



108 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Tables for U. S. Correction: Males, White 



rural-urban 

! 

Urban and Rural Non-Farm 


Farm 


age 








group 

marital 

status 

S 

M 

WAD 

S 

M 

W&D 

15-19 

Age Weight 
12.32 

8.86 

0.13 

0.00 

3.27 

0.06 

0.00 

20-24 

11.44 

6.49 

2.32 

0.04 

1,92 

0.66 

0.01 

25-29 

10.93 

3.19 

5.55 

0.11 

0.82 

1.24 

0.02 

30-34 

10.22 

1.68 

6.55 

0.17 

0.43 

1.36 

0.03 

35-39 

9.52 

1.15 

6.42 

0.24 

0.29 

1,38 

0.04 

40-44 

8.94 

0.90 

6.09 

0.30 

0.22 

1.38 

0.05 

45-49 

8.58 

0.77 

5.77 

0.35 

0.19 

1.43 

0.07 

50-54 

7.73 

0.69 

4.99 

0.43 

0.18 

1.35 

0.09 

55-59 

6.23 

0.52 

3.84 

0.44 

0.16 

1.16 

0.11 

60H- 

14.11 

1.10 

7.24 

2,33 

i 0.35 

2.41 

0.68 

Total 

100.02 

25,35 

48.90 

4.41 

7.83 

12.43 

1.10 


Table 10. Five-way breakdown, U. S. Census, 1940 

Weights to be used for correcting raw data on populations resulting from a 5-vvay 
successive breakdown on males where race, age, the rural-urban background, and 
MARITAL status are the items involved in the analyses. Cf. legend on Table 7. 


AGE 

group 

AGE 

WEIGHT 

rural-urban 

educational level 

marital status 

Non- 

farm 

Farm 

0-8 

9-12 

13+ 

No 

Rpt. 

s 

M 

W&D 

15-19 

12.33 

8.99 

3.33 

4.20 

7.70 

0.40 

0.1 

12.09 

0.16 

0.00 

20-24 

11.43 

8.85 

2.59 

3.48 

6.24 

1.62 

0.1 

8.33 

2.95 

0.06 

25-29 

10.93 

8.85 

2.08 

4.13 

5.10 

1.61 

0.1 

3.98 

6.73 

0.13 

30-34 

10.22 

8.38 

1.81 

4.58 

4.04 

1.51 

0.1 

2.10 

7.83 

0.20 

35-39 

9.51 

7.81 

1.71 

5.15 

3.02 

1.22 

0.1 

1.41 

7.71 

0.27 

40-44 

8.93 

7.29 

1.65 

5.30 

2.48 

1.01 

0.1 

1.11 

7.36 

0.32 

45-49 

8.59 

6.89 

1.69 

5.59 

1.99 

0.85 

0.1 

0.95 

7.10 

0.38 

50-54 

7.71 

6.11 

1.62 

5,37 

1.53 

0.69 

0.1 

0.85 

6.24 

0.50 

55-59 

6.24 

4.80 

1.43 

4.50 

1.13 

0.50 

0.1 

0.67 

4.12 

0.54 

60-1- 

14.11 

10.67 

3.44 

10.91 

1.90 

0.98 

0.3 

1.41 

9.44 

2.94 

Total 

100.00 

78.65 

22.35 

53.21 

35.13 

10.39 

1.2 

32.90 

60.44 

5.34 


Table 11. Four-way breakdown, U. S. Census, 1940 


Weights to be used for correcting data on populations resulting from a 4-way suc- 
cessive breakdown on males where race, age, and either the rural-urban background, 
or the educational level, or the marital status are the only items involved in the 
analyses. 





STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


109 


common in groups that constitute a larger segment of the population 
(e.g., pre-marital intercourse and the homosexual in a population that 
has gone into high school but not beyond) the raw data are distinctly lower 
than the U S. Corrections (e.g.. Figures 71-75, 77-81, 83-87). I he public 
opinion polls and most of the government surveys are aware of this prob- 
lem, but it is most unfortunate that students in psychology and the social 
sciences regularly publish raw data without corrections for the Census 
distributions of their populations. As these figures and many others will 
show, the raw data are sometimes as much as 34 per cent removed from the 
corrected data, and the general shape of the curve may be considerably 
changed by the corrections. Throughout the present volume, the figures 
given in the body of the text and the heavier lines shown in all the charts 
represent U. S. Corrections of the raw data, except in those relatively few 
instances where corrections have been impossible because of insufficient 
information in the Census. 

STATISTICAL ANALYSES 

All mathematical calculations on this project have been performed 
twice, independently by each of two persons. Computations have been set 
up on standard ruled forms, and these are all filed for consultation by any 
qualified student who needs to check the method or accuracy of the calcu- 
lations. 

The statistical manipulation the data in this study has been kept at 
an absolute minimum. The incidence data (the record of the number of 
persons involved in the various sexual activities) are subject to error be- 
cause of deliberate or unconscious cover-up, especially in regard to socially 
taboo items. The frequency data (the number of times the activities are 
engaged in) cannot be more than approximations to the actual fact, 
because sexual activities are more often irregular in their distribution, with 
days or weeks of high frequency alternating with days and weeks of low 
frequency, and only the persons accustomed to the handling of averages 
(as few people are) can estimate their mean frequencies in more than very 
approximate terms. Individuals who have kept diaries or calendars may 
have more accurate bases for their estimates; but few people have as yet 
turned in such records (see p. 74). For these reasons, the calculations in 
the present study are likely to involve greater errors than if it were a 
study of some other kind of phenomenon. In large series of cases, errors 
which are overestimates are sometimes compensated for by errors which 
are understatements, provided there is no bias which accumulates the 
errors primarily in one direction; but even then there can be no great 
precision to the calculations. 

In consideration of the approximate nature of the original data, it 
would then be misleading to subject them to more than relatively simple 



110 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


mathematical treatment. For that reason, only the following statistical 
operations have been performed on each history and on each series of 
histories. 

Individual Frequencies. Average frequencies of orgasm have been calcu- 
lated on each history for each type of sexual activity, namely, masturbation, 
nocturnal dreams, heterosexual petting, heterosexual coitus, homosexual 
contacts, and contacts with animals of other species. Heterosexual rela- 
tions have been calculated as pre-marital coitus with prostitutes, pre-mari- 
tal coitus with females who are not prostitutes, marital coitus, extra-marital 
coitus with prostitutes, extra-marital coitus with other females, post-marital 
coitus with prostitutes, or post-marital coitus with other females. For the 
purposes of the present volume, only sexual activities which have led to 
orgasm have been included in these frequency calculations, although there 
are many other aspects of human sexual behavior which will also be con- 
sidered in this and in later volumes. Throughout this volume all frequency 
figures have been calculated for each individual as average frequencies per 
week. In some of the previously published studies, such activities have been 
recorded as rates per month; but except for low frequencies, few persons 
are capable of estimating average rates for such a period of time. The 
social organization imposes a weekly periodicity on various human activi- 
ties, including the sexual (Ellis 1901 (1936): 85 ff.), and weekly rates are 
consequently better known to most persons. 

In summarizing the record on individuals and on groups, frequencies 
have been standardized as average frequencies per week extending over 
five-year periods involving ages 11-15 (inclusive), 16-20, 21-25, 26-30, 
etc. In these periods, weeks or years which were without sexual outlet have 
been averaged with the active periods, and in that way seasons of inactivity 
have lowered the weekly rates for the whole of a particular five-year 
period. Since the calculations apply only to the activities which occur after 
the onset of adolescence, the first age period really extends from adoles- 
cence to 15, and is usually something less than a five-year period. In the 
latter case, the averages shown are based on the active years, and are not 
reduced by being averaged with the pre-adolescent years. The last age 
period — the period in which the subject contributes his history — is treated 
in the same fashion, if it is less than a full five-year period. 

For each outlet, average frequencies per week, per five-year period, have 
been calculated precisely to the first decimal place. Group averages have 
consequently been calculated to the second decimal place. Because of the 
approximate nature of the raw data, finer calculations have not seemed 
warranted. 

Group Frequencies. The nature of any population has been found by 
classifying all of the individuals in it into frequency classes which have 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 1 1 1 


been named for their upper limits. The ranges of each class and the class 
means used for calculations have been as follows : 


1 

Class 

Range 

Mean Value 

0 

0 

0 

— 

0.01- 0.09 

0.05 

0.5 

0.1 - 0.5 

0.3 

1.0 

0.6 - 1.0 

0.8 

1.5 

1.1 - 1.5 

1.3 

2.0 

1.6 - 2.0 

1.8 

2.5 

2.1 - 2.5 

2.3 

etc. 



10.0 

9.6 -10.0 

9.8 

11.0 

10.1 -11.0 

10.5 

12.0 

11.1 -12.0 

11.5 

etc. 



28.0 

27.1 -28.0 

27.5 

29.0+ 

28 . 1 and higher 

28.5 


Frequency Curves. The number of individuals which fall into each of 
these frequency classes has been translated into percents of the whole 
population involved. Frequency curves throughout this volume have been 
based on such percents, rather than on the absolute number of cases in 
each frequency class. Many of the curves shown in psychologic and socio- 
logic literature are uninterpretable because they are based on the absolute 
number, instead of upon the percentages of cases involved. All of the 
frequency curves in this volume are based on the actual calculations, and in 
no instance have they been smoothed by any process or approximated by 
interpolations or other sorts of estimates or predictions. 

Group Averages. These have been calculated for each type of sexual 
outlet for the 5-year periods described above, for each population which 
has had 50 or more cases in it after 4-, 5-, or 6-way breakdowns of the 
total sample. All tabulations of data by groups, and all correlations, have 
been made by putting the data onto standard punch cards (Hollerith, IBM 
system), and all manipulations of cards have been performed on IBM 
machines. Both the punching of the cards and the handhng of the machines 
on this project have been done by members of the research staff, in order 
that there be no betrayal of the confidence of the record. Each series of 
punch cards has carried a particular portion of each history, e.g., the 
frequencies and sources of outlet on one set, the record of the pre-adoles- 
cent material on another set, the accumulative incidence data on another, 
etc. Thirteen sets of cards (z.e., thirteen or more cards for each of the 
histories) have been punched for the calculation of the data presented in 
the present volume. Each of the thirteen cards in each set has carried the 
identical record of the age, educational level, occupational class, and other 
social backgrounds of the subject, mechanically reproduced on the 

5 



112 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


thirteen cards to insure identity. Thus it has been possible to correlate all 
of the data on the thirteen sets with the same educational and social items. 
By a gang punch technique, it is possible to correlate the material on one 
card with the material on each other card. 

Means. The averages which have been calculated have included mean 
frequencies for the population in each group, and means for the “active 
populations” in each group (Le,, for those individuals who had any activity 
in that five-year period, in that particular type of sexual outlet). Means 
have been calculated by the formula: 



n 


For those who are not familiar with statistical practice, it may be 
pointed out that a mean represents the total number of measurements (in 
the present instance, the total number of orgasms) in each group divided 
by the number of individuals in the group. The mean represents the mid- 
point of the measurements. Its position (in contrast to the position of the 
median, which is described below) is therefore materially affected by the 
presence of even a few high-rating individuals in a population; and al- 
though the arithmetic mean is the average which is most commonly em- 
ployed, both by most people in their everyday affairs and by the trained 
statistician, it may give a distorted picture because a few high-rating individ- 
uals affect the means more than a large population of low-rating in- 
dividuals. Since nearly all of the distrfoution curves on human sex be- 
havior are strongly skewed to the right (to the high frequency end of the 
curve), the means are quite regularly higher than the location of the body 
of the population would lead one to expect. Conversely, inactive cases in a 
population (i.e., in the 0 class of frequencies) have a minimum effect on 
the position of the mean. 

Standard Deviation of the Mean. This is also known as the standard 
error of the mean, and as the sigma of the mean. It is represented by the 
symbol (Tm. The standard deviation of each mean has been calculated in 
every instance, using the formula: 



This formula is generally considered precise, and has the advantage of 
being calculable with maximum efficiency on a calculating machine. For 
the general reader, it may be pointed out that the standard deviation is 
attached to each mean shown in this volume, as follows : 

2.36 = 1 = 0.04 
Mean Om 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


113 


The is supposed to indicate the size of the error which may be involved 
in the mean — the limits, plus or minus, within which the true mean (as 
distingushed from the calculated mean) stands a 2 to 1 chance of lying. 
The size of in relation to the size of the mean indicates the degree of 
reliability of the calculated mean, and the smaller the the less the 
probable error. 

Medians. Median frequencies have been calculated, in every group, for 
the total sample population, and for the active population. Medians have 
been calculated by the formula: 


If all the individuals in a group are arranged linearly in accordance with 
the average frequencies of orgasm, the median designates the frequency of 
the individual who stands exactly midway in that series. Half of the 
individuals in the population have less frequent orgasm, half the individ- 
uals have more frequent orgasm. While the median is an average which is 
less often calculated by people in their everyday affairs, and while it is a 
statistic which has often been neglected by statisticians, it answers the 
very common question: “How frequently does the average individual 
engage in such activity?” and it provides, therefore, a most useful type of 
information. Recently statisticians have paid more attention to its signifi- 
cance. The location of a median is determined solely by the sequence of the 
individuals in a population, and It is unaffected by the low or high rates 
of particular individuals. Means and medians are averages which sum- 
marize two very different ideas, and in consequence their relative impor- 
tance cannot properly be discussed. Means measure average frequencies, 
medians describe the average individuals. 

Persons not familiar with these matters should understand that where 
most of the individuals in a sample belong in a frequency class which is 
midway between the extremes of the distribution, and where an equal 
number of individuals lie in symmetrical distribution on either side of the 
mid-point, the mean becomes identical with the median. Where the curve 
is asymmetric, the median becomes removed from the mean, sometimes 
by a very considerable distance. The median is lower than the mean when 
there are high-rating individuals who stand apart from the mass of the 
population; and this is almost always true as regards nearly all types of 
human sexual activity. The distance between the median and the mean is a 
measure of the extent to which the frequency distribution for the popula- 
tion (the frequency curve) is skewed in the direction of higher activity 
(extends to the right of the area which includes the body of the population). 
When a large portion of a population falls into the zero class (is without 
activity) in any particular calculation, the median for that population is 
so lowered that it loses significance. If more than 50 per cent of the popula- 



114 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


tioB falls into the zero class, the mean is in the zero class and is useless for 
any understanding of the situation. In the same instance, however, a 
median calculated on the active portion of the population may have signifi- 
cance. 

Percents of Individual Outlet. On each history, calculations have been 
made showing (in percents) the portion of his total sexual outlet which 
the individual has derived from each possible source (masturbation, 
dreams, coitus, etc.). The calculations have been made for the same five- 
year periods as were involved in the calculations of frequencies of total 
outlet. 

Percents of Group Outlet. Similarly, frequency distributions have been 
plotted for these percents of outlets; and means, standard errors of the 
means, and medians have been routinely calculated on these percents for 
the total population and for the active portion of each population. When 
means are calculated in the usual way, the figures are the averages of all 
these percentages. When medians are calculated in this way, they show the 
percentage of the total outlet which the average individual derives from 
each of the possible sources. Neither of these calculations, however, 
answers the more usual question: “What percentage of the total orgasms of 
the population as a whole is derived from each kind of sexual activity?” 
In order to answer that question, it is necessary to compare the means of 
the absolute frequencies (not the percentage frequencies) for each type of 
out et in each group, with the mean of the absolute frequency of total 
outlet in the same group. The sum of the percentages so derived should 
total 100 per cent, which is the total outlet for the population. 

Correlation Coefficients. At special points in the investigation, correla- 
tion coefiicients and still other statistics have been calculated by standard 
procedures. Unless otherwise indicated the correlation coefficients repre- 
sent the Pearsonian r, calculated by the formula : 

(n Zxy) — (zfxsfy) 

V(n*sfx^)^((sf^) ((nzfy2) ~ (sfy)") 

In correlating data for which only two classes are possible, as with a yes 
or no situation, or with a record of presence or absence, the calculated 
coefficients represent the tetrachoric r derived from the tables published 
by Cheshire, Saffir, and Thurstone (1933). 

Accumulative Incidence Curves, The one new statistical tool which we 
have had to develop for this study has been a curve which will show the 
number of persons who have ever had sexual experience of a particular 
sort up to any particular age of their lives. One of the questions most 
commonly asked is: “How many people do this — or that?” Specifically, 
it is “How many people masturbate?” — “How many people have homo- 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


115 


sexual experience?” — “What percentage of college students (or some other 
group) have intercourse before they marry?” — etc., etc. The question does 
not concern the number of persons having experience in any particular 
year (which is the active incidence figure), as often as it involves a question 
about the number of persons who ever have such experience in their lives, 
or in some portion of it. The answers usually given in both popular and 
technical literature are often incorrect because they are derived from curves 
based on cumulations of percentages. Such curves show the percentage 
increase of experienced persons (the increments) in each successive age 
group, the increments being totalled up to the end of the period of time 
under consideration. The cumulated percentages shown in Tables 28, 33, 
35-37, and in Figures 15, 26, 27, 29, covering data on the ages involved in 
adolescent developments among boys, are examples of such calculations. 
Such curves are known as integral curves or ogives (the two “are funda- 
mentally the same,” according to Pearl 1940:143), and these are the curves 
that are ordinarily used in growth studies, learning studies, studies of 
social developments, etc. 

But ogives are satisfactory only when the activity under consideration 
has involved a hundred percent of the population which is being studied, 
or when the histories of all the individuals in the study are concluded as 
far as that particular chapter in their lives is concerned. Cumulative per- 
centage figures are quite sufficient in the cases cited above because all of 
the individuals on which they afe based were adolescent when the data 
were gathered, z.^., a hundred percent of the population was ultimately 
involved, and all of the individuals had the experience (onset of adoles- 
cence) which was being studied. An ogive would be correctly used if the 
ages of first pre-marital intercourse were being studied, and the curve were 
based on persons all of whom were married. In an ogive, the size of the 
basic population is constant for each and every age group, since all of the 
persons are either experienced or past the age at which they could possibly 
begin experience, and the total sample in an ogive is the basis for calculating 
the percentage of experienced individuals at each particular age. 

Ogives, however, do not answer the question when only a portion of a 
population is eligible for experience, or when the histories of any of the 
individuals are not complete at the time the data are gathered. For instance, 
if the question is one of determining how many people have extra-marital 
intercourse, the real issue concerns the number of married people who ever 
will have such experience before they die. This could be determined by the 
use of an ogive if all persons in the study had been married, and if all the 
histories were taken after each person had terminated his marriage by 
separation or divorce, or after he had died. But since that is not easily 
effected, a technique must be used which will show the number of experi- 
enced persons in each age group, in relation to the number of persons in 



116 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Accumulative Incidence Data: Coitus with Prostitutes 
White Males, of College Level 



*1 

2 

*3 

4 

5 

*6 

7 

8 

9 

10 

Age 

Age, 

first 

exper. 

S of 1 

Age at 
report, 
exper. 
individ. 

S of 3 
Ages 
not 

reached 

2-4 

Freq. 

Age at 
report, 
inexper. 
individ. 

3 4- 6 
Total 
popu- 
lation 

Sof7, 

up. 

Years 

in- 

volved 

1 5/8 
Curve 
% 

Incre- 

ment 

13 

3 

3 



3 



2816 

0.1 


14 

19 

22 



22 



2816 

0.8 

-fO.7 

15 

42 

64 



64 

1 

1 

2816 

2.3 

4-1.5 

16 

72 

136 



136 

2 

2 

2815 

4.8 

4-2.5 

17 

119 

255 

8 


255 

68 

76 

2813 

9.1 

4-4.3 

18 

125 

380 

25 

8 

372 

139 

164 

2737 

13.6 

4-4.5 

19 

100 

480 

44 

33 

447 

191 

235 

2573 

17.4 

4-3.8 

20 

78 

558 

70 

77 

481 

236 

306 

2338 

20.6 

4-3.2 

21 

41 

599 

74 1 

147 

452 

286 

360 

2032 

22.2 

4-1.6 

22 

38 

637 

78 

221 

416 

197 

275 

1672 

24.9 

4-2.7 

23 

21 

658 

64 

299 

359 

181 

245 

1397 

25.7 

4-0.8 

24 

12 

670 

31 

363 

307 

119 

150 

1152 

26.6 

+0.9 

25 

11 

681 

36 

394 

287 

82 

118 

1002 

28.6 

4-2.0 

26 

9 

690 

29 

430 

260 

81 

no 

884 

29.4 

4-0.8 

27 

5 

695 

18 

459 

236 

57 

75 

774 

30.5 

4-1.1 

28 

6 

701 

23 

477 

224 

42 

65 

699 

32.0 

4-1.5 

29 

2 

703 

17 

500 

203 

44 

61 

634 

32.0 I 

4-0.0 

30 

4 

707 

10 

517 

190 

' 34 

44 

573 

33.2 

4-1.2 

31 


707 

16 

527 

180 

21 

37 

529 

34.0 

4-0.8 

32 

2 

709 

18 , 

543 

166 

26 

44 

492 

33.7 

-0.3 

33 

1 

710 

11 1 

561 

149 

25 

36 

448 

33.3 

-0.4 

34 

1 

711 

12 1 

572 

139 

18 

30 

412 

33.7 

-hO.4 

35 

5 

716 

8 

584 

132 

18 

26 

382 1 

34.6 

4-0.9 

36 

3 

719 

14 

592 j 

127 

19 

33 

356 

35.7 

4-1.1 

37 

2 

721 

5 

606 

115 

11 

16 

323 

35.6 

-0.1 

38 

1 

722 

9 

611 

111 

18 

27 

307 

36.2 

4-0.6 

39 


722 

9 

620 

102 

14 

23 

280 

36.4 

4-0.2 

40 

1 

723 

6 

629 

94 

13 

19 

257 

36.6 

4-0.2 

41 


723 

10 

635 

88 

7 

17 

238 

37.0 

4-0.4 

42 


723 

12 

645 

78 

17 

29 

221 

35.3 

-1.7 

43 


723 

2 

657 

66 

13 

15 

192 

34.4 

-0.9 

44 


723 

2 

659 

64 

14 

16 

177 

36.2 

4-1.8 

45 


723 

3 

661 

62 

7 

10 

161 

38.5 

4-2.3 

46 


723 

3 

664 

59 

7 

10 

151 

39.1 

4-0.6 

47 


723 

11 

667 

56 

14 

25 

141 

39.7 

4-0.6 

48 


723 

3 

678 

45 

3 

6 

116 

38.8 

-0.9 

49 


723 

1 

681 

42 

12 

13 

msm 

38.2 

-0.6 

50-1- 

1 

724 

42 

682 

42 

55 

97 

97 

43.3 

4-5.1 

Total 

724 


724 

724 


2092 

2816 





Table 12. Form for calculation of an accumulative incidence curve 

Starred columns (*) are derived from punch cards; other columns are calculations based 
on the starred columns. The curve derived from this table is shown in Figure 14. 




STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


117 


Explanation of an Accumulative Incidence Curve 
Coitus with Prostitutes 

Age. 

L Age of first experience. 

2. Summation of Column 1. This is the ogive. It is based on the fictitious 
conception that the number of experienced persons in this population 
cannot be increased beyond the number now shown. 

3. Ages at reporting, of experienced individuals. 

4. Summation of Column 3, one step in advance. This represents the ages 
which the experienced individuals had not yet reached at the time they 
contributed their histories. 

5. Subtraction of Column 4 from Column 2. This represents the years 
actually lived by the experienced individuals. 

6. Ages at reporting, of inexperienced individuals. 

7. Addition of Columns 3 + 6. This is the age distribution of all subjects 
(both experienced and inexperienced) at time of reporting. 

8. Summation of Column 7, in reverse. This is the basic population for 
incidence calculations at each age. 

9. Division of Column 5 by Column 8. This is the percent of the popula- 
tion at each age with experience in that year, or in any previous year. 

10. Increment, calculated from Column 9. 

Note: Pre-adolescent experience was eliminated from this calculation 
by punching cards only for experience that had occurred after the onset 
of adolescence. 

The problem was restricted at the lower ages by adolescence. If the prob- 
lem had been restricted at the upper ages to a particular portion of the life 
span, e.g., to pre-marital years, Column 1 would have been corrected by 
sorting the experienced married individuals by age of marriage, and elimi- 
nating those whose first experience with prostitutes occurred after marriage. 
Column 3 would then have represented the sum of two groups of data : 
(1) the ages at marriage of the experienced population, and (2) the ages 
at reporting of the unmarried individuals who are experienced with prosti- 
tutes. Column 6 would then have represented the same sort of sum for the 
population which is not experienced with prostitutes. 



118 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


each group who are eligible for such experience. This is the technique of the 
accumulative incidence curves which we have used in the present study. 

Similarly, an accumulative incidence curve should be used when the 
ages of first pre-marital coital experience are to be determined for a popu- 
lation which contains some individuals who are not yet married. In such a 
problem, each point on the curve is based on a population which is inde- 
pendently calculated for each age. Each point is fixed by determining the 
number of persons in the sample who were not yet married, and by sub- 
tracting the persons who are no longer available for such experience 
because of marriage, or because the calculation has passed the ages at 
which those persons had contributed histories. 

In order to build an accumulative incidence curve, two or more sets of 
data are needed on each individual involved in the study: 

1. Age of first experience, for each subject, 

2. Age of each subject at time of reporting. 

3. In some cases, the age at which each subject became eligible for the 
sort of experience which is being studied the age of adolescence, for 
the study of post-adolescent experience; the age of marriage, for the study 
of experience as a married person; etc.). 

4. In some cases, the age at which each subject became ineligible for 
experience (e.g., the age at adolescence, as the end of the period at which 



(0 15 IQ 25 30 35 40 45 50 

AGE 

Figure 14. An accumulative incidence curve 

Showing percentages of college males who have ever had intercourse with prostitutes 
by each of the indicated ages. Based on data in Table 12. 



STATISTICAL PROBLEMS 


119 


The derivation of an accumulative incidence curve was first worked out 
for a small sample by a hand manipulation of 1058 actual history sheets, 
adding them to piles as each individual became eligible, withdrawing them 
as each individual became ineligible for experience. It took some time to 
devise a procedure for Hollerith machine manipulation of punch cards on 
the problem, but a remarkably simple set-up has now been arrived at. It 
is shown in Table 12 where a specific problem is worked out in full detail 
(Figure 14). Since this seems to be a statistical procedure which has not been 
published before, it has seemed desirable to describe it at some length. 

The usefulness of an accumulative incidence curve cannot be over- 
emphasized. It supplies the answer to the commonest of questions : “How 
many people have such experience?” From such a curve, one may at a 
glance determine the percentage of the population which has ever had 
experience by any given age. At the same time, the curve gives the best 
possible basis for predicting what percentage of any group will ever, in its 
lifetime, have such experience. This use of the curve for making predictions 
is one of its most significant values. As already indicated, it is of prime 
concern in any research that the conclusions be extensible to wider areas 
than those covered by the particular sample which has been investigated, 
and accumulative incidence curves are the most effective tools for so 
translating data. An accumulative incidence curve can be built on data 
from subjects whose histories are not yet complete, and thus it utilizes a 
large body of data which is not^available for building an ogive (which 
depends upon completed histories). An accumulative incidence curve is 
less accurate nearer its end, because the populations which establish the 
successive points on the curve become smaller in these upper age levels. 
However, the area in which the curve becomes unreliable is well enough 
indicated by the wider scatter of the individual points, which is in sharp 
contrast to the smooth trends in the more reliable portions of the curve. 

In conclusion, it should be emphasized that, after all of this statistical 
manipulation, the calculations given in the present volume still should be 
taken as approximations which are not to be pushed in detail, although 
they undoubtedly show the general locus of the incidence and frequency 
figures, with plus or minus errors of some few percent. In the next chapter 
data will be given to show the size of the corrections that may need to be 
made. 



Chapter 4 


VALIDITY OF THE DATA 

Throughout research of the sort involved here, one needs to be continu- 
ously conscious, as already pointed out, that it is impossible to get more 
than approximations of the fact on the incidences and frequencies of 
various types of human sexual behavior. Memory cannot be wholly 
accepted as a source of information on what has actually happened in an 
individual’s history. There is both deliberate and unconscious cover-up, 
especially of the more taboo items, and in deahng with people of diverse 
mental levels and educational backgrounds, there are differences in their 
ability to comprehend and to answer questions with any precision in an 
interview. 

Moreover, it is difficult for a person who has not kept a diary, and espe- 
cially for one who is not accustomed to thinking in statistical terms, to 
know how to average events which occur as irregularly as sexual activities 
usually do. The mass of the population is not often called upon to estimate 
the frequencies with which they engage in any sort of activity, sexual or 
otherwise. This is most obvious in dealing with poorly educated persons, 
and with mentally low grade individuals. Most persons are inclined to re- 
member frequencies for periods when the activities were regular, and to for- 
get those periods in which there was material interference with activity. In 
marital intercourse, for instance, there are menstrual periods, periods of 
illness, periods of travel when spouses are apart, periods of preoccupation 
with special duties which, affecting either of the two partners, interfere 
with the regularity of intercourse for both of them. While other sources of 
outlet may fill in some of these gaps, there are situations in which no kind 
of sexual outlet is readily available; but these blank periods are not 
always taken into account by a subject who is estimating averages for a 
history. 

It has, therefore, been important to secure some measure of the size of 
the error for which allowance must be made on the calculations in the 
present volume. A number of techniques have been used for these tests, 
and a considerable body of information is now available on the validity 
of the data. We shall want to continue with these tests as the study ex- 
pands. 

In Chapter 2 it was pointed out that the techniques of the interviewing 
have provided a considerable control against exaggeration, but that there 
is a greater Ukelihood of understatement and cover-up getting by without 
being detected. In Chapter 3 the relation of validity to size of sample has 

120 



Validity of the data 121 

been discussed. The present chapter covers the special devices which have 
been used to test the significance of the calculated data. 

RE-TAKES 

In order to test the constancy of memory, re-takes of whole histories 
have been made on 162 of the males and females who have contributed to 
the present study. It is unfortunate that a larger series is not now available, 
and this is one of the programs that should be expanded in the future prog- 
ress of the research. In every case there has been a minimum lapse of 
eighteen months between the original history and the re-take, and in many 
cases three to seven years have intervened. The mean lapse has been 38.5 
months. Re-takes, of course, cover activities which had not been engaged 
in until after the time of the original history; but with allowance for that 
fact, correlations have been made between the two records, for a diverse 
fist of representative items. The results are shown in Table 13. There are 
no calculations of reliability which are more illuminating than these, and 
the table merits detailed study. 

The analysis indicates that memory and/or cover-up, or other chance 
factors, introduce errors on certain items and on certain whole groups of 
items, while there is greater validity on other items. The incidence data are 
the most consistent. The coefficients of correlation (tetrachoric) on inci- 
dences are better than 0.9 in every case and better than 0.95 in all but three 
of the cases. The number of identical responses is better than 90 per cent, 
in every instance. The differences between the means calculated for the 
original histories, and the means calculated for the re-takes, are less than 
2.4 per cent in most cases. The differences are larger only in regard to 
masturbation and to the homosexual, where the error is about 4 per cent. 
Throughout this volume, the incidence figures may, therefore, be accepted 
as very reliable. Many persons will find it difficult to believe that the high 
incidences shown for several types of sexual activity are not exaggerations 
of the fact, but every calculation indicates that they are understatements, 
if they are in error at all. 

The next most accurate material covers the vital statistics of the popu- 
lation. There are data on the age of the subject, his marital status, the ages 
of his parents, the number of his brothers and sisters, his educational and 
religious background, etc. The coefficients of correlation (Pearsonian) are 
higher than 0.8 in every instance, and in six cases out of eight they are 
higher than 0.9. There are identical responses on the original histories and 
re-takes for better than 80 per cent of the subjects, on eight out of twelve 
items. The lowest scores concern the ages of the mother, of the father, of 
the wife at marriage, and of the husband at marriage — in that descending 
order. The differences between the means for the original histories, taken 
as a group, and the means for the re-takes are, however, immaterial, and 
the averages shown throughout the present volume can be accepted with 



Comparing Originals and Re-takes on 162 Subjects 


d 


cn 

»n rvj wn 

00 on 

in 

m 

S 


0 

<N m 

<N 0 

0 

0 

‘ • y 9 

oOOO^OOOooOn 

d 

00000 

d 

d 

B s 5 A 

coO’— 

-H 

■H -H -H 

-H -H 

-H 

■H 

5 ° a S 

+ 1 1 + + + + 

r- 

(N 

^ 0 

CM 

VO 

H 


0 

^ r-t l> ^ 0 

T-H 

0 

ell 


d 

000 

0 0 

d 

d 

s 


+ 

+ + I 

1 

4 - 

4 



fs 

r- VO VO 

CM 00 

CM 

m 




CS 

CM 'r-i 

r-H 

cn 

CO 


d 

0 0 r-^ 

,-t 0 

d 

d 

il 

.r 4 cor<)wn'OinvoO»n 
na 

4 i 

■H -H -H 

■H -H 

•H 

-H 


‘rt'^OcnOoocor-OfN 







ya^ooo^fn'v 0 co' 0 ^n^ 

CO 

(N r' 

on 

C' 

VO 


P 

HH 

•Ti 

<S l> VO 

00 

m 

Ov 



«n 

cn VO 

CM CM 

CM* 

4 



(N 

<N csj on 

on 






VO on ov 

VO r- 0 

m 



VO 

^ (N 

■»— t ^ 

t— 1 

cn 


0^ 

d 

0 0 

>-H 0 0 

d 


.Tj-cncnvo'O'GVOcNvo 

T 3 • • * 


-H -H 

-H -H 

■n 

■H 

f 3 ^ S 

.iHOON-ci-cNr-r-r^voo 







yo^ooa^cn'OfO'o■^r-» 

VO 

m cso 

ov 

on 

0 

A 

P 

H-l 


00 on m 

CM Tj- 

CM 

ov 



»n 

r 4 r-( r-’ 

fO CM CM 

4 



(N 

CM cs on 

on 


rH 


fH 

}-4 





d J 

0 

’Hcocno^z^‘o>o^nv-)'«:^ 

d 

as 





« . w 

OCT\ 0 ^ 0 ^ 0 ^ 0 ^ 0 ^ 0 ^ 0 ^ 0 ^ 

P On 

wn ov VO 

C- VO 

tH 

ov 

EFl 

OF 

RR 

’S d d d d d d d d d 

0®; 

00 00 ov 

ov ov 


ov 

0 0 

U 0 

S A A A A A A 

^ 0 

2 

d d d 

d d 

d 

d 


0 

P 






H 

CLi 







a g 





(NOcScnOcNr^Tj-ro 

VOOO^nr-^ 0 ^«nTl' 0 '^ 

o^^o^o^o^o^o^o^c^ 


ooooooooo 

zzzzzzz;z; 2 ; 


a 

.2 8 


a 


§.*5 2 

S o 'i 

o cup 

cj . 


0 o ^ ^ 

1 § g 2‘?’5 g I 

s 3 ^ ^ « ss" « o ■3 

IzIlSSaml 


000 < 3 > 00 co 00 


0 

CM 

c- 

m 

cn 

T-H 

OV 


00 

d 


ov 

ov 

00 

•n 

OV 

o\ 

ov 

00 

C“ 

r- 

Ov 

OV 

ov 


»n'^ONror--'sOcot^r-HOOO 

ONoocnmOh-^'or^’ooN’— I 
ooo^vo‘n>n'<:^•o^ooo^a^o^co 


VH 5 V. 2 "3 

^ Z cs S c« 

3 ^ 5 ^ S? OJ <u <- 

^ o ^ ^ ^ ^ 

>-i <N CN 




is I 

j S*' cd 

! g - 
■’z'S 
«■§ 


o o 

bi) toil 
cs cb 

'P £2 

cb C!J M ^ ^ CO 

e s|||fe g 
3 3^ a £ -i -s 


Cy Cy 4 -( C4-1 

0000* 


f^OOf 


bo ElO t:m O O 3 

c<;<;<jzZff! 


S- Cl,3 
•a § g 
g feb'S 

S 00 CO 
.2 3 3 
•300 

O 

^ ’a> 

W 0 i!‘ 


•oo'c>^^>oooa^o' 00 ' 


122 



cn O 

(N NO NO 
O O 

cn ^ 

O ^ 
CM CM 

'ct 'st CO cn cn in 

CM CM cn cn cn cn 

rtinC'''EtNOcO(j\»nT-( 
CNCMOi— irMCMv-(CM'st 

CO 

cn 

o o o o o o o 

OOOOOOOO 

ooooooooo 

d 

-H -H 

■H -H H 

■H -H 

•H -H 

■H H H -H H H 



CO <N 

’-H r- ON 

o o ^ 

CM '-H 
(N ^ 

cn CM 

o T-i 

^ NO t- m NO o 

cn m CM ^ ^ CM 

cMcni-<OcncMOON'5t 

OOOcMcMcnT-icnrH 

o 

O O 
+ + 

o o o 

1 1 1 

d d 
+ + 

o o 

1 + 

o o o o o o 

1 +++++ 

oodoooooo 

1 1 H“ + + 1 4" H“ 1 

d 

+ 

in ('M 

m NO t- 

o O r-< 

MO 

Tt Tt 
CM CM 

CM CM 'St cn cn cn 

i>NOONNominT-ic4»n 

CMCMO^cncMcM'StNO 


‘o O 

o o o 

d d 

d d 

o o o o o d 

d d d d d d d d d 

d 


-H -H -H 

■H H 

■H -H 

■H -H -fi -H H -H 


■H 

^co t-- 

00 T-H 

r- r- NO 
CM cn CM 

r-H in 

Tt CM 

mooOcn^O'ctcM 

inTj-ONTtmOincM 

'r-HTtOO'f'stOOOOONCM 

OcnONDC-'cnNOcMcn 

r- 

NO 

cn r4 

d d d 

CM 

^ O 

cn cn m Tt Tt 'St 

cooocncnrnininr-oo 

r—t T~( t-H r~i r-H rH 

m 


o 


r-( r<i ^ 0^ m 

O O t-H r-H 

d d d d d d 


S-H-H-H-H-H-H-H 

^(N'OooT}■w-la^T^■ 
^ ON <N <4 ^ 

^ d d d d d CN d 


p-< 


i> o ‘/N oo c^ NO 

NO NO NO >0 m MO NO 


o o o o o o o 


00 ^ ON ^ O CO 

w-i oo ON 'rf oo ON ro 
NO NO cn NO <0 


ON I ON cn <N 00 

d NO m r~ rf !>• d 

r<N <N VN CN VN CO »0 


^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ 
^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ 


o' ^ 

d d 


^ ^ to lO lo 

d d d d d 


VNfOr-t^OOOONOO 

CSCMCSCSCMcnrJcci 

d d d d d d d d 


» O 'St CM Tf Tf 


oooooooo 


rHNOVNr-irtOOasO 
lo* d d NO d NO 00 

OQOOOQOOOOOOt>t^ 


Ocncci<N'-<ooNr^ 

cnNooN»o«oONOrM 

'^t'^cO'st'rt'ctmcn 


MWCOC/DiZlWicWCO 

cccsjctictfrtCTScdetf 

000<U<OD<DD 

I>^ 

CN<NC'1CM<NCSCS<S 


r--roOcnT-HONmoor-' 

cMrMi-Hr-icscS(NcoNo 

d d d d d d d d d 

cnor-'^r-noooONO 

OcnO'T‘/Nr-»noN’'<t 

oooocncocntrNsoNooo 


d 

■H 


ONOfNJCOON'^cn-^T-t o 

insOt^lOU-imNOOOCO MO 

d d d d d d d d d d 


T-ir^rC)OOT-<OOTt'<tC<Nt^NO 


t^<3Nr''<r)<Nfn'OfM'<tlO':t 

NO‘OCCOOOO<ONOC''-NOOONO 


rS 

03 

a 

T3 

Rt 

‘S 

o 


VOOJ>NO':tOO(NONCn'.-Hr^ 


'~<cnNor-oo(ri(NcnONt''-' 

cnir-(to'st'^<Nfn'':tcci«n':t 


cti 

a 


o 

O 


c^cdcdcScictSc^rtcJctJcd 

<L><UCU4>0CD0a)0(L>C^, 

I>-i 


§H^ 


i| 

i=g 


§ 

a 

a 

o 

o 




O _ .^-r- 

c3 03 O > O 

a 3 5:P “ Hf o 
-H 5 5 _g o o g 

CO 00 o -43 .'^ .■t::^ C 

SllliSSa 



Dsosa-ocup 

;o:ufi2<e>o 


00 CM ON CO 'Ct 

,-t o 'Ct ON C)0 00 
CS <N ^ 


r-i'^'?t»OT-tlO»nON 

CScOOOr-ilO'O'O'O 


Nor't^‘n<oOcooooot-HC)0 

C^t''»iOOONOOCO<7v<NCM'!t 


123 


108 males and 54 females are involved. The lapse between originals and re-takes ranged between 18 and 88 months (7 years and 4 months). The mean 
lapse is 38 . 5 months. The more than 200 “cases” in the frequency data depend on the fact that a single history may contribute data on more than one 
5-year period. 



124 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN TEE HUMAN MALE 


little reservation. On the other hand, wherever the vital statistics on an 
individual history are to be used in a calculation, there should be allow- 
ances of a year, plus or minus. 

Reports on ages of first experience in each type of sexual activity are 
much less accurate. The coefficients of correlation vary between 0.5 and 
0.8. The number of precisely identical responses is quite low, ranging from 
13 per cent to 57 per cent on most items. The number of responses that 
are identical within one year, plus or minus, is much higher, lying between 
70 and 87 per cent on more than half of the items. However, in spite of the 
inadequacies of individual histories, the means for the whole population 
may be accepted with less question. The differences between the means for 
the originals and the means for the re-takes ordinarily constitute 5 per cent 
or less of the quantity involved. The lowest scores on memory of first 
experience pertain to the pre-adolescent sex play and to the first experiences 
in nocturnal emissions and heterosexual petting. These items are more 
indefinite and therefore more difficult to remember than such things as 
first ejaculation, first coitus, or first experience in other socio-sexual 
activities. 

Reports on frequencies of sexual activity give correlations which run 
close to 0.6 on all of the items. This is a significant correlation, but not as 
reliable as that obtained on incidences or on many of the other items. The 
percents of identical responses are low, lying between 25 and 50 percent, 
and the percents of identical responses phis or minus one unit of measure- 
ment are still less than 65 per cent in most cases. The error on the means 
lies between 5 and 10 per cent on most items, and that much allowance 
should be made on any statement in this volume concerning frequencies of 
sexual activity. While the frequency data on any individual history are 
undoubtedly approximations to the fact, they are not accurate enough to be 
pushed in detail. 

The poorest memory applies to the ages at which the individual first 
learns particular things, e.g., the age at which he fiirst learns there is such a 
thing as intercourse, pregnancy, prostitution, etc. Even when a leeway 
of two years is allowed as identity, the coefficients of correlation are no 
higher than 0.4 and 0.5, and the number of identical responses is under 
50 per cent. If an additional allowance is granted of plus or minus two more 
years, the number of identical responses is brought up to something over 
80 per cent on most items. This means that a five-year allowance must be 
made for any answer in this area, z.e., an allowance of plus or minus 2.5 
on the given answer. Here again, however, the means calculated for whole 
populations are much better, and a correction of something between 1 
ai|d 5 per cent seems a sufficient allowance. 

There is no way of knowing whether the responses are more accurate on 
the first histories or on their re-takes, or whether either of them represents 



VALIDITY OF THE DATA 


125 


identity with the fact. Re-takes test the constancy of memory and the con- 
stancy of the degree of cover-up, rather than the validity of the record. 
There is reason for believing that memory which stays as fixed as it does 
on most of the items in this study is not wholly capricious, but allowance 
must be made for the fact that one may come to believe in a fiction on 
which he has decided at some time in his life. In general, the re-takes raise 
the incidence figures and, strikingly enough, they raise the record of age of 
first experience, first knowledge, etc. This suggests that as the individual 
grows older the period of beginning any type of activity seems to him to 
move up, to some degree. These are matters of broad import in psychology, 
but their more extensive examination has not yet been possible within the 
confines of the present study. 

COMPARISONS OF SPOUSES 

The histories of the two spouses in any marriage should contain a cer- 
tain number of identities, and comparisons of such pairs of histories have 
given some insight into the validity of memory. Therefore, in this study, 
especial attention has been given to securing histories from spouses, and 
231 pairs of spouses are the bases of the comparisons shown in Table 14. 
The items analyzed in the table include some vital statistics, the record of 
coital frequencies, and details concerning the foreplay, positions, and other 
techniques employed in the marital coitus. On the whole, the record shows 
an amazing agreement between the statements of the husbands and of the 
wives in each marriage, although allowance must be made for the possi- 
bility that there may have been collusion between some of the partners, 
and a conscious or unconscious agreement to distort the fact. 

The coefficients of correlation between the repHes of the husbands and 
the repHes of the respective wives have the following values: 


Correlations in Replies of Spouses 


Coefficients 

Items 

Percentage 
of Items 

Cumulated 

Percentages 

0.50-0.59 

2 

6.3 

100.0 

0.60-0.69 

6 

18.8 

93.7 

0.70-0.79 

10 

31.2 

74.9 

0.80-0.89 

5 

15.6 

43.7 

0.90-0.99 

9 

28.1 

28.1 

Totals 

32 

100.0 



In regard to three-quarters of the items, the coefficient of correlation 
between the replies of the husbands and of the wives is 0.7 or better; for 
nearly half of the items it is 0.8 or better ; and for more than a quarter of the 




126 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Comparing Data prom 231 Pairs of Spouses 


1 

CASES 

ITEMS 

INVOLVED 

UNIT 

OF 

MEASURE- 

MENT 

IDENT. 

RSPNS 

% 

WITH- 

IN 

1 UNIT 
OF 

IDENT. 

% 

COFFFIC. 

OF 

CORREL. 

MEAN OF 

husband’s 

REPORTS 

MEAN OF 

wife’s 

REPORTS 

DIFF. 

OF 

means: 

d^-9 


Vital Statistics 




Pears, r 




229 

Years married 

1 year 

88.6 

96.1 

0.99 

6.35 =b0.42 

6.40 ± 0.42 

-0.05 

214 

Pre-marital 









acquaint. 

12 mon. 

57.9 

86.9 

0.88 

42.11 =i=2.83 

40.88 ±2.74 

4-1.23 

156 

Engagement 

4 mon. 

57.1 

78.2 

0.83 

12.64 =b 1.03 

12.85 ± 1.07 

-0.21 

226 

Age, (f at marr. 

1 year 

68.6 

97,3 

0.99 

27.27 ±0.37 

27.17 ± 0.37 

40.10 

228 

Age, 9 at marr. 

1 year 

61.8 

92.5 

0.63 

24.88 ±0.32 

24.75 ±0.32 

+0.13 

231 

No. children 

1 child 

99.6 

100.0 

0.99 

0.90 ±0.09 

0.90 ±0.09 

0.00 

185 

No. abortions 

1 event 

90.3 

98.4 

0.76 

0.32 ±0.06 

0.41 ± 0.08 

-0.09 

227 

Lapse, first 









coitus — marr. 

6 mon. 

74.4 

89.4 

0.85 

5.09 ±0.88 

4.72 ± 0.86 

40.37 

87 

Lapse, marr. 









— first birth 

6 mon. 

66.7 

89.7 

0.96 

28.05 ± 1.99 

28.19 ±2.01 

-0.14 

220 

Educ. level, cf 

2 years 

84.1 

99.1 

0.97 

16.23 ±0.26 

16.16 ± 0.25 

40.07 

223 

Educ. level, 9 

2 years 

79.4 

97.8 

0.92 

14.41 ± 0.21 

14.67 ± 0.21 

-0.26 

219 

Occup. class, (f 

1 of 9 

91.8 

98.6 

0.98 

5.32 ±0.14 

5.27 ±0.14 

40,05 


Coital Freq. 








223 

Max. freq., 









marit. coitus 

2/wk. 

33.2 

68.2 

0.54 

6.72 ±0.31 

6.74 ± 0.31 

-0.02 

225 

Av. freq., 









early marr. 

1/wk. 

34.7 

73.3 

0.50 

2.73 ± 0.13 

3.00 ±0.14 

-0.27 

226 

Av. freq., now 

1/wk. 

56.6 

88.1 

0.60 

1.91 ± 0.11 

2.21 ± 0.13 

-0.30 

218 

% with 









orgasm, 9 

10% 

55.0 

71.1 

Q.75 

69.82 ±2.16 

66.83 ±2.26 

42.99 


Techniques in 




Tetra- 

% Husbands 

% Wives 



coital foreplay 




choric r 

ReportingYes 

ReportingYes 


229 

Kiss 

Yes, No ' 

97 


0.92 

95.6 ± 1 35 

99.1 ± 0.62 

-3.5 

228 

Deep kiss 

Yes, No 

85 


0.72 

85.1 ± 2.36 

82.0 ± 2.54 

43.1 

228 

Hand— 9 



1 






breast 

Yes, No 

95 


0.78 

95.6 ± 1.36 

196.5 ± 1.22 

-0.9 

228 

Mouth — 9 









breast 1 

Yes, No 

89 


0.79 

90.4 ±2.04 

86.0 ± 2.43 

44.4 

229 

Hand — 9 









genitalia 

Yes, No 

90 


0.61 

92.6 ± 1.73 

92.2 ± 1.77 

40.4 

226 

Hand — 






1 



genitalia 

Yes, No 

85 


0.70 

83.6 ± 2.46 

85.8 ± 2.32 ! 

- .2 

220 

Mouth — 9 









genitalia 

Yes, No 

82 


0.84 

35.9 ± 3.23 

37.3 ± 3.26 

-1.4 

226 

Mouth — cf 









genitalia 

Yes, No 

85 


0.93 

33.6 ± 3.14 

35.4 ± 3.18 

-1.8 


Coital techniq. 








228 

Male above 

Yes, No 

93 


0.75 

94.3 ± 1.54 

93.5 ± 1.63 

40,8 

228 

Female above 

Yes, No 

76 


0.74 

54.8 ± 3.30 

49.1 ± 3.31 

45.7 

224 

On side 

Yes, No 

76 


0,68 

39.3 ± 3.26 

37.1 ±3.23 

42.2 

221 

Sitting 

Yes, No 

81 


0.63 

19.0 ±2.64 

17.6 ±2.56 

41.4 

227 

Standing 

Yes, No 

89 


0.64 

10.1 ±2.00 

7.9 ± 1.79 

42.2 

224 

Rear entrance 

Yes, No 

83 


0.77 

24.6 ± 2.88 

17.4 ±2.53 

47.2 

186 

Coitus nude 

Yes, No 

90 


0.82 

87.1 ± 2.46 

89.2 ±2.28 

-2 1 

223 

Multiple 









orgasm, cf* 

Yes, No 

95 


0.74 

4.9 ± 1.45 

4.1 ± 1.33 

40.8 


Table 14. Comparisons of data obtained from spouses 



VALIDITY OF THE DATA 127 

items it is 0.9 or better. These are very high scores, as correlations go in 
social and psychological studies. 

The number of identical replies received from the two spouses in each 
marriage is as follows : 


Percents 

Exact Identity 

I Within One Unit (=t) of Identity 

of 







Identical 

No. of 

Percentage 

Cumulated 


Percentage 

Cumulated 

Replies ; 

Items 

Involved 

of 

Items 

Percentages 

Cases 

of 

Cases 

Percentages 

30-39 

2 

6.2 

100.0 




40-49 

0 

0.0 

93.8 




50-59 

4 

12.5 

93.8 




60-69 

3 

9.4 

81.3 

1 

6.2 

100.0 

70-79 

4 

12.5 

71.9 

3 

18.8 

93.8 

80-89 

10 

31.3 

59.4 

4 

25.0 

75.0 

90-99 

9 

28.1 

28.1 

8 

50.0 

50.0 

Totals 

32 

100.0 


16 

100.0 

100.0 


In regard to nearly 60 per cent of the items, the replies were identical for 
80 per cent or more of the couples. In regard to three-quarters of the items, 
they were within one unit of identity for 80 per cent of the couples. These 
close identities are particularly impressive when it is remembered that in 
many instances there were intervals of two to six years or more between 
the interviews with the two spouses. On half of the items, there is near 
identity in something between 90 and 100 per cent of the histories. In most 
instances, near identity is about all that a student of behavior is interested 
in ; it rarely matters whether the age of the spouse is a year one way or the 
other of the reported age, whether the other spouse has had six years or 
eight years of schooling, whether the male is a semi-skilled or a more 
skilled mechanic. Allowances are also to be made for the fact that some 
persons calculate their ages as from the last birthday, and some persons 
from the forthcoming birthday, and that a difference of a year in two re- 
ports may, in actuality, mean identity. 

Averages for the entire group of histories are still closer than the correla- 
tions on individual histories. The differences between the means calculated 
for all the males and the means calculated for all the females are quite 
insignificant on all but a few items, as an examination of Table 14 will 
show. 

The coeflacients of correlation and the percentages of identical replies 
are low only in regard to the frequencies of marital coitus, and in regard to 
the percentage of the time in which the female reaches orgasm during the 
marital coitus. On the latter point, the male believes that his female part- 


128 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


ner experiences orgasm more often than she herself reports ; but it is to be 
noted that the wife sometimes deceives her husband deliberately on that 
point. In regard to the frequencies of marital intercourse, there is an inter- 
esting psychological element involved. It is often the female who reports 
the higher frequencies, and this is undoubtedly related to the fact that 
females often complain of their husbands’ desire for more coitus. Conse- 
quently, the females may be overestimating the actual frequencies. Simi- 
larly, the husbands regularly complain of their wives’ lack of desire for 
coitus and, in consequence, are probably underestimating the frequencies 
with which they do have it. On individual histories, errors on this particu- 
lar point may be expected in as many as two-thirds of the cases ; but in 
regard to averages for whole populations, the correction is, again, remark- 
ably small. 

For most items of the sort covered in this study, it may be expected that 
something between 80 and 99 per cent of the subjects will give replies that 
will be verified, independently, by the partners in their marriages. 

OTHER CROSS-CHECKS 

In addition to re-takes and pairs of histories from spouses, a variety of 
other cross-checks have provided some further measure of the accuracy of 
memory. For instance, the internal consistency of a history, as it is pieced 
together in an interview, is of considerable significance as a test of validity. 
In each case, the subject is asked to supply a great many dates and records 
of ages in a sequence which is far from chronologic. Nevertheless, there is 
usually considerable coherence in the chronology that comes out of such 
a tabulation. Some time, it may be possible to reduce this matter to more 
precise calculation. 

In Chapter 2, in connection with a discussion of interviewing techniques, 
it was pointed out that a skillful interviewer develops a certain abihty to 
recognize falsification and cover-up when taking a history, and does have 
a considerable measure of the validity of the record he is getting, even 
though it may not be possible to reduce such a measure to statistical terms. 

In Chapter 2 it was also pointed out that the trained interviewer must 
have a considerable fund of information concerning patterns of sexual 
behavior in different segments of the population. The constancy with 
which such patterns are followed in individual histories is very high, as 
later chapters (particularly Chapter 10) in the present volume will show. 
While it may be questioned whether a subject sometimes reports what he 
thinks is usual and acceptable in his social group, it should be emphasized 
that exceedingly few subjects have any idea of the patterns of behavior 
of other persons in their group. The histories cover such a mass of detail 
as few persons have ever discussed with their friends, and they simply do 
not know how those friends or any other persons in the same group are 



VALIDITY OF THE DATA 


129 


answering such questions. When 90 to 95 per cent of the persons in any 
social level report histories which agree with the patterns shown in Chapter 
10, they not only establish the nature of the group patterns, but establish 
the validity of their own reports as well. 

Further cross-checks are provided by sexual partners other than spouses. 
Whether they are involved in heterosexual or homosexual relations, each 
partner may supply some information about the other individual’s history. 
The cross-references have been kept, and it may be possible to subject the 
material to statistical comparisons when the series are large enough. Now 
it can be reported that the secondhand information secured in this way has 
proved to be surprisingly accurate in most cases where there has been a 
chance to check it. Although none of this secondhand material has been 
used in any of the calculations in the present volume, it has been of value 
as a means of testing the accuracy of the related histories. Within the con- 
fines of the present chapter, a single example of this sort will have to suffice. 

This example concerns the accuracy of the incidence data on the homo- 
sexual experience had by men in penal institutions and reported by them 
while they are under confinement in such institutions. This is material 
about which it is especially diflBcult to secure information, although in 
nearly all prisons there is a continuous undercurrent of gossip concerning 
such activity. The gossip reflects a mixture of desire for experience and 
bitter condemnation of such activity — a conflict between the individual’s 
personal needs, and his training in the social traditions on matters of sex. 
There is, of course, official condemnation of such activity; and this may 
involve, especially in institutions for men, severe corporal punishment, loss 
of privileges, solitary confinement, and often an extension by a year or more 
of the sentence of an inmate who is discovered, suspected, or merely 
accused of homosexual relations. To persuade such an inmate to contrib- 
ute a record of his activity while he is still in prison, is a considerable test 
of the ability of an interviewer. Nonetheless, we have gotten such records 
from something between 35 and 85 per cent of the inmates of every insti- 
tution in which we have worked. 

In one prison, a male who was well acquainted with the institution 
agreed to take the list of three hundred and fifty men who had contributed 
histories to this study, and to indicate which of them were, to his knowl- 
edge, currently having homosexual relations. About most of these men he 
knew nothing, but from the list he picked 32 with whom he claimed to have 
had relations, or whom he had actually seen in such relations. The infor- 
mant never knew how his record compared with the data we had secured 
in the interviewing, but the histories showed that 27 of the 32 men (Le,, 
about 85 per cent!) had admitted their experience when they were first 
interviewed. Two of the others had left the institution before they could be 
interviewed again, but the remaining three readily admitted their activity 



130 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


when they were called back for a second conference. This provided a check 
on the validity of secondhand reports and, incidentally, gave some measure 
of the extent of the cover-up that we are getting in the histories. Considering 
the nature of the item involved, this 15 per cent failure probably approaches 
the maximum which will be found anywhere in this study. 

MEMORY VERSUS PHYSICAL FINDINGS 

It is possible to make comparisons of certain of the data obtained by 
interviews in the present study, and data obtained in some other studies 


Percent of Boys Beginning Pubic Hair Development 


AGE * 

CRAMPTON 

1908 

DIMOCK 

1937 

SCHONFELD 

1943 

PRESENT 

STUDY 



Cumu- 


Cumu- 


Cumu- 


Cumu- 


% 

lated 

% 

/o 

lated 

% 

/o 

lated 

% 

% 

lated 

% 

9 







0.2 

0.2 

10 



2.0 

2.0 



2.0 

2.2 

11 

7.0 

7.0 

15.0 

17.0 

12.0 

12.0 

7.7 

9.9 

12 

24.0 

31.0 

21.0 

38.0 

30.0 

42.0 

25.5 

35.4 

13 

28.0 

59.0 

22.0 

60.0 i 

25.0 

67.0 

33.5 

68.9 

14 

25.0 

84.0 

27.0 

87.0 ! 

»12.0 

79.0 

22.8 

91.7 

15 1 

11.0 

95.0 

11.0 

98.0 

19.0 

98.0 

5.5 

97.2 

16 

4.0 

99.0 

2.0 

100.0 

1,0 

99.0 

2.0 

99.2 

17 

1.0 

100.0 



1.0 

100.0 

0.7 

99.9 

18 








99,9 

19 








99.9 

20 







0.1 

100.0 

Cases 

3835 

1406 

1475 

2511 

Mean 

13.44 1.51 

13.08 

i 

13.45=1=0.03 

Median 



13.17 

13.43 


Table 15. Comparisons of data obtained in four studies on pubic hair development 


The data from the Crampton, Dimock, and Schonfeld studies were based upon phys- 
ical examinations of young boys. The present study has been dependent upon the mem- 
ory of older persons recalling their adolescent experiences. 


from direct observations on similar groups of males. The readiest body of 
such material concerns adolescent developments. In Table 15 and Figure 
15, data on pubic hair development, drawn from three of the observational 
studies (Crampton 1908, Dimock 1937, Schonfeld 1943), are compared 
with data contributed by the subjects in the present study, on the basis of 
recall. The near identity of the recall curve and the other curves is remark- 



VALIDITY OF THE DATA 


131 


able, especially in consideration of the fact that many subjects protest that 
such an item as pubic hair development is recalled with less certainty than 
most other items. The larger series in the present study gives a growth 
curve which is smoother and more usual in type than the curves which some 
of the smaller series give. It is to be noted, again, that though this compari- 
son goes a long way to justify recall as a source of averages for whole 
groups, it does not demonstrate how accurate the memory of any partic- 
ular individual may be concerning his own individual history. 

There are a number of other observational studies of adolescent develop- 
ments (cited in Chapter 5), but unfortunately none of them provides data 



AGE 


Figure 15. Comparison of memory with observational data 


Record for age of onset of growth of pubic hair. The Crampton, Dimock, and Schon- 
feld studies based upon physical examinations of boys. The present study based on 
memory of older persons recalling adolescent experience. 


which can be used in comparison with data from the present study. Some 
of these other studies are based on populations which are too small to 
be significant. Some of them appear to involve gross errors. In several 
cases, the other studies have used definitions of adolescent characters 
which are different from those used in the present study, e.g., voice change 
is defined in some of the studies as the first sign of deepening voice (which 
is in reality often a pre-adolescent development), and pubic hair is defined 
as kinky hair (which may not appear until sometime after the first distinct 
but straight hair develops in the pubic area). It will, therefore, be necessary 
to wait until further observational data are available for further testing of 
the validity of recall on these characters. 




VALIDITY OF THE DATA 


133 


HUNDRED PERCENT SAMPLES 

When a study of everyday people discloses such unexpected behavior 
as the present study has disclosed, it is natural enough that one should 
wonder whether there has been some bias in the investigator’s choice of 
subjects or his emphasis in interviewing. On this point, it has already been 
explained (Chapter 3) that the persons who have contributed histories have 
represented considerable samples and, whenever possible, hundred percent 
samples of each group that has been involved in the study. There has been 
next to no selection of subjects on the basis of anything that was pre- 
viously known about their histories. The only exceptions have come in 
regard to a few extreme items which, as already explained (Chapter 3), 
could not have been obtained by way of hundred percent samples ; and 
most of the histories have come from unselected individuals in whole 
groups. Such unselectcd series have been the prime bases for the incidence 
figures in this volume. See the preceding chapter for more detailed com- 
parisons of the data obtained from hundred percent samples, and from 
the remainder of the population. 

COMPARISONS OF INTERVIEWERS 

One of the questions most frequently raised about the present research, 
and a thoroughly legitimate question about any research, concerns 
the possibility of another investigator duplicating the results. Moreover, 
in any project which has involved two or more investigators, it is 
important to compare the results obtained by each, before one can 
fairly add together the data obtained by the several interviewers. In con- 
sequence, throughout the years of this investigation repeated comparisons 
of that sort have been made. 

Comparisons of the data obtained by different investigators can be 
significant only when the persons contributing histories to each investi- 
gator belong to the same sex, race, marital status, age, educational level, 
rural-urban group, religious group, etc. It is meaningless to compare 
data drawn from quite different sorts of groups. Even when comparisons 
are made for groups that are the products of six-way breakdowns, which 
is the limit possible with the sample now at hand, there are certainly many 
other factors which affect variation within each group. Consequently, it 
is not to be expected that the material obtained by two interviewers work- 
ing with two different populations, even after a six-way breakdown, 
should be quite identical. 

Table 16 compares the data obtained by the three authors of the present 
volume. The table includes all of those groups from which each of the 
interviewers has obtained at least 300 histories. To compare smaller popu- 
lations would have introduced errors consequent on size of sample (Chap- 
ter 3). The senior author began accumulating histories some years before 



134 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Comparisons of Freqxjency Data Obtained by Three Interviewers 


group 

inter- 

viewer 

cases 

TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

Incid 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

Age 11-15 








Total Outlet 

Kinsey 

644 

3.05 ± 0 14 

2.11 

93.9 

3.24 ± 0 15 

2.30 


Pomeroy 

607 

2.58 ± 0 11 

1.83 

97.2 

2.65 ± 0.11 

1.90 


Martin 

325 

2.69 0.16 

2.12 

96.0 

2.80 0.17 

2.23 

Masturbation 

Kinsev 

644 

2.36 ± 0.12 

1.46 

77.8 

3 03 0.14 

2.17 


Pomeroy 

607 

2,01 ± 0 10 

1 21 

81.7 

2.46 ± 0.12 

1.75 


Martin 

325 

2.32 i 0.17 

1.70 

82.5 

2.82 0.19 

2.28 

Nocturnal Emiss. 

Kinsey 

644 

0.43 i 0.03 

0.15 

74.1 

0 58 =t= 0.04 

0.31 


Pomeroy 

607 

0.37 =fc 0 03 

0 17 

76,3 

0.49 ± 0.03 

0.29 


Martin 

325 

0.28 ± 0.02 

0.12 

74.8 

0.37 ± 0 03 

0.26 

Pre-mar. Coitus 

Kinsey 

644 

0.09 ± 0.03 

0.00 

6.8 

1.27 ± 0.46 

0 17 


Pomeroy 

607 

0.03 ± 0.01 

0.00 

4.9 

0 54 ± 0 16 

0 20 


Martin 

325 

0.04 ± 0 02 

0.00 

6.8 

0 63 ± 0.27 

0.14 

Homosexual 

Kinsey 

644 

0.09 =i= 0.02 

0.00 

21.1 

0.43 =fc 0 06 

0.10 


Pomeroy 

607 

0 10 =1= 0 02 

0.00 

21.3 

0.45 ± 0.08 

0.09 


Martin 

325 

0.02 ± 0.004 

0.00 

17.5 

0 12 ± 0.02 

0.07 

Age 16-20 








Total Outlet 

Kinsey 

662 

3.05 ± 0.12 

2.30 

99.7 

3.06 =i= 0.12 

2 31 


Pomeroy 

612 

2.50 ± 0.09 

1 87 

99 8 

2.50 =b 0 09 

1.88 


Martin 

331 

2.66 ± 0.13 

2,17 

100 0 

2.66 ± 0.13 

2.17 

Masturbation 

Kinsey 

662 

2.11 ± 0.10 

1.33 

88.1 

2 40 ± 0.11 

1 66 


Pomeroy 

612 

1 72 =*= 0 08 

0.99 

86.9 

1 98 ± 0.09 

1.40 


Martin 

331 

1.99 =b 0 13 

• 1.48 

89.4 

2.22 =i= 0.14 

1.76 

Nocturnal Emiss. 

Kinsey 

662 

0.47 =t: 0.03 

0.27 

91.2 

0.51 0.03 

0.31 


Pomeroy 

612 

0.43 ± 0.02 

0.27 

91.5 

0.47 ± 0 02 

0.31 


Martin 

331 

0.36 =±= 0.02 

0.24 

89.7 

0.40 ± 0.03 

0.28 

Pre-mar. Coitus 

Kinsey 

662 

0.26 0.04 

0.00 

37.3 

0.70 =1= 0.11 

0.19 


Pomeroy 

612 

0.15 =1= 0.02 

0.00 

35.5 

0.44 ± 0.05 

0.15 


Martin 

331 

0.19 =1= 0.03 

0.00 

38.4 

0.49 =i= 0 07 

0.11 

Homosexual 

Kinsey 

662 

0.08 =fc 0.02 

0.00 

14.8 

0.50 0.09 

0 09 


Pomeroy 

612 

0.06 ± 0.01 

O.CO 

17.6 

0.33 ± 0.06 

0.08 


Martin 

331 

0.02 =t= 0.005 

0.00 

11.8 

0.15 ± 0.04 

0 07 

Age 21-25 








Total Outlet 

Kinsey 

533 

2,64 =L 0.12 

1.79 

99.6 

2.65 ± 0.13 

1.80 


Pomeroy 

432 

2.18 ± 0.09 

1.70 

100. 0 

2.18 ± 0.09 

1.70 

Masturbation 

Kinsey 

533 

1,43 0.09 

0.62 

86 3 

1.66 =1= 0.10 

0.87 


Pomeroy 

432 

1.12 0.06 

0.59 

87 7 

1.27 ± 0 07 

0.74 

Nocturnal Emiss. 

Kinsey 

533 

0.39 =1= 0.03 

0.23 

87.1 

0.45 ± 0.03 

0.29 


Pomeroy 

432 

0.38 =fc 0,02 

0.25 

87.7 

0.43 0.02 

0.30 

Pre-mar. Coitus 

Kinsey 

533 

0.49 0.06 

0.04 

57.4 

0.85 =i= 0.11 

0.29 


Pomeroy 

432 

0.40 ± 0.04 

0.03 

54.6 

0.72 ± 0.07 

0.30 

Homosexual 

Kinsey 

533 

0.11 0.03 

0.00 

10.7 

1.05 =fc 0,22 

0.34 


Pomeroy 

432 

0.08 0.02 

0.00 

8.8 

0.96 0.20 

0.37 


Table 16. Comparisons of data obtained by different interviewers 


Comparisons confined to groups with over 300 histories, of same background for sex, race, 
marital status, educational level (all of college level), and age. Based on histories taken during 
last four years of the research. 



VALIDITY OF THE DATA 


135 


the other two authors were involved; and on the chance that the first 
investigator’s techniques of interviewing and his methods of recording 
may have varied in that time, the comparisons in Table 16 are confined to 
the data obtained by each of the three interviewers during the same period 
of time, namely, the more recent four years of this study. 

The most important conclusions to be drawn from these comparisons 
are: 

1. Three different interviewers have obtained very similar data from 
three different populations. Out of the 75 sets of calculations which appear 
in Table 16, 35 are so similar that the differences are immaterial — closer 
than any person could calculate about his own history. Such identity is 
amazing. There seems no reason to doubt that any other group of investi- 
gators could dupHcate these results if their scientific objectivity and their 
methods in interviewing were comparable to those which have been used 
in the present study. In about 10 of the 75 sets of calculations, there are 
more or less material differences between the lowest and the highest 
figures. 

2. The incidence data are more nearly identical for the three inter- 
viewers than the frequency data. There is close identity in incidences even 
for such a taboo item as the homosexual where, it will be noted, the active 
incidence figures in each of the five-year periods prove to be five to ten 
times as high as any which have previously been pubhshed. Whether the 
actual incidence figure for the homosexual in any particular group is 
17.5 per cent or 21.3 per cent is of no great moment. The fact remains that 
the general locus of this, and of all the other figures, is established by the 
independent interviewing of three persons drawing their samples very 
largely at random, or from hundred percent groups which (especially in 
the case of the college level) constituted a considerable portion of the 
sample. 

3. Some selection has been involved in assigning subjects to interviewers. 
Older persons, persons with more promiscuous histories (whether hetero- 
sexual or homosexual), and persons who were expected to prove reticent 
because of socially unusual items which were known to be in their histories, 
have more often been interviewed by the senior investigator, especially 
during the early years in the training of the younger members of the staff^. 
This undoubtedly accounts for some of the differences between inter- 
viewers : for the lower homosexual incidence and frequency figures for the 
third interviewer, for the higher frequency data on pre-marital intercourse 
for the first interviewer, etc. 

4. The frequency data for masturbation and for pre-marital intercourse 
(and in consequence for total outlet) are highest in every age group for the 
first interviewer and lowest for the second interviewer. The incidence and 



136 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Comparisons of Data Obtained by Three Interviewers 


Accumulative Incidence: Masturbation 
Educational Level 13+ 


AGE 

HISTORIES TAKEN 

BY KINSEY 

HISTORIES TAKEN 

BY POMEROY 

HISTORIES TAKEN 

BY MARTIN 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

1783 

0.0 

595 

0.2 

324 

0.0 

9 

1783 

0.3 

595 

0.5 

324 

0.0 

10 

1783 

2.3 

595 

2.7 

324 

1.9 

11 

1783 

8.1 

595 

11.4 

324 

9.3 

12 

1783 

26.4 

595 

31.1 

324 

29.0 

13 

1783 

52.3 

595 

56.3 

324 

50.0 

14 

1783 

72.4 

595 

72.8 

324 

69.1 

15 

1783 

79.6 

595 

80.8 

324 

81.5 

16 

1782 

83.6 

595 

83.4 

324 

88.3 

17 

1781 

86.6 

594 

86.0 

324 

90.4 

18 

1756 

88.7 

573 

88.7 

306 

90.8 

19 

1642 

90.0 

548 

89.2 

283 

91.5 

20 

1472 

90.6 

522 

90.8 

255 

93.3 

21 

1273 

91.4 

472 » 

91.7 

216 

94.0 

22 

1038 

92.2 

413 

92.7 

167 

94.6 

23 

874 

92.6 

370 

93.5 

127 

96.1 

24 

740 

92.7 

314 

93.3 

86 

94.2 

25 

663 

93.5 

275 

94.2 

56 

96.4 

26 

582 

94.2 

250 

96.0 



27 

512 

94.7 

222 

96.4 



28 

467 

94.6 

202 

96.5 i 



29 

432 

94.2 

179 

96.6 



30 

400 

95.0 

154 

96.8 



31 

378 

94.7 

135 

96.3 



32 

353 

94.9 

125 

96.8 



33 

322 

94.7 

114 

96.5 



34 

299 

95.0 

101 

98.0 



35 

277 

94.9 

95 

97.9 



36 

261 

95.0 

86 

97.7 



37 

233 

94.8 

81 

97.5 



38 

222 

95.0 

78 

97.4 



39 

207 

94.7 

66 

97.0 



40 

191 

95.3 

60 

98.3 




Table 17. Comparisons of data obtained by dijBferent interviewers, on 

mastmbation 

Based on males of the college level. 




PERCENT OF TOTAL POPULATION PERCENT OF TOTAL POPULATION PERCENT OF TOTAL POPULATION 


DATA OBTAINED BY DIFFERENT INTERVIEWERS 



Figures 16-18. Comparing accumulative incidence data obtained by different 

interviewers 

Data on masturbation, nocturnal emissions, and intercourse of any sort. 

137 


138 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Comparisons of Data Obtained by Three Different Interviewers 


Accumulative Incidence Data 


Nocturnal Emissions 
Educ. Level 13+ 


age 

histories 

taken 

BY KINSEY 

histories 

taken 

BY POMEROY 

HISTORIES j 

taken 

BY MARTIN 

HISTORIES 

TAKEN 

BY KINSEY 

HISTORIES 

TAKEN 

BY POMEROY 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

1779 

0.0 

595 

0.0 

324 

0.0 

586 

0.0 

191 

0.0 

9 

1779 

0.0 

595 

0.0 

324 

0.0 1 

586 

0.0 

191 

0.0 

10 

1779 

j 0.4 

595 

0.5 ! 

324 

1.2 

586 

0.0 

191 

0.5 

11 

1779 

2.6 

595 

3.5 1 

324 

5.2 

585 

1.2 

191 

1.0 

12 

1779 

10.2 

595 

14.6 

324 

13.9 

585 

6.2 

191 

9.4 

13 

1779 

25.7 

595 

35.8 

324 

34.6 

585 

13.5 1 

191 

19.4 

14 

1779 

48.6 

595 

60.0 

324 

55.6 

583 

27.8 

191 

33.0 

15 

1779 

65.8 

595 

75.3 

324 

72.8 

582 

41.8 

186 

52.2 

16 

1778 

78.0 

595 

86.1 

324 

84.0 

574 

57.0 

177 

66.7 

17 

1777 

86.1 

594 

89.2 

324 

87.7 

556 

68.2 

155 

74.2 

18 

1752 

90.5 

573 

92.5 

306 

90.8 

536 

78.5 

144 

84.0 

19 

1638 

92.4 

548 

92.9 

283 

,92.9 

514 

83.1 

129 

86.8 

20 

1468 

93.4 

522 

94.1 

255 

93.7 

493 

86.8 

116 

89.7 

21 

1269 

94.2 

472 

95.3 

216 

94.9 

475 

89.1 

110 

90.9 

22 

1036 

94.4 

413 

95.6 

167 

94.6 

459 

91.7 

99 

91.9 

23 

873 

94.5 

370 

95.9 

127 

94.5 

443 

93.0 

95 

91.6 

24 

739 

95.5 

314 

96.2 

86 

95.3 

425 

95.1 

90 

94.4 

25 

662 

96.1 

275 

97.1 

56 

94.6 

407 

96.1 

83 

95.2 

26 

581 

96.2 

250 

98.0 



395 

96.7 

81 

96.3 

27 

511 

96.1 

222 

97.7 



383 

97.7 

77 

96.1 

28 

466 

96.4 

202 

98.0 



369 

97.8 

76 

96.1 

29 

431 

96.5 

179 

97.8 



347 

97.7 

72 1 

95.8 

30 

399 

97.0 

154 

98.1 



334 

97.9 

65 ! 

96.9 

31 

377 

96.8 

135 

99.3 



318 

97.8 

59 i 

98.3 

32 

352 

97.2 

125 

99.2 



306 

98.4 

57 

98.2 

33 

321 

97.2 

114 

99.1 



290 

98.6 

53 

98.1 

34 

298 

97.7 

101 

100.0 



281 

98.9 

53 

98.1 

35 

276 

98.2 

95 

100.0 



269 

99.3 

50 

98.0 

36 

260 

98.5 

86 

100.0 



257 

99.2 



37 

232 

98.3 

81 

100.0 



242 

99.2 



38 

221 

98.6 

78 

100.0 



235 

99.1 



39 

206 

98.5 

66 

100.0 



215 

99.1 



40 

190 

98.9 

60 

100.0 



196 

99.0 




Total Intercourse 
Educ. Level 0-8 


Table 18. Comparisons of data obtained by different interviewers, on nocturnal 
emissions and on heterosexual coitus 

The coitus data include intercourse from any source, pre-marital, marital, extra- 
marital, or post-marital, with companions or with prostitutes. 



VALIDITY OF THE DATA 


139 


Comparisons of Data Obtained by Three Different Interviewers 


Accumulative Incidence Data 


Total Pre-marital Intercourse 
Educ. Level 13+ 


Total Intercourse with 
Prostitutes 
Educ. Level 0-8 


AGE 

HISTORIES 

TAKEN 

BY KINSEY 

inSTORIES 

TAKEN 

BY POMEROY 

HISTORIES 

TAKEN 

BY MARITN 

HISTORIES 

TAKEN 

BY KINSEY 

HISTORIES 

TAKEN 

BY POMEROY 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

1 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

1783 

0.0 

595 

0.0 

324 

0.0 

587 

0.0 

191 

0.0 

9 

1783 

0.0 

595 

0.0 

324 

0.3 

587 

0.0 

191 

0.0 

10 

1783 

0.0 

595 

0.0 

324 

0.3 

586 

0.0 

191 

0.0 

11 

1783 

0.2 

595 

0.0 

324 

0.3 

585 

0.2 

191 

0.0 

12 

1 1783 

1.1 

595 

0.7 

324 

0.6 

585 

0.5 

191 

1.0 

13 

1783 

3.6 

595 

2.4 

324 

1.9 

585 : 

1.0 

191 

1.0 

14 

1783 

7.0 

595 

4.0 

324 

3.7 

583 

3.4 

191 

4.2 

15 

1783 

11.2 

595 

5.9 

324 

6.5 

582 ' 

7.6 

186 

7.5 

16 

1782 

17.3 

595 

11.9 

324 

11.4 

574 

18.5 

177 

14.7 

17 

1781 

25.4 

592 

18.1 

324 

19.1 

556 

30.0 

155 

22.6 

18 

1755 

32.8 

570 

25.8 

306 

27.8 

536 

42.2 

144 

36.1 

19 

1638 

40.3 

545 

34.9 

282 

35.5 

514 

47.3 

129 

39.5 

20 

1452 

46.7 

517 

39.7 

251 

42.2 

493 

50.9 

116 

49.1 

21 

1243 

50.4 

461 

46.9 

210 

47.1 

475 

54.9 

110 

52.7 

22 

985 

54.7 

388 

51.0 

159 

57.2 

459 

58.2 

99 

57.6 

23 

111 

56.5 

336 

55.1 

113 

61.1 

443 

59.4 

95 

60.0 

24 

623 

58.6 

267 

59.2 

68 

63.2 

425 

61.6 

90 

63.3 

25 

506 

64.4 

210 1 

61.9 



407 

64.9 

83 

68.7 

26 

398 

63.6 

171 

65.5 



i 395 

66.6 

81 

69.1 

27 

295 

63.4 

115 

67.0 



1 383 

67.4 

77 

68.8 

28 

224 

64.3 

88 

71.6 



369 

67.8 

76 

68.4 

29 

179 

65.4 

69 

72.5 



' 347 

67.7 

72 

66.7 

30 

140 

65.0 

53 

75.5 



i 334 

69.8 

1 65 

70.8 

31 

107 

65.4 





318 

70.4 

59 

69.5 

32 

92 

65.2 





306 

71.6 

57 

70.2 

33 

79 

65.8 





290 

71.7 

53 

71.7 

34 

60 

68.3 





281 

72.6 

53 

71.7 

35 

53 

66.0 





269 

72.9 

50 

74.0 


Table 19. Comparisons of data obtained by different interviewers, on pre-marital 
intercourse and on intercourse with prostitutes 

Total pre-marital intercourse includes the coitus had with companions and with 
prostitutes. Total intercourse with prostitutes includes pre-marital, extra-marital, and 
post-marital data. 



140 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Comparisons of Data Obtained by Three Interviewers 


Accumulative Incidence: Homosexual Outlets 
Educ. Level 13-f- 


AGE 

HISTORIES TAKEN BY 

KINSEY 

HISTORIES TAKEN BY 

POMEROY 

HISTORIES TAKEN BY 

MARTIN 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

1783 

0.0 

595 

0.2 

324 

0.0 

9 

1783 

0.1 

595 

0.2 

324 

0.0 

10 

1783 

0.5 

595 

0.7 

324 

0.0 

11 

1783 

1.9 

595 

2.4 

324 

0.9 

12 

1783 

6.1 

595 

7.6 

324 

4.3 

13 

1783 

11.5 

595 

12.9 

324 

9.6 

14 

1783 

18.6 

595 

17.8 

324 

14.8 

15 

1783 

21.6 

595 

21.3 

324 

18.2 

16 

1782 

23.5 

595 

23.2 

324 

20.7 

17 

1781 

24.8 

594 

24.1 

324 

21.6 

18 

1756 

26.1 

573 

26.0 

306 

22.9 

19 

1642 

27.3 

548 

27.4 

283 

23.7 

20 

1472 

28.1 

522 

28.9 

255 

23.9 

21 

1273 

28.4 

472 

30.7 

216 

26.4 

22 

1038 

29.4 

413 • 

31.2 

167 

29.9 

23 

874 

30.8 

370 

33.0 

127 

33.1 

24 

740 

32.3 

314 

32.2 

86 

32.6 

25 

663 

33.8 

275 

31.6 

56 

32.1 

26 

582 

33.7 

250 

32.4 



27 

512 

34.0 

222 

33.8 



28 

467 

34.5 

202 

33.7 



29 

432 

34.0 

179 

35.2 



30 

400 

34.5 

154 

33.8 



31 

378 

34.9 

135 

34.1 



32 

353 

33.7 

125 

32.0 



33 

322 

34.8 

114 

32.5 



34 

299 

35.5 

101 

33.7 



35 

277 

35.0 

95 

31.6 



36 

261 

35.2 

86 

29.1 



37 

233 

33.9 

81 

32.1 



38 

222 

33.8 

78 

32.1 

t 


39 

207 

34.3 

66 

31.8 



40 

191 

33.0 

60 

33.3 



41 

180 

31.1 

53 

35.8 




Table 20. Comparisons of data obtained by different interviewers, on total 

homosexual outlets 

Based on males of the college level. Includes pre-marital, extra-marital, and post- 
marital experience in the homosexual. 



data OBTAII^®D BY DIFFERENT 


interviewers 


"kE- MARITAL intercourse 

£OUC LEVEL 13+ 

I FIG. 19 I 


^5 20 


30 


• KINSEY 
POMEROY ‘ 


iNmcooRse with PRosTirures 

eooc LEVEL 0-0 -j ^ 

Fie. 20 


20 25 
AGE 


homosexual outlets 


EOOC LEVEL 13 + 


X X K MARTIN 




P mantal intercourse, intercourse with prostitutes, and homosexual outlets. 






142 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Comparisons of Frequency Data Obtained in Successive Periods 


GROUP 

PERIOD 

CASES 

TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

Age 11-15 








Total Outlet 

1938-42 

1113 

2 92 =fc 0.07 

2.46 

96.1 

3.04 =b 0.07 

2.58 


1943-46 

644 

3.05 0.14 

2.11 

93.9 

3.24 =i= 0.15 

2.30 

Masturbation 

1938-42 

1113 

2.25 ± 0.06 

1.82 

85.2 

2.64 ± 0.07 

2.18 


1943-46 

644 

2.36 ± 0.12 

1.46 

77.8 

3.03 ± 0.14 

2.17 

Nocturnal Emiss. 

1938-42 

1113 

0.29 0.02 

0.06 

61.8 

0.47 ± 0.03 

0 25 


1943-46 

644 

0.43 =fc 0.03 

0.15 

74.1 

0.58 =b 0.04 

0.31 

Pre-mar. Coitus 

1938-42 

1113 

0.11 ± 0.02 

0.00 

13.7 

0.81 =±= 0.10 

0.34 


1943-46 

644 

0 09 =t: 0.03 

0.00 

6.8 

1.27 ± 0.46 

0.17 

Homosexual 

1938-42 

1113 

0.11 =1= 0.01 

0.00 

23.4 

0.46 ± 0.06 

0.10 


1943-46 

644 

0.09 0.02 

0.00 

21.1 

0.43 ^ 0.06 

0.10 

Age 16-20 








Total Outlet 

1938-42 

1146 

2.64 0.06 

2.14 

99.7 

2.65 =fc 0 06 

2.15 


1943-46 

662 

3.05 =fc 0.12 

2.30 

99.7 

3 06 0.12 

2.31 

Masturbation 

1938-42 

1146 

1.59 0.05 

1.12 

89.4 

1.77 ± 0.05 

1.38 


1943-46 

662 

2.11 =•= 0.10 

1.33 

88.1 

2.40 =i= 0.11 

1.66 

Nocturnal Emiss. 

1938-42 

1146 

0 41 ± 0.02 

0.22 

91.3 

0.45 =fc 0.02 

0 26 


1943-46 

662 

0.47 =fc 0.03 

0.27 

91.2 

0.51 =fc 0.03 

0.31 

Pre-mar. Coitus 

1938-42 

1146 

0.31 =«= 0.03 

0.00 

41.9 

0.74 =fc 0.06 

0.28 


1943-46 

662 

0.26 =•= 0.04! 

0.00 

37.3 

0.70 =fc 0.11 

0.19 

Homosexual 

1938-42 

1146 

0.08 0.01 

0.00 

17.8 

0.47 =fc 0.07 

0.09 


1943-46 

662 

0.08 ± 0.02 

0.00 

14.8 

0.50 =fc 0.09 

0.09 

Age 21-25 








Total Outlet 

1938-42 

691 

2.58 ± 0.08 

1.99 

99.9 

2.58 =b 0.08 

2.00 


1943-46 

533 

2.64 ± 0.12 

1.79 

99.6 

2.65 =b 0.13 

1.80 

Masturbation 

1938-42 

691 

1.26 ± 0 05 

0.74 

86.3 

1.46 =t= 0.06 

0.91 


1943-46 

533 

1.43 0.09 

0.62 

86.3 

1.66 ± 0.10 

0.87 

Nocturnal Emiss. 

1938-42 

691 

0.40 =b 0.03 

0.19 

86.1 

0.46 =b 0.03 

0.26 


1943-46 

533 

0.39 i 0.03 

0.23 

87.1 

0.45 0.03 

0.29 

Pre-mar. Coitus 

1938-42 

691 

0.50 =fc 0.05 

0.02 

52.4 

0.95 =fc 0.08 

0.36 


1943-46 

533 

0.49 ± 0.06 

0.04 

57.4 

0.85 0.11 

0.29 

Homosexual 

1938-42 

691 

0.10 =1= 0.02 

0.00 

10.6 

0.95 0.17 

0.28 


1943-46 

533 

0.11 0.03 

0.00 

10.7 

1.05 ± 0.22 

0.34 


Table 21. Comparisons of data obtained in two successive four-year periods 

Comparing results obtained by the same interviewer (Kinsey) in the earlier half and in 
the later half of the study. All data from groups of college level (“educational level 13+”). 




VALIDITY OF THE DATA 


143 


the frequency data on all of the other items and the incidence data even 
on masturbation and pre-marital intercourse are very closely duplicated 
by the two interviewers. On the item which is most difficult to uncover, 
namely, the homosexual, the two interviewers secured almost exactly the 
same results. The differences in their findings on masturbation and pre- 
marital intercourse may be due to some selection of the subjects who have 
contributed the histories, or to real differences in the populations with 
which the two interviewers dealt; but this does not seem a sufficient expla- 
nation. On this point, further investigation needs to be made. 

5. The accumulative incidence curves (Chapter 3), derived from the 
data gathered by the three interviewers (Tables 17-20, Figures 16-21), 
provide a striking demonstration of the capacities of different investigators 
to secure similar results, even on such intangible material as must be dealt 
with in the study of human sex behavior. In most cases, the incidence data 
obtained by the several interviewers are so nearly in accord that the curves 
he precisely on top of each other. In those cases where there are differences, 
the general loci of the data are still confirmed, although there may be some 
question of the precise position of the fact between the two calculations, or 
to one or the other side of the extreme calculations. 

STABILITY OF TECHNIQUES 

The question involved here concerns the capacity of an interviewer to 
obtain uniform results over any long period of years. Is there a possibility 
that one’s methods of recording change, particularly in regard to evalua- 
tions of items that are not strictly measurable? Is there a possibihty that 
changes have entered in the methods of calculating from the raw data, 
since the first procedures were devised nine years ago? As a study of these 
problems, comparisons are shown in Table 21 between the data obtained 
by the senior author during the first four years of the research (1938-1942), 
and the data obtained by the same interviewer in the last four years (1943- 
1946). The two junior authors have not been involved in interviewing long 
enough to make such comparisons possible. Table 21 includes calculations 
on every group for which 300 or more cases were available from the inter- 
viewing done by the senior author in each and both of the periods. Com- 
parisons of the accumulative incidence curves for masturbation, nocturnal 
emissions, pre-marital intercourse, and the homosexual in the two periods 
are shown in Tables 22 and 23, and Figures 22-24. 

A study of the data leads to the following conclusions: 

1. The active incidence data are phenomenally close in the two succes- 
sive four-year samples. They leave no doubt of the general locus of the 
fact for every type of sexual behavior, and they even suggest that there can 
be considerable precision in determining these facts. 

a 



144 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Comparisons of Accumulative Incidence Data Obtained in Successive 

Periods 

Single Males, Educ. Level 13+ 


age 

masturbation 

nocturnal emissions 

histories taken 

histories taken 

histories taken 

histories taken 


1938- 

-1942 

1943- 

-1946 

1938- 

-1942 

1943- 

-1946 


Cases 

% with 

Cases 

% With 

Cases 

% with 

Cases 

%Wlth 



Exper. 


Exper. 


Exper. 


Exper. 

8 

1166 

0.0 

637 

0.0 

1163 

0.0 

636 

0.0 

9 

1166 

0.2 

637 

0.6 

1163 

0.0 

636 

0.0 

10 

1166 

1.7 

637 

3.5 

1163 

0.4 

636 

0.5 

11 

1166 

6.9 

637 

9.1 

1163 

2.1 

636 

3.1 

12 

1166 

26.5 

637 

25.3 

1163 

8.6 

636 

12.7 

13 

1166 

54.0 

637 

49.1 

1163 

21.5 

636 

33.5 

14 

1166 

74.3 

637 

68.6 

1163 

44.5 

636 

56.1 

15 

1166 

81.6 

637 

75.7 

1163 

62.8 

636 

71.7 

16 

1166 

84.8 

637 

81.6 

1163 

75.7 

636 

83.0 

17 

1165 

87.6 

637 

84.9 

1162 

85.2 

636 

88.5 

18 

1147 

89.7 

629 

87.0 

1144 

90.4 

628 

91.4 

19 

1047 

91.2 

612 

88.1 

1044 

92.4 

611 

93.1 

20 

878 

91.1 

594 

89.9 

875 

93.6 

593 

94.1 

21 

705 

92.1 

558 

91.0 

702 

94.6 

557 

94.6 

22 

502 

92.6 

503 

91.8 

501 

94.8 

502 

94.8 

23 

351 

91.5 

i 445 

92.8 

351 

94.3 

444 

94.8 

24 

256 

91.8 

385 

92.5 

256 

95.7 

384 

95.6 

25 

199 

93.0 

320 

92.2 

199 

95.0 

319 

96.2 

26 

154 

93.5 

256 

92.6 

154 

95.5 

255 

96.9 

27 

102 

94.1 

204 

93.1 

102 

95.1 

203 

96.6 

28 

> 68 

94.1 

163 

92.6 

68 

95.6 

162 

95.7 

29 

51 

92.2 

134 

94.0 

51 

96.1 

133 

95.5 

30 



107 

95.3 



106 

96.2 

31 



82 

93.9 



82 

95.1 

32 



72 

94.4 



72 

94.4 

33 



61 

93.4 



61 

95.1 


Table 22. Comparisons of data obtained at two different periods, on masturbation 
and nocturnal emissions 

Accumulative incidence data based on the pre-marital histories of males of the 
college level, taken by one interviewer (Kinsey) in two successive four-year periods. 




A^E 

Figures 22-24. Comparing accumulative incidence data obtained by one inter- 
viewer in successive four-year periods 

Data on nocturnal emissions, pre-marital intercourse, masturbation, and homosexual 
contacts. All calculations based on males of college level (13-1-). 

145 





146 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Comparisons of Accumulative Incidence Data Obtained in Successive 

Periods 

Single Males, Educ. Level 13+ 


age 

TOTAL 

PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

PRE-MARITAL HOMOSEXUAL 

OUTLETS 


histories taken 

HISTORIES TAKEN 

HISTORIES TAKEN 

HISTORIES TAKEN 


1938-1942 

1943- 

-1946 

1938- 

-1942 

1943- 

-1946 


Cases 

% with 

Cases 

%with 

Cases 

% with 

Cases 

% with 



Exper. 


Exper. 


Exper. 


]^per. 

8 

1167 

0.0 

637 

0.0 

1163 

0.0 

637 

0.0 

9 

1167 

0.0 

637 

0.0 

1163 

0.0 

637 

0.2 

10 

1167 

0.0 

637 

0.0 

1163 

0.2 

637 

1.1 

11 

1167 

0.3 

637 

0.0 

1163 

1.5 

637 

2.2 

12 

1167 

1.5 

637 

0.2 

1163 

5.9 

637 

5.7 

13 

1167 

4.5 

637 

1.7 

1163 

12.0 

637 

10.2 

14 

1167 

7.1 

637 

3.5 

1163 

19.3 

637 

17.1 

15 

1167 

13.4 

637 

6.8 

1163 

22.2 

637 

20.7 

16 

1167 

19.6 

637 

12.7 r 

1163 

24.6 

637 

21.5 

17 

1166 

28.6 

637 

19.2 

1162 

25.6 

637 

23.1 

18 

1148 

36.7 

629 

25.4 

1144 

27.0 

629 ; 

24.6 

19 

1048 

44.9 

612 

32.5 

1044 

28.3 

612 

25.5 

20 

879 

51.4 

594 

39.6 

875 

29.3 

594 

25.8 

21 

706 

55.0 

558 

44.4 

702 

29.6 

558 

26.7 

22 

503 

59.4 

503 

49.9 

501 

30.7 

503 

28.2 

23 

351 

59.3 

445 

54.4 

351 

32.8 

445 

29.2 

24 

256 

60.9 i 

385 

56.9 

256 

35.2 

385 

29.9 

25 

199 

64.3 

320 

63.4 

199 

40.7 

320 

31.3 

26 

154 

64.3 

256 

62.5 

154 

42.2 

256 

33.2 

27 

102 

64.7 

204 

62.3 

102 

40.2 

204 

33.8 

28 

68 

66.2 

163 

62.6 

68 

47.1 

163 

38.0 

29 

51 

66.7 

134 

64.9 

51 

45.1 

134 

38.8 

30 



107 

66.4 



107 

42.1 

31 



82 

68.3 



82 

46.3 

32 



72 

66.7 



72 

47.2 

33 



61 

67.2 



61 

45.9 


Table 23. Comparisons of data obtained at two different periods 

Accumulative incidence data based on histories of males of the college level, taken 
by one interviewer (Kinsey) in two successive four-year periods. Total pre-marital 
intercourse includes relations with companions and with prostitutes. 




VALIDITY OF THE DATA 


147 


2. The accumulative incidence data are so nearly identical for the two 
different periods that it is highly improbable that two groups obtained in 
the same period would ever compare more closely. There is practically 
identity in regard to masturbation, nocturnal emissions, and the homo- 
sexual. The curves for pre-marital intercourse are about a year and a half 
apart during most of their rise, but reach nearly identical levels. 

3. The conformance of frequency data, in the successive four-year 
samples, is quite close. In general, the medians are closer than the means. 
Since the values of means are affected by a few high-rating individuals, as 
the values of medians are not, this greater constancy of the medians 
indicates that the frequencies of persons with unusually high rates of outlet 
vary in successive samples, while the frequencies of the individuals in the 
mass of the population do not vary so much. This is obviously due to the 
fact that there are fewer extreme individuals and that they are not picked 
up in any process of sampling as regularly as the more average persons in 
the population. Only large samples can smooth out the effects of high- 
rating cases, 

4. Some of the differences that do exist between the successive four-year 
samples may be the product of instabihty in the techniques of recording 
and calculating data, but it is just as likely that they are due to actual differ- 
ences in the samples which have been involved. The comparisons in Table 
21 have been made for populations resulting from a five-way breakdown 
(sex, race, marital status, age, and educational level), but there are many 
other factors that can modify the picture for particular groups. 

5. The differences in calculations on successive four-year periods are not 
consistently higher nor lower. This means that no consistent bias has 
entered into the processes of the study. 

6. There was, inevitably, some experimentation and some trial and 
error in developing the techniques of interviewing and in the manipulation 
of the data in the early years of the research. However, the errors that may 
have entered in this way do not appear to have been so large that the earlier 
histories need be eliminated from calculations which are based on the 
total body of histories. Throughout the present volume the data from the 
two four-year periods have, of course, been combined in all of the calcula- 
tions, and the consequent statistics may (or may not) be nearer the realities 
of behavior than either of the sets of calculations shown in Table 21. 

7. The comparisons in Tables 21-23 seem to indicate that methods of 
securing subjects, proficiency in interviewing, skill in using the code in 
which the data are recorded, and calculations and judgments which the 
data undergo in their statistical treatment, can be maintained at such uni- 
form levels as many persons would have considered impossible in a case 
history study which is hable to error from so many sources, and which 
deals with as taboo a subject as sex. 



148 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


IMMEDIATE VERSUS REMOTE RECALL 

Any consideration of the validity of case history data involves the ques- 
tion of the relative accuracy of immediate memory versus remote recall. 
Does the subject, in an interview, give a more accurate record of his more 
recent or of his more remote activities? Since the subjects were of various 
ages when they contributed their histories, the data shown for any partic- 
ular age period have been obtained partly from the more immediate recall 
of younger persons, and partly from the more remote — sometimes the 
quite remote — memory of older persons. Can data obtained by such differ- 
ent processes fairly be added together? 

It has not been possible to undertake any study which would go into all 
of the complexities of this situation, although we have some measurements 
toward such a study. In the course of the interviewing, we have acquired 
certain impressions that may be worth recording in anticipation of the 
time when there will be enough material to make more precise determina- 
tions. These impressions cover the following points: 

1. More recent events seem, in general, to be recalled more easily, and 
more remote events are recalled with greater difficulty. This seems reason- 
able enough, but it proves nothing concerning the validity of the recall. It 
seems reasonable to believe that more immediate and more easily recalled 
events would be reported with greater accuracy, but there are at least 
certain circumstances where that is clearly not so. 

2. Pre-adolescent children, as young as three or four years of age, are 
ordinarily capable enough of recalling very immediate events, but often 
fail to recall activities and knowledge acquired only a few months or a year 
or two before. How much of this forgetting is a simple lapse of memory, 
and how much is psychologic blockage, is not readily determined. The 
psychoanalysts are undoubtedly correct in seeing considerable significance 
in the sometimes deliberate but more often unconscious repressions that 
develop in these early years, but they do not sufficiently allow for the simple 
failures of memory which seem sufficient explanation of some of the inade- 
quacies of recall among younger subjects. There is no doubt that the ana- 
lysts are correct in believing that more of this early experience is lost to 
memory than of the experience of any other segment of the life cycle. 

3. Older persons seem to recall remote events, in many cases in minute 
detail, while forgetting what happened in recent weeks. This is rather 
generally accepted, and our own experience seems to confirm this. There 
are some psychologic studies that show the poor quality of the immediate 
recall of the aged (Thorndike 1928, Gilbert 1941), but apparently no pre- 
cise studies on the validity of their remote recall. 

4. While the quality of memory may show some degree of correlation 
with intelligence and with the extent of the individual’s formal education, 



VALIDITY OF THE DATA 


149 


there appears to be considerable accuracy of memory among some less 
intelligent and many poorly educated persons. Illiterate persons may 
remember such an amazing amount of detail about dates, names, and 
places, as is rarely found among educated persons whose minds are con- 
tinually preoccupied with what they read in newspapers, magazines, and 
books. We still need precise measures of the accuracy of memory among 
these lower levels, but the data secured on the histories from such persons 
show consistencies in the chronology that are often remarkable. On the 
other hand, some professionally trained persons, for some reason still to 
be analyzed, may be much confused in attempting to construct a chronol- 
ogy of their own activities, and the most extreme and absurd disparities 
secured on any of our re-takes have come from graduate students and 
university instructors who had especial interest in the research and were 
doing their best to cooperate. 

5. As usual, incidence figures are more accurate than frequency data. 
Estimates of average frequencies are especially difficult for children, for 
individuals of low mentality, and for most poorly educated individuals. 
Frequencies are more difficult to estimate when they concern remote 
periods of time. 

6. The possibly greater accuracy of recent memory is at least partially 
offset by the greater extent of cover-up on recent events. Legal statutes of 
limitation are in line with the general human tendency to forgive something 
that is more remote, while reacting violently to more recent happenings. 
Consequently many subjects in a case history study will admit participation 
in the more taboo sexual activities at some time in the past, while insisting 
that such activities are no part of their current histories, or that the fre- 
quencies are now very much reduced. An undue number of persons have 
discontinued masturbation, pre-marital coitus, extra-marital coitus, mouth- 
genital contacts, homosexual activities, prostitution, or animal intercourse 
the year before they contributed their Mstories — or a few months or even 
weeks before! Re-takes have subsequently shown that the year of the first 
history was in actuality involved, although the activities are again supposed 
to have terminated before the date of the re-take. 

7. Certain items are minimized, certain items played up, depending upon 
the immediate mental state of the subject. Re-takes, especially series of 
re-takes on the same person, and histories which can be compared with 
the precise records of a diary, show that these reactions fluctuate, rather 
than erring always in the same direction. For instance, the pre-marital 
heterosexual experiences which were reported on the first history may be 
minimized on the re-takes, while the report of the homosexual experience 
may be extended. A second re-take on the same individual may play up 
the heterosexual and minimize the homosexual experience, especially if 
the subject is now in the army and conscious of official attitudes on that 



150 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


subject. Six months after being released from the army, the homosexual 
record may again be obtained in something like the form which was 
reported on the earlier histories. 

The generalization to be drawn from these several impressions is that the 
memory of more recent events may be more accurate (except in the aged), 
but its accuracy is more or less offset by a considerable amount of cover-up 
on more immediate activities. What is the final effect on the quality of the 
individual record, and on the averages calculated for whole groups of 
individuals? The quality of the individual record is the clinician’s constant 
problem, but one on which we, unfortunately, can contribute nothing more 
at this time. That the individual record is not wholly specious, the data 
elsewhere in this chapter, especially the data on re-takes, definitely show. 
We shall hope to make still further studies on this point as soon as we 
have the large series of histories which such a study will demand. 

OLDER VERSUS YOUNGER GENERATIONS 

As a further comparison of more immediate versus remote recall, the 
data in Chapter 1 1 bear on this point. The tables and charts in that chapter 
compare the younger half with the older half of the population which has 
entered into this study. The younger half is, of course, reporting more 
immediate experience. The older half, averaging about 22 years older than 
the first group, is recalling its early years from a distance which is that much 
more remote. While these data were originally calculated to study possible 
changes in behavior in successive generations, they also provide one more 
test of memory of recent versus more remote events. An examination of 
Chapter 11 will show that the incidence data for the two groups are, in 
most cases, almost precise duplicates. The frequency data are further apart 
in some groups but, again, precise duplicates in other groups. The factors 
involved in these diverse results are discussed in Chapter 11. 

CONCLUSIONS 

Throughout the whole period of this study, a variety of techniques has 
been employed to test the effectiveness of the methods of interviewing, the 
validity of the data which are obtained in an interview, and the appro- 
priateness of the statistical techniques by which the data have been manipu- 
lated. It is unfortunate that these studies of method are not yet complete 
and, indeed, that they could not have been completed before the central 
problems of the research were laid out and their study initiated. But such 
investigations of method often demand more material than is needed to 
solve the problems which are central to the main study. In order to deter- 
mine the necessary size of sample, for instance, it has been necessary to 
study some samples that were larger than those that have ultimately proved 
adequate. In order to compare results obtained by different interviewers, it 



VALIDITY OF THE DATA 151 

is necessary that each interviewer have secured a sample as large as may be 
needed in the combined sample from all the interviewers. 

Moreover, finished studies of method have to be made with populations 
which are homogeneous for at least the major items on the twelve-way 
breakdown used in this study ; and the methodological investigations which 
are reported here in Chapters 3 and 4 are still restricted to a certain few 
segments of the population, chiefly to the college groups, because we do 
not yet have enough histories from other groups to make studies there. It 
will be highly important to secure some measure of the differences in 
validity of the data obtained from persons of different ages, of different 
educational and social levels, of different mental capacity, and different in 
still other respects. To that end, we shall continue this study of method as 
this research progresses. 

The materials now in hand seem to justify the following conclusions 
concerning the validity of data obtained through personal interviewing 
in a case history study: 

1. The accuracy varies considerably with different individuals. The 
inaccuracies are the product of simple forgetting, the deliberate or uncon- 
scious suppression of memory, and deliberate cover-up. Definite allowances 
must be made for such errors on individual histories obtained in the con- 
centrated, relatively short interviews which have been used in the present 
investigation. Careful studies of J:he effectiveness of other types of inter- 
viewing — for instance, of the effectiveness of the psychoanalytic technique, 
with its two hundred or more hours spent on each individual — have never 
been made; and it is as yet impossible to make comparisons of the relative 
effectiveness of the methods used in the present study and in these other 
types of interviewing. It is not sufficient to depend upon the optimistic 
claims of each cUnician for his own technique; and we hope, in time, to 
make joint studies with some other groups that will throw more light on 
these problems of interviewing. 

2. In the present study, the validity of the individual histories varies 
with particular items and for different segments of the population. On the 
whole, the accuracy of the individual history is far greater than might have 
been expected, with correlation coefficients ranging above 0.7 in most 
cases, and percents of identical responses ranging between 75 and 99 on 
particular items. There are some low correlations which are highly signifi- 
cant because they give some insight into the factors which are responsible 
for errors and falsifications in reporting. 

3. It is unfortunate that there is, as yet, insufficient experience to allow 
us to identify, in more than a part of the cases, those individuals who are 
least accurate in their reporting. The clinician needs this information, for 
he is most often concerned with the validity of the individual history, and 



152 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


less often with the vahdity of the data from any group of individuals. But 
further studies will need to be made before we are able to say how one can 
identify those particular individuals who are more accurate, and those who 
are less accurate as reporters of the events that have occurred in their lives. 

4. The accuracy of the averages calculated for whole groups of individ- 
uals is definitely higher than the accuracy of the individual histories, as 
statisticians will readily understand. Where there is no bias which accumu- 
lates errors in a particular direction, errors on one side will compensate 
for errors on the other side and the averages come nearer the fact, as the 
various tests in this chapter indicate. 

5. For all types of sexual activity, in all segments of the population, the 
incidence data are more accurate than the frequency data. The incidence 
data are the most accurate of all. The actualities must lie within 1 to 10 per 
cent, plus or minus, of the published incidence figures, and within 1 to 5 
per ceni of most of them. In regard to nearly all types of sexual activity 
there has undoubtedly been some cover-up, and the actual incidences are 
probably higher than the published figures. This applies especially to 
masturbation at lower social levels, to pre-marital and extra-marital inter- 
course at upper levels, and to the homosexual and animal intercourse at 
all levels. There is httle likelihood that the calculated figures on any of these 
items are too high. There are abundant social reasons why an individual 
should deny or minimize the frequency of any activity which is taboo and, 
in the last analysis, all sexual activities except marital intercourse may, in 
some social groups, fall under that head. 

6. Data concerning such individual and social statistics as age, educa- 
tion, events concerned with marriage, parents, siblings, etc., are the next 
most accurate. The averages for whole groups are so close to the averages 
obtained by direct observation that they may be accepted as precise state- 
ments of fact, although they are not so dependable on individual histories. 

7. The frequency data are much less accurate than the incidence data. 
On individual histories they may be removed by as much as 50 per cent 
from the reality. Nevertheless, mean frequencies and median frequencies 
calculated for whole groups will not need more than a 5 to 10 per cent 
allowance, plus or minus. Since differences in frequencies of sexual 
behavior in different segments of the population may amount to some- 
thing between 100 and 800 per cent, the comparisons will not be materially 
affected by the necessary corrections. The statements made concerning 
mean and median frequencies for whole groups are much more accurate 
than the best trained individual could be in reporting his own individual 
frequencies. 

8. The least accurate data are those that concern an individual’s first 
knowledge of an event. This is true of the individual histories, but the 



VALIDITY OF THE DATA 


153 


averages calculated for whole groups are still reliable. The inaccuracies 
on these points are obviously dependent upon the indefinite nature of the 
educational processes by which one finally becomes conscious of the fact 
that he has definite knowledge on some subject. On individual histories an 
allowance of 2,5 years, plus or minus, should be made on the reported 
data. The means calculated for whole groups will not need more than a 4 
or 5 per cent correction, plus or minus, on these items. 

9. Again it should be emphasized that most of these calculations of 
validity have been based on the college segment of the population, which 
is the only group represented now by large enough series to warrant such 
examination. Comparable studies are needed to determine the validity of 
the data obtained from other segments of the population, and we plan to 
undertake such studies as soon as sufficient re-takes, pairs of spouses, 
series from different interviewers, etc., are available. Preliminary examina- 
tions of data from lower social levels suggest that variations in the quality 
of such reports are wider than the variations among college males. Con- 
sequently quite large series will be needed before it will be possible to make 
satisfactory validity studies on the more poorly educated groups. 


Throughout the remainder of this volume, the raw data and the calcula- 
tions based on the raw data are treated with a precision that must not be 
misunderstood by the statistically inexperienced reader. It has not been 
practical to carry this warning in Svery paragraph of every chapter. Neither 
has it been possible to qualify every individual statistic, as every statistic 
in any study of the human animal should be quahfied. For the remainder 
of the volume it should, therefore, be recognized that the data are probably 
fair approximations, but only approximations of the fact. 




Part II 


FACTORS AFFECTING SEXUAL OUTLET 


CHAPTER 5. 
CHAPTER 6. 
CHAPTER 7. 
CHAPTER 8. 
CHAPTER 9. 
CHAPTER 10. 
CHAPTER 11. 
CHAPTER 12. 
CHAPTER 13. 


EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 

TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 

AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 

MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 

AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 

SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 

STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 

RLnEIAL-URBAN BACKGROUND, AND SEXUAL OUTLET 

RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 




Chapter 5 


EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 

The present volume is concerned, for the most part, with the record of 
the frequency and sources of sexual outlet in the biologically mature male, 
i.e.^ in the adolescent and older male. This chapter, however, will discuss 
the nature of sexual response, and will show something of the origins of 
adult behavior in the activities of the younger, pre-adolescent boy. 

The sexual activity of an individual may involve a variety of experiences, 
a portion of which may culminate in the event which is known as orgasm 
or sexual climax. There are six chief sources of sexual climax. There is 
self stimulation (masturbation), nocturnal dreaming to the point of climax, 
heterosexual petting to climax (without intercourse), true heterosexual 
intercourse, homosexual intercourse, and contact with animals of other 
species. There are still other possible sources of orgasm, but they are rare 
and never constitute a significant fraction of the outlet for any large seg- 
ment of the population. 

EROTIC AROUSAL AND ORGASM 

Sexual contacts in the adolescent or adult male almost always involve 
physiologic disturbance which is recognizable as “erotic arousal.” This 
is also true of much pre-adolescent activity, although some of the sex play 
of younger children seems to be devoid of erotic content. Pre-adolescent 
sexual stimulation is much more common among younger boys than it is 
among younger girls. Many younger females and, for that matter, a cer- 
tain portion of the older and married female population, may engage in 
such specifically sexual activities as petting and even intercourse without 
discernible erotic reaction. 

Erotic arousal is a material phenomenon which involves an extended 
series of physical, physiologic, and psychologic changes. Many of these 
could be subjected to precise instrumental measurement if objectivity 
among scientists and public respect for scientific research allowed such 
laboratory investigation. In the higher mammals, including the human, 
tactile stimulation is the chief mechanical source of arousal; but the higher 
mammal, especially the human, soon becomes so conditioned by his 
experience, or by the vicariously shared experiences of others, that psy- 
chologic stimulation becomes the major source of arousal for many an 
older person, especially if he is educated and his mental capacities are well 

157 



158 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


trained. There is an occasional individual who comes to climax through 
psychologic stimulation alone. 

Erotic stimulation, whatever its source, effects a series of physiologic 
changes which, as far as we yet know, appear to involve adrenal secretion, 
typically autonomic reactions, increased pulse rate, increased blood pres- 
sure, an increase in peripheral circulation and a consequent rise in the 
surface temperature of the body; a flow of blood into such distensible 
organs as the eyes, the lips, the lobes of the ears, the nipples of the breast, 
the penis of the male, and the clitoris, the genital labia and the vaginal 
walls of the female; a partial but often considerable loss of perceptive 
capacity (sight, hearing, touch, taste, smell) ; an increase in so-called nerv- 
ous tension, some degree of rigidity of some part or of the whole of the 
body at the moment of maximum tension; and then a sudden release which 
produces local spasms or more extensive or all-consuming convulsions. 
The moment of sudden release is the point commonly recognized among 
biologists as orgasm. 

The person involved in a sexual situation may be more or less conscious 
of some of the physiologic changes which occur although, unless he is 
scientifically trained, much of what is happening escapes his comprehen- 
sion. Self observation may be especially inadequate because of tlie con- 
siderable (and usually unrecognized) loss of sensory capacities during 
maximum arousal. The subject’s awareness of the situation is summed up 
in his statement that he is “emotionally aroused”; but the material sources 
of the emotional disturbances are rarely recognized, either by laymen or 
by scientists, both of whom are inclined to think in terms of passion, a 
sexual impulse, a natural drive, or a libido which partakes of the mystic 
more than it does of solid anatomy and physiologic function. 

The most important consequence of sexual orgasm is the abrupt release 
of the extreme tension which preceded the event and the rather sudden 
return to a normal or subnormal physiologic state after the event. In the 
mature male, ejaculation of the liquid secretions of the prostate and 
seminal vesicles, through the urethra of the penis, is a usual consequence 
of the convulsions produced by orgasm in those particular organs; and 
such ejaculation usually provides the most ready proof that the individual 
has passed through chmax. But orgasm may occur without the emission 
of semen. This latter situation is, of course, the rule when orgasm 
occurs among pre-adolescent males and among females. It also occurs 
among a few adult males (11 out of 4,102 adult males in our histories) 
who either are afflicted with ejaculatory impotence (6 cases : 2 operative, 2 
hormonal, 1 after severe illness, 1 in an apparently normal individual), or 
who deliberately constrict their genital muscles (5 cases) in the contra- 
ceptive technique which is known as coitus reservatus. These males experi- 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 159 

ence real orgasm, which they have no difficulty in recognizing, even if it is 
without ejaculation. 

Among pre-adolescent boys, however, and among younger females, 
orgasm is not so readily recognized, partly because of the lack of an ejacu- 
late, and partly because the inexperienced individual is without a back- 
ground from which to judge the event. In the younger boy there is no 
ejaculate because the prostate and seminal vesicles are not yet functionally 
developed, and in the female those glands are rudimentary and never 
develop. Nevertheless, erotic arousal and orgasm where it occurs among 
younger boys and among females appears to involve the same sequence of 
physiologic events that has been described for the older, ejaculating males; 
and many of the younger boys and most of the older females who have 
contributed to the present study have been able to supply apparently 
reliable records of such experience. 

While climax is thus clearly possible without ejaculation, it is doubtful 
if ejaculation can ordinarily occur without a preceding climax. There are 
some (the implication is in Reich 1942; also in Wolfe 1942) who consider 
that this latter situation does occur, and not infrequently, among some males. 
Subjects are quoted who have had erections and who have ejaculated 
under conditions which they insist brought them no satisfaction. But in 
our histories there are many subjects who make similar statements. There 
are husbands who report unsatisfactory intercourse with unresponsive 
wives; there are other males who so characterize their intercourse with 
prostitutes ; and there are males who insist that they are “not at all aroused” 
in the stray homosexual relations which they have. Most of these individ- 
uals do, however, erect and ejaculate in such situations; and these reports 
probably amount to little more than records of varying degrees of physio- 
logic disturbance during arousal and orgasm; or they are merely evidence 
of minimal psychic components with good enough physical responses, or, 
sometimes, of good enough psychic reactions that are inhibited, disguised, 
or rationahzed in order to evade moral responsibility for socially taboo 
behavior. To repeat: the biologist thinks of ejaculation as the product of 
the convulsions which result from the physiologic event commonly known 
as orgasm; and, except under laboratory experimental conditions (as in 
the direct, electrical stimulation of erectile centers in the spinal cord) it is 
difficult to understand what mechanisms could produce ejaculation without 
a precedent orgasm. The confusion in the literature seems to be the result 
of making the term orgasm and orgastic pleasure synonymous. It is, of 
course, quite possible to recognize many degrees of physiologic change, 
and many degrees of satisfaction among sexual experiences, and there are 
admittedly occasions when there is little pleasure accompanying an ejacu- 
lation. But we have no statistics on the frequencies of physiologic differ- 
ences, or of the various degrees of satisfaction, and, in the present study, all 



160 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


cases of ejaculation have been taken as evidence of orgasm, without regard 
to the different levels at which the orgasms have occurred. 

Behavior during orgasm varies considerably with different individuals 
just as all other aspects of sexual behavior differ in any population (Chap- 
ter 6). The descriptions of orgasm in clinical texts, marriage manuals, and 
other literature are, however, remarkably uniform, partly because of each 
author’s limited experience, and chiefly because of his failure to search for 
variation in securing data from clinical subjects. In consequence, there has 
been little comprehension of the complexity of the problem involved in 
advising different persons about their sexual adjustments, and about 
sexual techniques in marriage. There is great variety among adult males ; 
and, it is interesting to note, there is as great variety and the same sort of 
variety among pre-adolescent boys. One of our subjects, who has had con- 
tacts with certain males over long periods of years (as many as sixteen 
years in some cases), from their early pre-adolescence into their late teens 
and twenties, states that the particular type of orgasm experienced by a 
younger boy remains as his particular type into his adult years. The varia- 
tion in pattern of orgastic response thus seems to depend, at least to some 
degree (and in the limited number of cases so far studied), on inherent 
differences in the biologic constitution of different individuals. 

Our several thousand histories have included considerable detail on the 
nature of orgasm; and these data, togetlj^er with the records supplied by 
some older subjects who have had sexual contacts with younger boys, 
provide material for describing the different sorts of reactions which may 
occur. In the pre-adolescent, orgasm is, of course, without ejaculation of 
semen. In the descriptions which follow, the data supplied by adult 
observers for 196 pre-adolescent boys are the sources of the percentage 
figures indicating the frequency of each type of orgasm among such young 
males. While six types are listed, it should be understood that all gradations 
occur between the situations which are herewith described. 

1. Reactions primarily genital: Little 'or no evidence of body tension; 
orgasm reached suddenly with little or no build-up ; penis becomes more 
rigid and may be involved in mild throbs, or throbs may be limited to 
urethra alone; semen (in the adult) seeps from urethra without forcible 
ejaculation; climax passes with minor after-effects. A fifth (22%) of the 
pre-adolescent cases on which there are sufficient data belong here, and 
probably an even higher proportion of older males. 

2. Some body tension: Usually involving a tension or twitching of one 
or both legs, of the mouth, of the arms, or of other particular parts of the 
body. A gradual build-up to a climax which involves rigidity of the whole 
body and some throbbing of the penis; orgasm with a few spasms but little 
after-effect. This is the most common type of orgasm, involving nearly 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


161 


half (45%) of the pre-adolescent males, and perhaps a corresponding 
number of adult males. 

3. Extreme tension with violent convulsion: Often involving the sud- 
den heaving and jerking of the whole body. Descriptions supplied by 
several subjects indicate that the legs often become rigid, with muscles 
knotted and toes pointed, muscles of abdomen contracted and hard, 
shoulders and lieck stiff and often bent forward, breath held or gasping, 
eyes staring or tightly closed, hands grasping, mouth distorted, sometimes 
with tongue protruding; whole body or parts of it spasmodically twitching, 
sometimes synchronously with throbs or violent jerking of the penis. The 
individual may have some, but little, control of these involuntary reactions. 
A gradual, and sometimes prolonged, build-up to orgasm, which involves 
still more violent convulsions of the whole body; heavy breathing, groan- 
ing, sobbing, or more violent cries, sometimes with an abundance of tears 
(especially among younger children), the orgasm or ejaculation often 
extended, in some individuals involving several minutes (in one case up to 
five minutes) of recurrent spasm. After-effects not necessarily more marked 
than with other types of orgasm, and the individual is often capable of 
participating in a second or further experience. About one sixth (17%) of 
the pre-adolescent boys, a smaller percentage of adult males. 

4. As in either type 1 or 2; but with hysterical laughing, talking, 
sadistic or masochistic reactioi^, rapid motions (whether in masturbation 
or in intercourse), culminating in more or less frenzied movements which 
are continued through the orgasm. A small percentage (5%) of either pre- 
adolescent or adult males. 

5. As in any of the above; but culminating in extreme trembling, 
collapse, loss of color, and sometimes fainting of subject. Sometimes 
happens only in the boy’s first experience, occasionally occurs throughout 
the life of an individual. Regular in only a few (3%) of the pre-adolescent 
or adult males. Such complete collapse is more common and better known 
among females. 

6. Pained or frightened at approach of orgasm. The genitalia of many 
adult males become hypersensitive immediately at and after orgasm, and 
some males suffer excruciating pain and may scream if movement is con- 
tinued or the penis even touched. The males in the present group become 
similarly hypersensitive before the arrival of actual orgasm, will fight away 
from the partner and may make violent attempts to avoid climax, although 
they derive definite pleasure from the situation. Such individuals quickly 
return to complete the experience, or to have a second experience if the 
first was complete. About 8 per cent of the younger boys are involved here, 
but it is a smaller percentage of older boys and adults which continues 
these reactions throughout life. 



162 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Ages Involved, Pre-adolescent Sex Play 


AGE 

TOTAL 

POPULATION, U. 

S. CORRECTION 

EDUC. LEVEL 0-8 

Cases 

Any 

Sex 

Play 

% 

Het- 

ero- 

sexual 

% 

Co- 

itus 

% 

Homo- 

sex- 

ual 

0/ 

/o 

Cases 

Any 

Sex 

Play 

% 

[ 

Het- 

ero- 

sexual 

/o 

Co- 

itus 

7o 

Homo- 

sex- 

ual 

y 

/o 

5 

4321 

9.8 

6.5 

2.6 

5.7 

822 

7.9 

4.7 

2.8 

5.7 

6 

4321 

15.6 

10.1 

4.4 

10.0 

821 

13.4 

7.8 

4.8 

10.4 

7 

4320 

20.0 

13.2 

6.7 

13.5 

819 

17.6 

11.2 

7.4 

14.0 

8 

4316 

26.9 

17.0 

8.7 

18.4 

820 

25.7 

16.0 

10.9 

20.1 

9 

4302 

28.5 

16.7 

8.7 

21.4 

817 

28.4 

17.0 

11.5 

22.9 

10 

4216 

36.6 

20.8 

11.2 

27.5 

812 

36.3 

21.4 

15.0 

28.6 

11 

3933 

37.4 

22.0 

12.3 

27.9 

784 

36.7 

21.7 

15.2 

29.0 

12 

2975 

38.8 

22.7 

12.8 

29.4 

677 

37.4 

21.9 

14.9 

29.8 

13 

1610 

35.0 

20.2 

12.9 

26.5 

491 

33.4 

18.1 

13.2 

26.7 

14 

424 

33.6 

17.8 

9.3 

27.6 

181 

36.5 

16.6 

11.6 

29.8 

15 

112 

24.1 

16.0 

5.0 

19.9 

40 

17.5 

7.5 

5.0 

15.0 



EDUC. LEVEL 

9-12 


r 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13+ 


AGE 


Any 

Het- 

Co- 

Homo- 


Any 

Het- 

Co- 

Homo- 


Cases 

Sex 

Play 

0/ 

/o 1 

ero- 

sexual 

% 

itus 

% 

sex- 

ual 

/o 

Cases 

Sex 

Play 

% 

ero- 

sexual 

y 

/o 

itus 

% 

sex- 

ual 

% 

5 

637 

9.7 

6.6 

2.8 

5.2 

2862 

14.0 

10.2 

1.5 

7.3 

6 

637 

16.5 

11.0 

4.6 

9.9 

2863 

16.5 

11.3 

2.4 

9.8 

7 

638 

21.8 

14.7 

7.4 

13.6 

2863 

18.5 

11.7 

2.8 

11.8 

8 

637 

28.1 

18.2 

8.8 

17.7 

2859 

24.8 

14.8 

3.5 

17.1 

9 

634 

29.5 

17.4 

8.4 

21.3 

2851 

24.6 

13.2 

3.5 

18.2 

10 

623 

38.2 

21.8 

10.9 

27.8 

2781 

31.4 

15.7 

4.2 

24.2 

11 

593 

39.6 

24.5 

13.0 

28.2 

2556 

30.2 

13.3 

3.7 

24.5 

12 

467 

41.5 

25.9 

14.1 

30.0 

1831 

31.4 

12.3 

3.2 

26.3 

13 

270 

38.5 

24.1 

15.2 

27.8 

849 

25.3 

10.0 

3.5 

21.4 

14 

59 

35.6 

22.0 

10.2 

28.8 

184 

19.6 

4.3 

1.1 

18.5 


Table 24. Ages involved in pre-adolescent sex play 

“Educ. level 0-8” are the males who never go beyond grade school. “9-12” are the 
males who enter high school but never go beyond. *T3-f-” are the males who will 
ultimately go to college. 




EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


163 


PRE-ADOLESCENT SEX PLAY 

It has been assumed that the development of sexual attitudes and the 
first overt sexual activities occur in the early history of the infant, but there 
have been few specific data available. Recently we have begun the accumu- 
lation of information through conferences with quite young children and 
with their parents ; and in addition we now have material obtained by some 
of our subjects through the direct observation of infants and of older pre- 
adolescents. These histories emphasize the early development of the atti- 
tudes which largely determine the subsequent patterns of adult sexual 
behavior; but this material must be analyzed in a later volume, after we 
have accumulated a great many more specific data. For the time being we 
can report only on the specifically genital play and overt socio-sexual 
behavior which occurs before adolescence. 

We are not in a position to discuss the developing child's more generalized 
sensory responses which may be sexual, but which are not so specific as 
genital activities are. Freud and the psychoanalysts contend that all tactile 
stimulation and response are basically sexual, and there seems considerable 
justification for this thesis, in view of the tactile origin of so much of the 
mammalian stimulation. This, however, involves a considerable extension 
of both the everyday and scientific meanings of the term sexual, and we are 
not now concerned with recording every occasion on which a babe brings 
two parts of its body into juxtaposition, every time it scratches its ear or its 
genitalia, nor every occasion oif which it sucks its thumb. If all such acts 
are to be interpreted as masturbatory, it is, of course, a simple matter to 
conclude that masturbation and early sexual activity are universal phenom- 
ena; but it is still to be shown that these elemental tactile experiences 
have anything to do with the development of the sexual behavior of the 
adult. There is now a fair list of significant and in many cases observational 
studies of this “pre-genital” level of reaction among infants and young 
children (Bell 1902, Blanton 1917, Hattendorf 1932, Isaacs 1933, Dudycha 
1933, Halverson 1938, 1940, Campbell 1939, Conn 1939, 1940, Levy 1940. 
See Sears 1943 for a summary). 

Adult behavior is more obviously a product of the specifically genital 
play which is found among children, and on which we can now provide a 
statistical record. Our own interviews with children younger than five, and 
observations made by parents and others who have been subjects in this 
study, indicate that hugging and kissing are usual in the activity of the very 
young child, and that self manipulation of genitalia, the exhibition of 
genitalia, the exploration of the genitalia of other children, and some 
manual and occasionally oral manipulation of the genitalia of other 
children occur in the two- to five-year olds more frequently than older 
persons ordinarily remember from their own histories. Much of this 
earliest sex play appears to be purely exploratory, animated by curiosity. 



164 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


and as devoid of erotic content as boxing, or wrestling, or other non-sexual 
physical contacts among older persons. Nevertheless, at a very early age 
the child learns that there are social values attached to these activities, and 
his emotional excitation while engaged in such play must involve reactions 
to the mysterious, to the forbidden, and to the socially dangerous perform- 
ance, as often as it involves true erotic response (Sears 1943). Some of the 
play in the younger boy occurs without erection, but some of it brings 
erection and may culminate in true orgasm. 

In pre-adolescent and early adolescent boys, erection and orgasm are 
easily induced. They are more easily induced than in older males. Erection 
may occur immediately after birth and, as many observant mothers (and 
few scientists) know, it is practically a daily matter for all small boys, from 
earliest infancy and up in age (Halverson 1940). Slight physical stimulation 
of the genitalia, general body tensions, and generalized emotional situations 
bring immediate erection, even when there is no specifically sexual situation 
involved. The very generalized nature of the response becomes evident 
when one accumulates a list of the apparently non-sexual stimuli which 
bring erection. Ramsey (1943) has published such a list gathered from a 
group of 291 younger boys which he had interviewed, and his histories 
provide part of the data which we have used in the present volume. A 
complete tabulation, based on the total sample now available on all cases, 
is as follows : 


NON-SEXUAL SOURCES OF EIK>TIC RESPONSE 
AMONG PRE-ADOLESCENT AND YOUNGER ADOLESCENT BOYS 


Chiefly Physical 


Sitting in class 

Airplane rides 

Friction with clothing 

A sudden change in environment 

Taking a shower 

Sitting in church 

Punishment 

Motion of car or bus 

Accidents 

A skidding car 

Electric shock 

Sitting in warm sand 

Fast elevator rides 

Urinating 

Carnival rides, Ferris wheel 

Boxing and wrestling 

Fast sled riding 

High dives 

Fast bicycle riding 

Riding horseback 

Fast car driving 

Swimming 

Skiing 



Chiefly Emotional 

Being scared 

Big fires 

Fear of a house intruder 

Setting a field afire 

Near accidents 

Hearing revolver shot 

Being late to school 

Anger 

Reciting before a class 

Watching exciting games 

Asked to go front in class 

Playing in exciting games 

Tests at school 

Marching soldiers 

Seeing a policeman 

War motion pictures 

Cops chasing him 

Other movies 

Getting home late 

Band music 

Receiving grade card 

Hearing “extra paper” called 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


165 


Chiefly Emotional (Confd) 

Harsh words Adventure stories 

Fear of punishment National anthem 

Being yelled at ^ Watching a stunting airplane 

Being alone at night Finding money 

Fear of a big boy Seeing name in print 

Playing musical solo Detective stories 

Losing balance on heights Running away from home 

Looking over edge of building Entering an empty house 

Falling from garage, etc. Nocturnal dreams of fighting, accidents, 

Long flight of stairs wild animals, falling from high places, 

giants, being chased, or frightened 

Among these younger boys, it is difficult to say what is an erotic response 
and what is a simple physical, or a generalized emotional situation. 

Specifically sexual situations to which the younger boys respond before 
adolescence include the following: 


SEXUAL SOURCES OF EROTIC RESPONSE AMONG 
212 PRE- ADOLESCENT BOYS 


Seeing females 

107 

Physical contact with females 

34 

Thinking about females 

104 

Love stories in books 

32 

Sex jokes 

104 

Seeing genitalia of other males 

29 

Sex pictures 

89 

Burlesque shows 

23 

Pictures of females 

76 

Seeing animals in coitus 

21 

Females in moving pictures 

55 

Dancing with females 

13 

Seeing self nude in mirror 

47 




The above table is based on the histories of 212 boys who were pre- 
adolescent at the time of interview. Since the questions were not systemat- 
ically put in all the pre-adolescent cases, the figures represent frequencies 
of answers in particular boys, and should not be taken as incidence figures 
for the population as a whole. 

The record suggests that the physiologic mechanism of any emotional 
response (anger, fright, pain, etc.) may be the basic mechanism of sexual 
response. Originally the pre-adolescent boy erects indiscriminately to the 
whole array of emotional situations, whether they be sexual or non-sexual 
in nature. By his late teens the male has been so conditioned that he rarely 
responds to anything except a direct physical stimulation of genitalia, or 
to psychic situations that are specifically sexual. In the still older male even 
physical stimulation is rarely effective unless accompanied by such a 
psychologic atmosphere. The picture is that of the psychosexual emerging 
from a much more generalized and basic physiologic capacity which 
becomes sexual, as an adult knows it, through experience and conditioning. 

The most specific activities among younger boys involve genital exhibi- 
tion and genital contacts with other children. Something more than a 
half (57%) of the older boys and adults recall some sort of pre-adolescent 
sex play. This figure is much higher than some other students have found 



166 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 



Duration of Pre-adolescent Sex Play 

NO. OF 

YEARS 

total population 

1 u.s. correction 

EDUC. LEVEL 

0-8 

EDUC. LEVEL 

9-12 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13 + 

IN- 

VOLVED 

% 

Cumu- 

lated 

/o 

% 

Cumu- 

lated 

/o 

7o 

Cumu- 

lated 

% 

% 

Cumu- 

lated 

% 


Any Sex Play 


1 

24.3 

100.0 

18.6 

100.0 

24.0 

100.0 

37.9 

100.0 

2 

17.9 

75.7 

11.8 

81.4 

20.1 

76.0 

22.8 

62.1 

3 

10.4 

57.8 

10.3 

69.6 

10.2 

55.9 

11.1 

39.3 

4 

11.2 

47.4 

11.5 

59.3 

11.6 

45.7 

8.8 

28.2 

5 

11.0 

36.2 

14.3 

47,8 

10.4 

34.1 

6.1 

19.4 

6 

9.5 

25.2 

12.0 

33.5 

9.2 

23.7 

5.3 

13.3 

7 

7.2 

15.7 

10.8 

21.5 

5.9 

14.5 

4.3 

8.0 

8 ! 

5.7 

8.5 

8.2 ’ 

10.7 

5.2 

8.6 

2.3 

3.7 

9+ 

2.8 

2.8 

2.5 

2.5 

3.4 

3.4 

1.4 

1.4 

Cases 

2749 

426 

404 

1919 

Mean 

Median 

3.72 years 

2.82 years 

4.32 years 

3 . 83 years 

3.63 years 

2.60 years 

2.76 years 

1.53 years 


Any Heterosexual Play 


1 

36.3 

100.0 

25.6 

100.0 

37.6 

100.0 

54.0 

100.0 

2 

15.8 

63.7 

12.2 

74.4 

17.1 

62.4 

18.6 

46.0 

3 

9.4 

47.9 

8.5 

62.2 

10.1 

45.3 

8.7 

27.4 

4 

10.2 

38.5 

11.5 

53.7 

.►10.4 

35.2 

6.4 

18.7 

5 

7.4 

28.3 

9.6 

42.2 

7.1 

24.8 

3.9 

12.3 

6 

7.3 

20.9 

10.4 

32.6 

6.7 

17.7 

3.0 

8.4 

7 

7.7 

13.6 

13.7 

22.2 

5.7 

11.0 

2.7 

5.4 

8 

3.3 ! 

5.9 

5.9 

8.5 

2.3 

5.3 

1.6 

2.7 

9+ 

2.6 

2.6 

2.6 

2.6 

3.0 

3.0 

1.1 

1.1 

Cases 

1850 

270 

298 

1282 

Mean 

3.23 years 

4.00 years 

3.06 years 

2.22 years 

Median 

2.11 years 

3.34 years 

1.74 years 

0.93 years 


Heterosexual Coitus 


1 

39.2 

100.0 

32.7 

100.0 

38.3 

100.0 

56.3 

100.0 

2 

13.6 

60.8 

9.0 

67.3 

15.6 

61.7 

15.7 

43.7 

3 

10.2 

47.2 

6.0 

58.3 

13.0 

46.1 

8.8 

28.0 

4 

10.2 

37.0 

15.1 

52.3 

8.5 

33.1 

6.0 

19.2 

5 

7.1 

26.8 

9.6 

37.2 

6.5 

24.6 

4.1 

13.2 

6 

7.4 

19.7 

7.0 

27.6 

8.4 

18.1 

4.7 

9.1 

7 

9.2 

12.3 

14.6 

20.6 

7.8 

9.7 

2.8 

4.4 

8 

1.6 

3.1 

3.5 

6.0 

0.6 

1.9 

1.3 

1.6 

9+ 

1,5 

1.5 

2.5 

2.5 

1.3 

1.3 

0.3 

0.3 

Cases 

671 

199 

154 

318 

Mean 

Median 

3.09 years 

2.07 years 

3.72 years 

3.17 years 

2.97 years 

1.77 years 

2.19 years 

0.89 years 


{Table continued on next page) 


EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


167 



Duration of Pre-adolescent Sex Play 

NO. OF 

1 TOTAL 

POPULATION 

1 EDUC. LEVEL 

1 EDUC. LEVEL I 

EDUC. LEVEL 

YEARS 

IN- 

VOLVED 

i 

! 

U.S. CORRECTION 


0-8 


9-12 


13 + 

% 

Cumu- 

lated 

% 

% 

Cumu- 

lated 

% 

7o 

Cumu- 

lated 

% 

% 

Cumu- 

lated 

% 


Homosexual Play 


1 

27.1 

100.0 

20.0 

100.0 

26.9 

100.0 

43.2 

100.0 

2 

17.8 

72.9 

11.2 

80.0 

20.9 

73.1 

19.8 

56.8 

3 

10.0 

55.1 

11.2 

68.8 

9.1 

52.2 

11.1 

37.0 

4 

10.8 

45.1 

8.8 

57.6 

12.8 

1 43.1 

7.7 

25.9 

5 

11.5 

34.3 

14.7 

48.8 

11.1 

30.3 

6.0 

18.2 

6 

8.8 

22.8 

12.9 

34.1 

7.4 

19.2 

5.1 

12.2 

7 

6.7 

14.0 

10.9 ' 

21.2 

5.1 

11.8 

3.8 

7.1 

8 

5.2 

7.3 

8.2 

10.3 

4.4 

6.7 

2.1 

3.3 

9+ 

2.1 

2.1 

2.1 

2.1 

2.3 

2.3 

1.2 

1.2 

Cases 

2096 

340 

297 

1459 

Mean 

Median 

3.54 years 

2.63 years 

4.24 years 

3.88 years 

3.39 years 

2.26 years 

2.63 years 

1.35 years 


Table 25. Number of years involved, pre-adolescent sex play 


(e.g,, Hamilton 1929) ; but it is probably still too low, for 70 per cent of the 
pre-adolescent boys who have contributed to the present study have 
admitted such experience, and there is no doubt that even they forget many 
of their earlier activities. It is not Improbable that nearly all boys have some 
pre-adolescent genital play with other boys or with girls. Only about one- 
fifth as many of the girls have such play. 

Most of this pre-adolescent sex play occurs between the ages of eight 
and thirteen (Table 24, Figure 25), although some of it occurs at every age 
from earliest childhood to adolescence. For a quarter of the boys who 
have such play, the activity is limited to a single year (24.3%) or two 
(17.9%) or three (10.4%) in pre-adolescence (Table 25). For many of them 
there is only a single experience. A third of the active males (36.2%) con- 
tinue the play for five years or more. That the activity does not extend fur- 
ther is clearly a product of cultural restraints, for pre-adolescent sex play 
in the other anthropoids is abundant and continues into adult performance 
(Bingham 1928). Most of the play takes place with companions close to 
the subject’s own age. On the other hand, the boy’s initial experience is 
often (although not invariably) with a slightly older boy or girl. Older 
persons are the teachers of younger people in all matters, including the 
sexual. The record includes some cases of pre-adolescent boys involved in 
sexual contacts with adult females, and still more cases of pre-adolescent 
boys involved with adult males. Data on this point were not systematically 
gathered from all histories, and consequently the frequency of contacts 
with adults cannot be calculated with precision. 




168 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Homosexual Play. On the whole, the homosexual child play is found in 
more histories, occurs more frequently, and becomes more specific than 
the pre-adolescent heterosexual play. This depends, as so much of the 
adult homosexual activity depends, on the greater accessibility of the boy’s 
own sex (Table 26). In the younger boy, it is also fostered by his socially 
encouraged disdain for girls’ ways, by his admiration for masculine prow- 
ess, and by his desire to emulate older boys. The anatomy and functional 
capacities of male genitalia interest the younger boy to a degree that is not 


Companions of Pre-adolescent Boys 
AT Subject’s Age 10-11 



total 

educational level 


u. s. 




classefication 

POPULA- 





TION 

% 

0-8 

% 

9-12 

13+ 

% 

Males predominate 

72.3 

73.8 

69.2 

80.7 

Males many, females some 

36.0 

32.7 

34.3 

49.4 

Males many, females none 

32.8 

37.1 

31.4 

29.0 

Males some, females none 

3.5 

4.0 1 

3.5 j 

2.3 

Males equal females 

23.0 

22.8 

24.8 1 

16.7 

Males many, females many 

17 . 6 ^ 

17.4 

18.8 

13.7 

Males some, females some 

3.3 

2.0 

4.3 

2.3 

Males none, females none 

2.1 

3.4 

1.7 

0.7 

Females predominate 

4.7 

3.4 

6.0 

2.6 

Males none, females some 

0.3 

0.0 

0.6 

0.1 

Males none, females many 

0.4 

0.4 

0.5 

0.1 

Males some, females many 

4.0 

3.0 

4.9 

2.4 

Cases 

4311 

820 

633 

2858 


Table 26. Sex of companions of pre-adolescent boys 


A record of the boy’s associates, in his play and social activities, when he is 10-11 
years of age. 

appreciated by older males who have become heterosexually conditioned 
and who are continuously on the defensive against reactions which might 
be interpreted as homosexual. 

About half of the older males (48%), and nearer two-thirds (60%) of 
the boys who were pre-adolescent at the time they contributed their 
histories, recall homosexual activity in their pre-adolescent years. The 
mean age of the first homosexual contact is about nine years, two and a 
half months (9.21 years) (Table 28, Figures 25, 26). 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


169 


The order of appearance of the several homosexual techniques is: 
exhibition of genitalia, manual manipulation of genitaUa, anal or oral 
contacts with genitalia, and urethral insertions (Table 27). Exhibition is 
much the most common form of homosexual play (in 99.8 per cent of all 
the histories which have any activity). It appears in the sex play of the 
youngest children, where much of it is incidental, definitely casual, and 
quite fruitless as far as erotic arousal is concerned. The most extreme 
development of exhibitionism occurs among the older pre-adolescents and 
the younger adolescent males who have discovered the significance of self 
masturbation and may have acquired proficiency in effecting orgasm. By 
that time there is a social value in establishing one’s ability, and many a 


ANY SEX PLAY HETEROSEXUAL COITAL HOMOSEXUAL 

PLAY PLAY PLAY 



Figure 25. Percent of males involved in sex play at each pre-adolescent age 


Data all corrected for U. S. Census distribution. 


boy exhibits his masturbatory techniques to lone companions or to whole 
groups of boys. In the latter case, there may be simultaneous exhibition as 
a group activity. The boy’s emotional reaction in such a performance is 
undoubtedly enhanced by the presence of the other boys. There are teen- 
age boys who continue this exhibitionistic activity throughout their high 
school years, some of them even entering into compacts with their closest 
friends to refrain from self masturbation except when in the presence of 
each other. In confining such social performances to self masturbation, 
these boys avoid conflicts over the homosexual. By this time, however, the 
psychic reactions may be homosexual enough, although it may be diflS.cult 
to persuade these individuals to admit it. 



170 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Exhibitionism leads naturally into the next step in the homosexual play, 
namely the mutual manipulation of genitalia. Such manipulation occurs in 
the play of two-thirds (67.4%) of all the pre-adolescent males who have any 
homosexual activity (Table 27). Among younger pre-adolescents the man- 
ual contacts are still very incidental and casual and without any recogni- 
tion of the emotional possibilities of such experience. Only a small portion 


Techniques in Pre-adolescent Sex Play 

% OF ACTIVE POPULATION 


EDUC. 

LEVEL 

CASES 

SEXUAL PLAY, 
% OF TOTAL 


HETEROSEXUAL PLAY 




POPULATION 









Exhibition 

Manual 

Coital 

Oral 

Vaginal 

Insertions 

Total 

1843 

39.6 

99.6 

81.4 

55.3 

8.9 

49.1 

0-8 

270 

30.0 

99.6 

91.7 

74.4 

17.0 

59.3 

9-12 

295 

44.0 

99.7 

80.0 

52.4 

5.8 

49.8 

13+ 

1277 

43.0 

99.0 

64.8 

25.7 

3.4 

24.4 



ANY HOMO- 


HOMOSEXUAL PLAY 


EDUC. 


SEXUAL PLAY, 






LEVEL 

CASES 

% OF TOTAL 
POPULATION 

' Exhibition 

Manual 

Anal 

Oral 

Urethral 

Insertions 

Total 

2102 

44.0 

99.8 

67.4 

17.0 

15.9 

6.0 

0-8 

341 

38.0 

100,0 

66.7 

17.5 

13.3 

4.8 

9-12 

298 

45.0 

99.7 

67.4 

16.7 

16.8 

6.9 

13-f- 

1463 

53.0 

99.5 

69.2 

16.9 

17.7 

5.3 


Table 27, Techniques in pre-adolescent sex play 

In order to determine the percent of the total pre-adolescent male population which 
has experience with any particular technique, multiply the figure in Column 3 (which 
shows the percent of the total population which has any kind of heterosexual or homo- 
sexual experience) by the incidence figure for the particular technique irnder considera- 
tion. 


of the cases leads to the sort of manipulation which does effect arousal and 
possibly orgasm in the partner. Manual manipulation is more likely to 
become so specific if the relation is had with a somewhat older boy, or 
with an adult. Without help from more experienced persons, many pre- 
adolescents take a good many years to discover masturbatory techniques 
that are sexually effective. 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


171 


Anal intercourse is reported by 17 per cent of the pre-adolescents who 
have any homosexual play. Anal intercourse among younger boys usu- 
ally fails of penetration and is therefore primarily femoral. Oral manipu- 
lation is reported by nearly 16 per cent of the boys (Table 27). Among 
younger boys, erotic arousal is less easily effected by oral contacts, more 
easily effected by manual manipulation. The anal and oral techniques are 
limited as they are because even at these younger ages there is some 
knowledge of the social taboos on these activities; and it is, in conse- 
quence, probable that the reported data are considerable understatements 
of the activities which actually occur. 

Pre-adolescent homosexual play is carried over into adolescent or adult 
activity in something less than a half of all the cases (Table 29). There are 



Figure 26. Age of first pre-adolescent sexual experience 

Each curve an ogive, where 100 per cent is the total number of boys who ever have 
such experience. 

differences between social levels. In lower educational levels, the chances 
are 50-50 that the pre-adolescent homosexual play will be continued into 
adolescence or later. For the group that will go to college, the chances are 
better than four to one that the pre-adolescent activity will not be followed 
by later homosexual experience. In many cases, the later homosexuality 
stops with the adolescent years, but many of the adults who are actively 
and more or less exclusively homosexual date their activities from pre- 
adolescence. In a later volume these data will be examined in more detail, 
in connection with an analysis of the factors involved in the development 
of a heterosexual-homosexual balance. 




First Pre-adolescent Sexual Experience 


TOTAL U.S. 

POPULATION 

educ. level 

0-8 

educ. level 

9-12 

educ. level 

13 + 

% 

Cumu- 
lat. % 

Cases 

% 

Cumu- 
lat. % 

Cases 

% 

Cumu- 
lat. % 

Cases 

% 

Cumu- 
lat. % 


Any Sex Play 


5 

16.1 

16.1 

65 

15.2 

15.2 

62 

15.3 

15, 

.3 

401 

20. 

,9 

20.9 

6 

13.9 

30. Ol 

56 

13.1 

28.3 

58 

14.4 

29, 

.7 

260 

13. 

^5 1 

34.4 

7 

11.5 

41.5 

46 

10.6 

38.9 

49 

12.1 

41. 

,8 

215 

11. 

2 

45.6 

8 

14.5 

56.0 

67 

15.7 

54.6 

55 

13.6 

55. 

.4 

298 

15. 

5 

61.1 

9 

8.0 

64.0 

38 

8.9 

63.5 

30 

7.4 

62. 

,8 

166 

8. 

6 

69.7 

10 

15.1 

79.1 

68 

15.9 

79.4 

60 

14.9 

77. 

.7 

270 

14. 

1 

83.8 

11 

8.1 

87.2 

26 ' 

6.1 

85.5 

38 

9 4 

87. 

,1 

145 

7. 

5 

91.3 

12 

8.5 

95.7 

35 

8.2 

93. 7| 

37 

9.2 

96. 

,3 

129 

6. 

7 

98.0 

13 

3.4 

99.1 

19 

4.4 

98.1 

13 

3.2 ! 

99. 

.5 

33 

1. 

7 

1 99.7 

14 

0.9 

100.0 

8 i 

1.9 

100.0 

2 

0.5 i 

100. 

,0 

4 

0. 

3 

100.0 

Cases 

2753 

428 

404 

1921 

Mean 

Median 

8.81 years 

7 . 59 years 

8.93 years 

7.69 years 

8 . 84 years 

7.61 years 

8 . 45 years 

7.29 years 


Heterosexual Play 


5 

15.4 

15.4 

39 

14 4 

14.4 

42 

14.1 

14.1 

292 

22.8 

22.8 

6 

13.2 

28.6 

34 

12.5 

26.9 

39 1 

13.1 

27.2 

194 

15.1 

37.9 

7 

13.5 

42.1 

34 

12.5 

39 4 

43 1 

14.4 

41.6 

153 

11.9 

49.8 

8 

14.9 

57.0 

44 

16.3 

55.7 

41 

13.8 

55.4 

206 

16.1 

65.9 

9 

7.4 

64.4 

21 

7.8 

63.5 

21 

7.0 

62.4 

105 

8.2 

74.1 

10 

14.1 

78.5 

41 

15.1 

78.6 

41 

13.8 

76.2 

166 

13.0 1 

87.1 

11 

8.8 

87.3 

19 

7.0 

85.6 

31 

10.4 

86.6 

86 

6.7 

93.8 

12 

9.2 

96.5 

26 

9.6 

95.2 

3P 

10.1 

96.7 

62 

4.8 

98.6 

13 

2.7 

99.2 

11 

4.1 

99.3 

7 

2.3 

99.0 

17 

1.3 

99.9 

14 

0.8 

100.0 

2 

0.7 

100.0 

3 

1.0 

100.0 

1 

0.1 

100.0 

Cases 

1851 

271 

298 

1282 

Mean 

Median 

1 8.81 years 
7.33 years 

8.91 years 

7.66 years 

8.91 years 

7 . 22 years 

8.20 years 

7.01 years 


Homosexual Play 


5 

12.4 

12.4 

47 

13.7 

13.7 

1 

33 I 

11.1 

11.1 

208 

14.2 

14.2 

6 

12.0 

24.4 

43 

12.6 

26.3 

36 1 

12.1 

23.2 

153 ! 

10.5 

24.7 

7 

10.5 

34.9 

37 

10.8 

37.1 

31 1 

10.4 

33.6 

145 1 

9.9 ; 

34.6 

8 

13.5 

48.4 

51 

14.9 

52.0 

37 

12.4 

46.0 

217 

14.9 

49.5 

9 

9.1 

57.5 

29 

8.5 

60.5 

27 

9.1 

55.1 

146 

10.0 

59.5 

10 

16.9 

74.4 

56 

16.4 

76.9 

51 

17.0 

72.1 

257 

17.6 

77.1 

11 

10.2 

t 84.6 

23 

6.7 

83.6 

36 

12.1 

84.2 

156 

10.7 

87.8 

12 

9.5 

94.1 

29 

8.5 

92.1 

30 

10.1 

94.3 

138 

9.4 

97.2 

13 

4.7 

> 98.8 

18 

5.3 

^ 97.4 

15 

5.0 

99.3 

34 

2.3 

99.5 

14 

1.2 

1 100.0 

9 

2.6 

100.0 

2 

0.7 

100.0 

8 

0.5 

100.0 

Cases 

2102 

342 

298 

1462 

Mean 

9.21 years 

9.10 years 


9.31 years 


9 . 06 years 


Median 

8.22 years 

7 . 87 years 


8.46 years 


8.06 years 



Table 28. Age of first pre-adolescent sex play 

The cumulated percentages (ogives) are based on the total number of individuals who ever 
have such sexual experience (the active population). They do not represent percents of the 
total population. 


172 





EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


173 


Heterosexual Play. The average age for beginning pre-adolescent hetero- 
sexual play is about eight years and ten months (a mean of 8.81 years) 
(Table 28, Figures 25, 26). This is approximately five months earlier than the 
average age for the beginning of homosexual play; but heterosexual activity, 
nonetheless, does not occupy quite as much of the attention of the pre- 
adolescent boys. It is found in 40 per cent of the pre-adolescent histories. 

Just as with the homosexual, the heterosexual play begins with the 
exhibition of genitalia; and of those pre-adolescent boys who have any 
sex play with girls, about 99 per cent engage in such exhibition (Table 27). 
For nearly 20 per cent of the boys, this is the limit of the activity. There is 
considerable curiosity among children, both male and female, about the 
genitalia of the opposite sex, fostered, if not primarily engendered, by the 
social restrictions on inter-sexual display. The boy is incited by the greater 
care which many parents exercise in covering the genitalia of the girls in 
the family — a custom which reaches its extreme in some other cultures where 
the boys may go completely nude until adolescence, while the girls are 
carefully clothed at least from the ages of four or five. 

Of those pre-adolescent boys who have any heterosexual play, 81.4 per 
cent carry it to the point of manually manipulating the genitalia of the 
female (Table 27). For many of the youngest boys this is even more inci- 
dental than the manual manipulation which occurs in homosexual contacts. 
Among certain groups, particularly in upper social levels, the children 
sometimes lack information on coitus, and there may be no comprehension 
that there are possibilities in heterosexual activity other than those afforded 
by manual contacts. There are vaginal insertions which involve objects of 
various sorts, but most often they are finger insertions. Pre-adolescent 
attempts to effect genital union occur in nearly 22 per cent of all male 
histories, which is over half (55.3%) of the histories of the boys who have 
Siny pre-adolescent play. On this point, there are considerable differences 
between social levels (Table 27). Three-quarters (74.4%) of the boys who 
will never go beyond eighth grade try such pre-adolescent coitus, but such 
experience is had by only one-quarter (25.7%) of the pre-adolescent boys 
of the group which will ultimately go to college (Chapter 10). 

The lower level boy has considerable information and help on these 
matters from older boys or from adult males, and in many cases his first 
heterosexual contacts are with older girls who have already had experi- 
ence. Consequently, in this lower level, pre-adolescent contacts often 
involve actual penetration and the children have what amounts to real 
intercourse. The efforts of the upper level boys are less often successful, in 
many cases amounting to little more than the apposition of genitalia. With 
the lower level boy, pre-adolescent coitus may occur with some frequency, 
and it may be had with a variety of partners. For the upper level boy, the 
experience often occurs only once or twice, and with a single partner or 



174 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


two. These diflferences between patterns at different social levels, even in 
pre-adolescence, are of the utmost significance in any consideration of a 
program of sex education (Chapter 10). 

Oral contacts with females occur in only 8.9 per cent of the boys who 
have pre-adolescent heterosexual play. Oral contacts are more likely to 
occur where the girl is older, or where an adult woman is involved. There 
is considerable evidence that oral contacts are recognized as taboo, even 
at pre-adolescent ages. 


Continuity of Pre-adolescent Sex Play 

% OF ACTIVE POPULATIONS 


Population 

HETEROSEXUAL 

PETTING 

heterosexual 

COITUS 

homosexual 

PLAY 

Cases 

% with 
Con- 
tinuity 

Cases 

%with 

1 Con- 
tinuity 

Cases 

% with 
Con- 
tinuity 

Total (U. S. Correction) 

1227 

64.9 

628 

54.7 

1412 

42.1 

Educ. level 0-8 

243 

77.0 

196 

73.5 

243 

50.6 

Educ. level 9-12 

221 

67.4 

147 

53.7 

207 

42.5 

Educ. level 13-1- 

763 

{ 

29.8 

285 

18.6 

962 

22.5 


Table 29. Continuity of pre-adolescent "sex play with adolescent activity 


Pre-adolescent heterosexual play is carried over into corresponding 
adolescent activities in nearly two-thirds of the cases (Table 29). There is 
a somewhat higher carry-over of heterosexual petting, a lesser carry-over 
of heterosexual coitus. Again there are tremendous differences between 
social levels. If coitus is had by a pre-adolescent boy who will never go 
beyond eighth grade in school, the chances are three to one that he will 
continue such activity, without any major break, in his adolescent and 
adult years. If the boy who has pre-adolescent coitus belongs to the group 
that will ultimately go to college, the chances are more than four to one 
that the activity will not be continued in his adolescent years. Community 
attitudes on these matters are already exerting an influence on the pre- 
adolescent boy. 

Animal Contacts. Animal contacts are largely confined to farm boys. Of 
the boys who will ever be involved, a third have had their first contacts by 
nine years of age; but between 10 and 12 there is a more rapid increase in 
the active incidence figures. The level which is reached in these years is 
never again equalled, either in pre-adolescence or in adolescent or in later 
years. In about a third of the cases, there is direct continuity between the 
pre-adolescent and the adolescent experiences with animal intercourse. 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITy 


175 


PRE-ADOLESCENT ORGASM 

In the technical literature there seem to be only a few references (e.g., 
Moll 1912, Merrill 1918, Moses 1922, Krafft-Ebing 1924, Rohleder 1921, 
Hamilton 1929:427) to the possibility of the pre-adolescent child experi- 
encing orgasm. But, as we have already indicated, orgasm is not at all 


First Pre-adolescent Erotic Arousal and Orgasm 

NUMBER OF CASES 


AGE 

EROTIC AROUSAL 

ORGASM 

In Any 
Sex Play 

In Hetero- 
sexual 
Play 

In Homo- 
sexual 
Play 

Data from 
Present 
Study 

Data from 
Other 
Subjects 

Total 

Cases 

%0f 

Total 

1 





12 

12 

2.5 

2 





8 

8 . 

1.6 

3 




2 

7 

9 

1.8 

4 

10 

9 

2 


12 

12 

2.5 

5 

30 

23 

8 

5 

9 

14 1 

2.9 

6 

26 

21 

8 

15 

19 

34 1 

7.0 

7 

32 

29 

6 

21 

17 

38 

7.8 

8 

38 

29 

12 

27 

21 

48 

9.9 

9 i 

38 

37 

• 3 

24 

26 

50 

10.3 

10 

83 

71 

17 

56 

26 

82 

16.8 

11 

72 

67 

13 

54 i 

22 

76 

15.6 

12 

92 

84 

13 

51 

23 

74 

15.2 

13 

37 

37 

3 

15 

9 

24 

4.9 

14 

10 

10 


3 

3 

6 

1.2 

15 

3 

2 

1 





Total 

471 

419 

86 

273 

214 

487 

100.0 

Mean 








Age 

10.28 

10.41 

9.62 

10.40 

8.51 

9.57 


Median 








Age 

9.75 

9.87 

9.26 

9.77 

8.10 

9.23 



Table 30. Pre-adolescent eroticism and orgasm 

All data based on memory of older subjects, except in the column entitled “data 
from other subjects.” In the later case, original data gathered by certain of our subjects 
were made available for use in the present volume. Of the 214 cases so reported, all 
but 14 were subsequently observed in orgasm (see Table 31). 


rare among pre-adolescent boys, and it also occurs among pre-adolescent 
girls. Since this significant fact has not been well established in scientific 
publication, Tt will be profitable to record here the nature of the data for 
the male in some detail. 


7 



176 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Pre-adolescent boys, since they are incapable of ejaculation, may be as 
uncertain as some inexperienced females in their recognition of orgasm. 
In consequence, the record on such early experience is incomplete in most 
of the histories, and it is as yet impossible to make any exact calculation of 


Pre-adolescent Experience in Orgasm 


AGE WHEN 

OBSERVED 

TOTAL 

POPULA- 

TION 

CASES NOT 

REACHING 

CLIMAX 

CASES 

REACHING 

CLIMAX 

CUMU- 

LATED 

POPULA- 

TION 

CUMU- 

LATED 

CASES TO 

CLIMAX 

PERCENT OF 

EACH AGE 

REACHING 

CLIMAX 

2mon, 

1 

1 

0 




3 mon. 

2 

2 

0 




4mon. 

1 

1 

0 




5 mon. 

2 

1 

1 




8 mon. 

2 

1 

1 




9 mon. 

1 

1 

0 




10 mon. 

4 

1 

3 




11 mon. 

3 

1 

2 




12 mon. 

12 

10 

2 




Up to 1 yr. 

28 

19 

9 

28 

9 

32.1 

Up to 2 yr. 

22 

11 

i 11 

50 

20 


Up to 3 yr. 

9 

2 

7 

59 

27 

1 cn 1 

Up to 4 yr. 

12 

5 

7 e 

71 

34 

) D7. 1 

Up to 5 yr. 

6 

3 

3 

77 

37 

] 

Up to 6 yr. 

12 

5 

7 

89 

44 


Up to 7 yr. 

17 

8 

9 

106 

53 


Up to 8 yr. 

26 

12 

14 

132 

67 

^ 63.4 

Up to 9 yr. 

29 

10 

19 

161 

86 


Up to 10 yr. 

28 

! 6 

22 

189 

108 


Up to 1 1 yr. 

34 

9 

25 

223 

133 

1 

Up to 12 yr. 

46 

7 

39 

269 

172 

80.0 

Up to 13 yr. 

35 

7 

28 

304 

200 

1 

Up to 14 yr. 

11 

5 

6 

315 

206 


Up to 15 yr. 

2 

2 

0 

317 

206 


Total 

317 

111 

206 

317 

206 

65.0 


Table 31. Ages of pre-adolescent orgasm 
Based on actual observation of 317 males. 


the incidence or frequency in the population as a whole. Nevertheless, 
some of the younger boys who have contributed to the present study have 
described what is unmistakably sexual orgasm in their pre-adolescent his- 
tories, and a larger number of adults remember such experience (Table 30). 

Better data on pre-adolescent climax come from the histories of adult 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


177 


males who have had sexual contacts with younger boys and who, with 
their adult backgrounds, are able to recognize and interpret the boys’ 
experiences. Unfortunately, not all of the subjects with such contacts in 
their histories were questioned on this point of pre-adolescent reactions; 
but 9 of our adult male subjects have observed such orgasm. Some of these 
adults are technically trained persons who have kept diaries or other 
records which have been put at our disposal; and from them we have 
secured information on 317 pre-adolescents who were either observed in 
self masturbation, or who were observed in contacts with other boys or 
older adults. The record so obtained shows a considerable sexual capacity 
among these boys. Before presenting the data, however, it should be em- 
phasized that this is a record of a somewhat select group of younger males 
and not a statistical representation for any larger group. These records are 
based on more or less uninhibited boys, most of whom had heard about 
sex and seen sexual activities among their companions, and many of whom 
had had sexual contacts with one or more adults. Most of them knew of 
orgasm as the goal of such activity, and some of them, even at an early 
age, had become definitely aggressive in seeking contacts. Most boys are 
more inhibited, more restricted by parental controls. Many boys remain in 
ignorance of the nature of a complete sexual response until they become 
adolescent. 

Orgasm has been observed in boys of every age from 5 months to adoles- 
cence (Table 31). Orgasm is in our records for a female babe of 4 months. 
The orgasm in an infant or other young male is, except for the lack of an 
ejaculation, a striking duplicate of orgasm in an older adult. As described 
earher in this chapter, the behavior involves a series of gradual physiologic 
changes, the development of rhythmic body movements with distinct 
penis throbs and pelvic thrusts, an obvious change in sensory capacities, 
a final tension of muscles, especially of the abdomen, hips, and back, a 
sudden release with convulsions, including rhythmic anal contractions — 
followed by the disappearance of all symptoms. A fretful babe quiets down 
under the initial sexual stimulation, is distracted from other activities, 
begins rhythmic pelvic thrusts, becomes tense as climax approaches, is 
thrown into convulsive action, often with violent arm and leg movements, 
sometimes with weeping at the moment of climax. After climax the child 
loses erection quickly and subsides into the calm and peace that typically 
follows adult orgasm. It may be some time before erection can be induced 
again after such an experience. There are observations of 16 males up to 
1 1 months of age, with such typical orgasm reached in 7 cases. In 5 cases 
of young pre-adolescents, observations were continued over periods of 
months or years, until the individuals were old enough to make it certain 
that true orgasm was involved; and in all of these cases the later reactions 
were so similar to the earlier behavior that there could be no doubt of the 
oigastic nature of the first experience. 



178 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


While the records for very young boys are fewer than for boys nearer 
the age of adolescence, and while the calculations for these youngest cases 
are consequently less reliable, the data do show a gradual increase, with 
advancing age, in the percentage of cases able to reach climax : 32 per cent 
of the boys 2 to 12 months of age, more than half (57.1%) of the 2- to 5- 
year olds, and nearly 80 per cent of the pre-adolescent boys between 10 and 
13 years of age (inclusive) came to climax. Half of the boys had reached 
climax by 7 years of age (nearly half of them by 5 years), and two-thirds 
of them by 12 years of age. The observers emphasize that there are some 
of these pre-adolescent boys (estimated by one observer as less than one- 
quarter of the cases), who fail to reach climax even under prolonged and 
varied and repeated stimulation; but, even in these young boys, this 
probably represents psychologic blockage more often than physiologic 
incapacity. 


TIME 

CASES 

TIMED 

PERCENT OF 

POPULATION 

CUMULATED 

PERCENT 

Up to 10 sec. 

12 

6.4 

6.4 

10 sec. to 1 min. 

46 

24.5 

30.9 

1 to 2 min. 

40 

21.3 

52.2 

2 to 3 min. 

23 

12.2 

64.4 

3 to 5 min. 

33 

17.5 

81.9 

5 to 10 min. 

23 

12.2 

94,1 

Over 10 min. 

11 

5.9 

100.0 

Total 

188 

100.0 



Mean time to climax: 3 .02 minutes 
Median time to climax: 1.91 minutes 


Table 32. Speed of pre-adolescent orgasm 

Duration of stimulation before climax ; observations timed with second hand or stop 
watch. Ages range from five months of age to adolescence. 

In the population as a whole, a much smaller percentage of the boys 
experience orgasm at any early age, because few of them find themselves 
in circumstances that test their capacities; but the positive record on these 
boys who did have the opportunity makes it certain that many infant males 
and younger boys are capable of orgasm, and it is probable that half or 
more of the boys in an uninhibited society could reach climax by the time 
they were three or four years of age, and that nearly all of them could 
experience such a climax three to five years before the onset of adolescence. 

Erection is much quicker in pre-adolescent boys than in adults, although 
the speed with which climax is reached in pre-adolescent males varies 
considerably in different boys (Table 32), just as it does in adults. There are 
two-year olds who come to climax in less than 10 seconds, and there are 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


179 


two-year olds who may take 10 or 20 minutes, or more. There is a similar 
range among pre-adolescents of every other age. The mean time required 
to reach climax was almost exactly 3 minutes, and the median time was 
under 2 minutes. From earliest infancy until the middle twenties there is 
no effect of age on this point, although beyond that older males slow up 
in speed of response (Chapter 6). 

The most remarkable aspect of the pre-adolescent population is its 
capacity to achieve repeated orgasm in limited periods of time. This capac- 
ity definitely exceeds the ability of teen-age boys who, in turn, are much 
more capable than any older males (Tables 33, 34, 48, Figure 36). Among 
182 pre-adolescent boys on whom sufficient data are available, more than 


NO. OF 

ORGASMS 

CASES 

OB- 

SERVED 

PERCENT 

OF 

POPULA- 

TION 

CUMU- 

LATED 

PERCENT 

TIME BETWEEN 

ORGASMS 

CASES 

TIMED 

PERCENT 

OF 

POPULA- 

TION 

CUMU- 

LATED 

PERCENT 

1 

81 

44.5 

100.0 

Up to 10 sec. 

3 

4.7 

4.7 

2 

17 

9.3 

55.5 

11 to 60 sec. 

15 

23.5 

28.2 

3 

18 

9.9 

46.2 

Up to 2 min. 

8 

12.5 

40.7 

4 

10 

5,5 

36,3 

Up to 3 min. 

10 

15.6 

56.3 

5 

14 

7.7 

30.8 

Up to 5 min. 

7 

10.9 

67.2 

6-10 

30 

16.5 

23.1 

Up to 10 min. 

11 

17.2 

84.4 

11-15 

9 

4.9 1 

6.6 

Up to 20 min. 

7 

10.9 

95.3 

16-20 

2 

1.1 

1.7* 

Up to 30 min. 

1 

1.6 

96.9 

21 + 

1 

0.6 

0.6 

Over 30 min. 

2 

3.1 

100.0 

Total 

182 

100.0 

100.0 

Total 

64 

100.0 

100.0 














Mean No. of Orgasms: 3.72 Mean Time Lapse: 6.28 minutes 

Median No. of Orgasms: 2.62 Median Time Lapse: 2.25 minutes 


Table 33. Multiple orgasm in pre-adolescent males 

Based on a small and select group of boys. Not typical of the experience, but sug- 
gestive of the capacities of pre-adolescent boys in general. 

half (55.5%, 101 cases) readily reached a second climax within a short 
period of time, and nearly a third (30.8%) of all these 182 boys were able 
to achieve 5 or more climaces in quite rapid succession (Tables 32-34). 
It is certain that a higher proportion of the boys could have had multiple 
orgasm if the situation had offered. Among 64 cases on which there are 
detailed reports, the average interval between the first and second climaces 
ranged from less than 10 seconds to 30 minutes or more, but the mean 
interval was only 6.28 minutes (median 2.25 minutes) (Table 33). There 
are older males, even in their thirties and older, who are able to equal this 
performance, but a much higher proportion of these pre-adolescent males 
are so capable. Even the youngest males, as young as 5 months in age, are 




SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


m 

capable of such repeated reactions. Typical cases are shown in Table 34. 
The maximum observed was 26 climaces in 24 hours, and the report 
indicates that still more might have been possible in the same period of 
time. 

About a third of these boys remain in erection after the first orgasm and 
proceed directly to a second contact. There is another third that stays in 
erection but experiences some physical and erotic let-down before trying 
to achieve a second orgasm. In another third, the erection quickly subsides 
and there is a complete disappearance of arousal as soon as orgasm is 
reached. Any repetition depends upon new arousal, and that may not be 
possible for some minutes or hours after the original experience. Among 


AGE 

NO. OF 

ORGASMS 

TIME 

INVOLVED 

AGE 

NO. OF 

ORGASMS 

TIME 

INVOLVED 

5 m©n. 

3 

? 

11 yr. 

11 

1 hr. 

11 mon. 

10 

1 hr. 

11 yr. 

19 

1 hr. 

11 mon. 

14 

38 min. 

12 yr. 

7 

3 hr. 


/ 7 

9 min. 



3 min. 

2yr. 


65 min. 

12 yr. 

1 9 

2 hr. 

2iyr. 

^ 4 

2 mm. 

12 yr. 

12 

2 hr. 

4 yr. 

6 

5 min. 

12 yr. 

15 

1 hr. 

4 yr. 

17 

10 hr. 

13 yr. 

7 

24 min. 

4 yr. 

26 

24 hr. 

13,yr. 

s 

2i hr. 

7yr. 

7 

3 hr. 

13 yr. 

9 i 

8 hr. 

8 yr. 

8 

2 hr. 


f ^ 1 

70 sec. 

9 yr. 

7 

68 min. 

13 yr. 

11 

8 hr. 

10 yr.’ 

9 

52 min. 


[26 

24 hr. 

10 yr. 

14 

24 hr. 

14 yr. 

11 

4 hr. 


Table 34. Examples of multiple orgasm in pre-adolescent males 
Some instances of higher frequencies. 


adult males, more individuals belong to this last class, and a much 
smaller number remains in erection until there is a repetition of the 
sexual contact. 

These data on the sexual activities of younger males provide an impor- 
tant substantiation of the Freudian view of sexuality as a component that 
is present in the human animal from earliest infancy, although it gives no 
support to the Freudian concept of a pre-genital stage of generalized erotic 
response that precedes more specific genital activity; nor does it show any 
necessity for a sexually latent or dormant period in the later adolescent 
years, except as such inactivity results from parental and social repressions 
of the growing child. It would seem that analysts have been correct in 
considering these capacities for childhood sexual development, or their 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


181 


suppression, as prime sources of adult patterns of sexual behavior and of 
many of the characteristics of the total personality. There are, of course, 
some who have questioned the truly sexual nature of the child’s experi- 
ences. Moore, for instance, remarks (1943, p. 45): ‘‘One would think that 
psychoanalysts would have confirmed their theories of infantile emotional- 
ity by a careful observation and study of large numbers of children . . . but 
I have been unable to find any such study by a member of the psycho- 
analytic school.” And again (p. 48): “As to the presence of specific sexual 
experience in infancy and early childhood, we shall never be able to solve 
the problem by appealing to the introspection of the infant and the child. 
Neither does the memory of the adult reach back to those early years so 
that he can tell us whether or not it is really true that in infancy and early 
childhood he experienced specific sexual excitement, and that this was 
repressed and became latent, as Freud maintained.” 

Moore leans heavily on Bridges (1936) and Buhler (1931) to argue 
(pp. 46-48) that the earliest manifestations of emotion may be labelled 
distress or delight; but that, although young children may perform “acts 
similar to masturbation” and seek a partner for genital manipulation, 
“there is no evidence . . . that these acts are accompanied by specific sexual 
pleasure . . . even though there are signs that the child in some manner 
enjoys them.” The conclusion is that although the child is capable of a 
tender personal love, it is of a non-erotic character and has nothing to do 
with the beginnings of sexuality.«Adding data from endocrinologic sources, 
he concludes that specifically sexual behavior is the product of biologic 
growth and of experience. 

Complying with the scientifically fair demand for records from trained 
observers, and answering Moore’s further demand (p. 71) that “writers 
. . . test their theories ... by empirical study and statistical procedures,” 
we have now reported observations on such specifically sexual activities 
as erection, pelvic thrusts, and the several other characteristics of true or- 
gasm in a list of 317 pre-adolescent boys ranging between infants of five 
months and adolescence in age. Adding the records based on the memories 
of older subjects concerning their own, and often clearly established, early 
experiences, there is a record of orgasm in 604 pre-adolescent boys (Tables 
30 and 31 combined). The existence of such an early capacity is exactly 
what students of animal behavior have reported for other mammals (Beach 
1947), and it is, therefore, not surprising to find it in the human infant. 
Important as learning and conditioning may be in the later development 
of specific types of sexual techniques and in the socio-sexual adjustments 
of the adolescent and adult, it must be accepted as a fact that at least some 
and probably a high proportion of the infant and older pre-adolescent 
males are capable of specific sexual response to the point of complete 
orgasm, whenever a sufficient stimulation is provided. 



182 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


ADOLESCENCE 

While the sexual history of the human male thus begins in earliest 
infancy and develops continuously to its maximum activity somewhere 
between the middle teens and twenty years of age (Chapter 7), the steady 
progress of the development is, among primates, accelerated in a period of 
growth which is known as adolescence. 

During adolescence the young male rather suddenly acquires physical 
stature and adult conformation, and he begins to produce an ejaculate 
which contains mature sperm and which can, therefore, effect fertilization 
when in contact with the egg of a mature female. These are the most 
obvious and the biologically significant developments of the period; but 
the student of human sexual behavior is concerned with adolescence, and 
must consider its physical signs and stigmata, not because the physical 
developments are in themselves of prime importance, but because adoles- 
cence marks what is, in most individuals, a considerable break between the 
patterns of sexual activity of the pre-adolescent boy and the patterns of the 
older boy or adult. The sexual life of the younger boy is more or less a part 
of his other play ; it is usually sporadic, and (under the restrictions imposed 
by our social structure) it may be without overt manifestation in a fair 
number of cases. The sexual life of the older male is, on the other hand, an 
end in itself, and (in spite of our social organization) in nearly all boys its 
overt manifestations become frequent and regular, soon after the onset of 
adolescence. 

In a portion of the cases the pre-adolescent sexual activities have pro- 
vided the introduction to adult activities : simple heterosexual play turns 
into more sophisticated petting; pre-adolescent attempts at intercourse 
lead to adult coitus; some of the pre-adolescent homosexual play leads 
into similar adult contacts. This is true in about 50 per cent of all male 
histories which include any pre-adolescent play (Table 29). In an equal 
number of the cases the pre-adolescent play ends well before or with the 
onset of adolescence, and adolescent and more adult sexual activities must 
start from new points, newly won social acquirements, newly learned 
techniques of physical contact. In many cases the newly adolescent boy’s 
capacity to ejaculate, his newly acquired physical characters of other sorts, 
do something to him which brings child play to an end and leaves him 
awkward about making further socio-sexual contacts. The psychologic and 
social factors involved in this break between pre-adolescent sexuality and 
adult sexual activity are questions that will deserve considerable study 
by some qualified student. Those boys in whom child play does merge 
directly into adult activity are more often from less inhibited, lower social 
levels (Table 29). 

For all boys, the experiences of pre-adolescence, whether directly con- 
tinued or not, must provide considerable conditioning which encourages 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 183 

or inhibits their sexual development in adolescent and in more adult 
years. 

Adolescence is a period of time, and not a particular point in the life of 
the growing boy. It involves a whole series of developmental changes, some 
of which come earlier, some later in the course of events. Individuals differ 
materially in the ages at which they experience the first of these events, and 
somewhat in the sequence in which the other transformations follow (Table 
35, Figure 27). 

Among most boys, the physical changes of adolescence come on more 
or less abruptly, usually between the ages of 11 and 14, and in that period 
their sexual activities are suddenly stepped up until, within another few 
years, most of them reach the maximum rate of their whole lives (Chapter 
7). Among most females, as the data in another volume will show, sexual 
development comes on more gradually than in the male, is often spread 
over a longer period of time, and does not reach its peak until a good many 
years after the boy is sexually mature. 

Chiefly within the past decade, several studies based on physical exami- 
nations of boys and girls have given precise information on the variation 
and average ages involved in the developmental changes of adolescence. 
Some of the studies (Baldwin 1916, Crampton 1908, 1944, Dimock 1937, 
Kubitschek 1932, Schonfeld 1943) have been cross-sectional, based on 
examinations of numbers of children of each age group; some, utilizing a 
longitudinal approach, have involved the more exact task of following the 
development of individual cases over a period of successive years (Boas 
1932, Dearborn and Rothney 1941, Greulich et al. 1938, Jones 1944, 
Meredith 1935, 1939, Shuttleworth 1937, 1939). The latter, however, are 
not always the more fruitful studies, for such observations are tedious, and 
long-time contacts so often fail that only a few subjects can be followed 
through to conclusion. 

The studies which are based on direct physical examinations may be 
accepted as more accurate than our own, for we have relied for the most 
part on the memory of persons who were removed by various and some- 
times long periods of years from the events which they were recalhng; but 
it is interesting to find that our records give averages and total curves 
which are not significantly different from the data in the observational 
studies (Chapter 4, Table 15, Figure 15). According to the memory of our 
subjects, physical changes in the adolescent boy usually proceed as follows: 
Beginning of development of pubic hair, first ejaculation, voice change, 
initiation of rapid growth in height, and, after some lapse of time, com- 
pletion of growth in height (Table 35, Figure 27). Similar data have been 
previously published from our laboratory (Ramsey 1943a) for a small 
sample of 291 younger males who were in or near the beginning of adoles- 
cence at the time of the study. Our present, larger sample gives curves that 



184 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Adolescent Physical Development 



PUBIC 

HRST 

VOICE 

BODY 

COMPLETION 

AGE 

HAIR 

EJACUL. 

CHANGE 

GROWTH 

OF GROWTH 


Percent Beginning 

8 


0.1 


0.1 


9 

0.2 

0.2 


0.0 


10 

2.0 

1.8 

0.6 

3.0 

0.1 

11 

7.7 

6.1 

2.9 

3.8 

0.2 

12 

25.5 

19.5 

14.0 

14.3 

! 1.3 

13 

33.5 

29.2 

26.4 

19.4 

3.4 

14 

22.8 

25.1 

26.0 

22.7 

8.2 

15 

5.5 

10.2 

14.3 

15.9 

11.5 

16 

2.0 

4.2 

9.1 

11.4 

18.6 

17 

0.7 

1.8 

! 3.3 

6.4 

17.1 

18 


0.8 

' 1.6 

2.3 

17.9 

19 


0.4 

0.7 

0.5 

8.4 

20 

0.1 

0.1 

0.4 

0.2 

7.5 

21 


0.1 

0.3 


3.1 

22 


0.1 

0.3 


1.8 

23 


0.1 



0.4 

24 


0.1 

0.1 


0.2 

25 





0.2 

Cases 

2511 

3573 

2279 

1355 1 

2621 

Mean Age 

Mnnw 


14.44 

14.49 

17.47 

Median Age 



14.23 

14.42 

17.40 

AGE 

Cumulated Percent 

8 


0.1 


0.1 


9 

0.2 

0.3 


0.1 


10 

2.2 

2.1 

0.6 

3.1 

0.1 

11 

9.9 

8.2 

3.5 

6.9 

0.3 

12 

35.4 

27.7 

17.5 

21.2 

1.6 

13 

68.9 

56.9 

43.9 

40.6 

5.0 

14 

91.7 

82.0 

69.9 

63.3 

13.2 

15 

97.2 

92.2 

84.2 

79.2 

24.7 

16 

99.2 

96.4 

93.3 

90.6 

43.3 

17 

99.9 

98.2 

96.6 

97.0 

60.4 

18 

99.9 

99.0 

98.2 

99.3 

78.3 

19 

99.9 

99.4 

98.9 

99.8 

86.7 

20 

100.0 

99.5 

99.3 

100.0 

94.2 

21 


99.7 

99.6 


97.3 

22 


99.8 

99.9 


99.1 

23 


99.9 

99.9 


99.5 

24 


100.0 

100.0 


99.7 

25 





100.0 

Cases 

2511 

3573 

2279 

1355 

2621 


Table 35. Adolescent developments 









EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


185 


are in most respects in close agreement with the Ramsey series ; but his 
records show voice change beginning sooner after the onset of pubic hair 
growth and before the first ejaculation (also see Jerome 1937, Curry 1940, 
Pedrey 1945). The Ramsey data indicate that “breast knots,” or subareo- 
lar nodes which are homologous to those which regularly occur in the 
female, are found in at least one-third of these boys between the ages of 
12 to 14 (Jung and Shafton 1935, Ramsey 1943). Physical examinations 
(Meredith 1935, 1939) on limited and selected series of males have shown 
that sudden body growth may begin nearer the time of pubic hair devel- 
opment than our older subjects recall. There are many individual differ- 
ences in the sequence of events. 



The pubhshed studies of younger boys almost completely lack data on 
the most significant of all adolescent developments, the occurrence of the 
first ejaculation. There have been several attempts to secure information 
by indirect methods, including a technique of examining for sperm in 
early morning samples of urine (Baldwin 1928). These methods will not 
soon supply any quantity of data; and the only other sources of informa- 
tion on this point have been the records obtained from the recall of sub- 
jects in the previously pubhshed case history studies. This material is now 
augmented by a considerable record based on the memory of persons who 
have contributed to the present study, and on an important body of data 
from certain of our subjects who have observed first ejaculation in a hst of 
several hundred boys. 

The earliest ejaculation remembered by any of our apparently normal 
males was at 8 (three males). We have the history of one unusual boy (a 



186 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Negro, interviewed when he was 12) who fixed 6 as his age at first ejacula- 
tion. The boy had been diagnosed by the clinician as “idiopathically pre- 
cocious in development.” In the literature (e,g., Ford and Guild 1937, 
Young 1937, Weinberger and Grant 1941) there are clinical cases for still 
younger ages, most of them involving endocrine pathologies. Pubic hair 
has been recorded for one year of age and non-motile sperm in urine after 
prostate massage at four and a half years. Eight, however, is the earliest 
age of first ejaculation known for apparently normal males. 

Except for the 6 cases of life-long ejaculatory impotence referred to 
earlier in the present chapter, the latest ages of first ejaculation reliably 


SCHOOL 

GRADE 

.NO. OF BOYS 

BEGINNING 

ADOLESCENCE 

% OF BOYS 
BEGINNING 

ADOLESCENCE 

CUMULATED % 
ADOLESCENT AT 

END OF GRADE 

1 

6 

0.16 

0.16 

2 

14 

0.38 

0.54 

3 

42 

1.13 

1.67 

4 

82 

2.20 

3.87 

5 

180 

4.83 

8.70 

6 

409 

10.96 

19.66 

7 

667 

17.88 

37.54 

8 

959 

25.71 

63.25 

9 

863 

23.14 

86.39 

10 

376 

10.08 

96.47 

11 

96 

2.57 

99.04 

12 

24 

0.64 

99.68 

13 

11 

0.29 

99.97 

14 

1 

0.03 

100.00 

Total 

3730 

100.00 

100.00 


Mean grade at onset of adolescence: 8.33 =t 0.028 
Median grade at onset of adolescence: 8.49 


Table 36. School grade at adolescence 

Most of the boys reaching adolescence in the lowest grades are retarded individuals 
of more advanced age than the average in the grade. 

recorded in the histories are 21 for two apparently healthy males, 24 for a 
religiously inhibited individual, and 22 and after 24 for two males with 
hormonal deficiencies. The spread between the youngest and the oldest 
non-endocrine case is 16 years. A variety of educational and social prob- 
lems arise out of these differences between chronologic and sexual age. 
For instance, an occasional boy in third or fourth grade is sexually as 
mature as an occasional senior in college (Table 36, Figure 28). 

In spite of this spread in the population as a whole, the record shows 
(Table 35, Figure 27) that about 90 per cent of the males ejaculate for the 




EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


187 


first time between the ages of 11 and 15 (inclusive). This is an age range of 
5 years. At the end of the seventh grade in school, about a third (37.5%) 
of the boys are adolescent; by the end of the tenth grade, nearly all of them 
(96.5%) are so (Table 36, Figure 28). The average boy turns adolescent in 
the eighth grade (a mean grade of 8.33). 

The mean age of first orgasm resulting in ejaculation is 13 years, lOi 
months (13.88 years). On this point, the male data are in striking contrast 
with preliminary calculations on the female. By 15 years of age, 92 per 
cent of the males have had orgasm, but at that same age less than a quarter 
of the females have had such experience; and the female population is 29 


SCHOOL 

ORADE 

1 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

7 

8 
9 
fO 
11 
12 

13 

14 



ADOUSCgNT 


I r I I I I • » » ' • 

0 20 40 60 SO 100 

PERCENT 

Figure 28. Percent of adolescent boys in each school grade 


years old before it includes as high a percentage of experienced individuals 
as is to be found in the male curve at 15. Precise data on the female must 
await the publication of a later volume. 

In the male the age of first ejaculation varies by nearly a year between 
different educational (social) levels: the mean is 14.58 for boys who never 
go beyond eighth grade in school, 13.97 for boys who go into high school 
but not beyond, and 13.71 for boys who will go to college (Table 37). The 
differences are probably the outcome of nutritional inequalities at different 
social levels, and they are in line with similar differences in mean ages of 
females at menarche, where nutrition is usually considered a prime factor 
effecting variation. 


1S8 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Ages at Onset of Adolescence 


AGE 

EDUCATIONAL LEVEL 

0-8 

EDUCATIONAL LEVEL 

9-12 

EDUCATIONAL LEVEL 

134- 

Cases 

% of 
Popula- 
tion 

Cumu- 

lated 

Per- 

cent 

Cases 

7oOf 

Popula- 

tion 

Cumu- 

lated 

Per- 

cent 

Cases 

%of 

Popula- 

tion 

Cumu- 

lated 

Per- 

cent 

8 




1 

0.2 

0.2 

2 

0.1 

0.1 

9 j 

1 

0.1 

0.1 

2 

0.3 ! 

0.5 

7 

0.3 

0.4 

10 

7 

0.9 

1.0 

11 

1.8 

2.3 

70 

2.5 

2.9 

11 ! 

22 

2.9 

3.9 

32 

5.2 

7.5 

221 

7.8 

10.7 

12 

104 

13.6 

17.5 

123 

19.9 

27.4 

707 

25.1 

35.8 

13 

185 

24.1 

41.6 

189 

30.5 

57.9 

980 

34.8 

70.6 

14 

275 

35.8 

77.4 

205 

33.0 

90.9 

647 

23.0 

93.6 

15 

137 

17.8 

95.2 

39 

6.3 

97.2 

126 

4.5 

98.1 

16 

31 

4.0 

99.2 

13 

2.1 

99.3 

37 

1.3 

99.4 

17 

5 

0.7 

99.9 

3 

0.5 

99.8 

15 

0.5 

99.9 

18 

1 

0.1 

100.0 

1 

0.2 

100.0 1 

2 

0.1 

100.0 

19 







T 

— 

100 0 

Total 

768 

100.0 


619 

100.0 


2815 

100.0 


Mean 

14.14 db 0,044 

13.67 0.049 

13.39 =i= 0.023 

Median 

14.24 years 

13.75 years 

13.41 years 




TOTAL SAMPLE POPULATION 


CORRECTED FOR 
U. S. POPULATION 


AGE 


Cases 

%of 

Population 

Cumulated 

Percent 

%of 

Population 

Cumulated 

Percent 

8 

3 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 

9 

10 

0.2 

0.3 

0.2 

0.4 

10 

93 

2.0 

2.3 

1.6 

2.0 

11 

304 

6.6 

8.9 

4.9 

6.9 

12 

1035 

22.5 

31.4 

18.7 

25.6 

13 

1507 

32.8 

64.2 

29.1 

54.7 

14 

1209 

26.3 

90.5 

32.4 

87.1 

15 

316 

6.9 

97.4 

9.6 

96.7 

16 

85 

1.9 

99.3 

2.6 

99.3 

17 

23 

0.5 

99.8 

0.6 

99.9 

18 

4 

0.1 

99.9 

0.2 

100.0 

19 

1 

0.1 

100.0 

— 

100.0 

Total 

4590 

100.0 

100.0 


Mean 

Median 

13.55 ± 0.018 

13.56 years 


Table 37. Ages at onset of adolescence 

Comparing development for three groups defined in accordance with the years of schoohng 
ultimately attained. Figures for the U. S. population are based on the figures for the sample 
population corrected for the educational distribution shown ih the U. S. Census for 1940. 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


189 


Since so many developments are involved, it is difficult to mark a single 
point at which an individual may be said to have begun adolescence. In 
the case of the male, it is not customary to attach that distinction to the 
very first appearance of any adolescent change, but to pay more attention 
to the time of first ejaculation, or to evidence that the boy would be 
capable of ejaculation if the proper opportunity were at hand. We have, 
to a large degree, followed this convention, in order that the calculations 
may be compared with other published figures. If the year of first ejacula- 
tion coincides with the year in which the pubic hair first appears, with the 
time of onset of growth in height, or with other developments, there is no 
question involved. If first ejaculation follows these other events by a year 


ioo 


80 


60 


UJ 

CL 

40 


20 


0 

Figure 29, Age at onset of adolescence, by three educational levels 

Curve for total population, on the basis of the U. S. Correction, is shown in the broken 
line. 

or more, the record must be examined to see whether there was overt 
sexual behavior which would have provided previous opportunity for 
orgasm, and the reliability of the record on the other adolescent characters 
must be checked. First ejaculation which is derived from nocturnal dreams 
usually occurs a year or more after the onset of other adolescent characters 
and after ejaculation would have been possible by other means, if circum- 
stances had aUowed. Taking these several thmgs into account, “adolescent 
ages” have been assigned to each of the subjects in the present study, and 
the distribution is shown in Table 37, Figure 29. When computed thus 
the average age of onset of adolescence in the white male is about 13 
years and 7 months. 


mi 




1 

1 



n 


EDUC l^\ 

EL 

m 





EDUC 

LEVEL 9 - 

M 

m 



1 

■ 

i 

m 

m 

DC LEVEL 

0 

1 

CD 


■ 


■ 


■ 






9 10 II 12 13 14 15 16 17 

AGE 



190 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


For the U. S. population, the sources of first ejaculation (Table 38, 
Figure 30) are, in order of frequency, masturbation (in about two-thirds 
of the males), nocturnal emissions (in an eighth of the cases), heterosexual 
coitus (in one boy out of eight), and homosexual contacts (in one boy out 
of twenty), with spontaneous ejaculation, petting to climax, and inter- 
course with other animals as less frequent stimuli for the initial experience 
{cf, Rohleder 1921). There are considerable differences in first sources in 
different educational levels. The highest incidence of masturbation as the 
first source of ejaculation occurs among the boys who will leave school 



NUMBER OF ADOLESCENT 








MALES 


PERCENT 

OF POPULATION 

SOURCE 











Popu- 

Educ. 

Educ. 

Educ. 

Popu- 

Educ. 

Educ. 

Educ. 

Total 


lat. in 

Level 

Level 

Level 

lat. in 

Level 

Level 

Level 

u. s. 


Sample 

0-8 

9-12 

13+ 

Sample 

0-8 

9-12 

134- 

Popu- 

lation 

No Ejaculation 1 

14 

4 1 

2 

3 

0.4 

0.6 j 

0.4 

0.1 

0.42 

Masturbation 

2378 

455 

346 

1293 

66.2 

68.2 

70.1 

62.2 

68.39 

Noct. Emiss. 

798 

47 

58 

654 

22.2 

7.1 

11.7 

31.4 

13.11 

Petting 

13 

2 

2 

9 

0.4 

0.3 

0.4 

0.4 

0.37 

Coitus 

222 

123 ! 

60 

' 29 

6.2 1 

18.5 i 

12.1 

1.4 

12.53 

Homosexual 

103 

32 

23 

39 

2.9 1 

4.8 

4.7 

1.9 

4.33 

Animal Coitus 

6 

0 

0 

6 

r 0.2 

0.0 

0.0 

0.3 

0.04 

Spontaneous 1 

54 

3 

3 

48 

1.5 

0.5 

0.6 

2.3 

0.81 

Total 

3588 

666 

494 

2081 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.00 


Table 38. Sources of first ejaculation 

The final column shows percent involved if each educational level were represented 
in the proportions shown in the 1940 Census. 


between the ninth and twelfth grades, the highest incidence of nocturnal 
emissions as the first source occurs among the boys who will subsequently 
go to college, and the highest incidence of heterosexual intercourse as the 
first source occurs among the boys who never get beyond the eighth grade 
in school. 

While “spontaneous” ejaculation, meaning ejaculation without specific 
genital contact, is the first source of experience for only a small percentage 
of the boys (0.81%), the items which stimulate such response constitute 
an interesting list which includes non-sexual and more definitely sexual 
emotional situations, and a variety of circumstances which involve physical 
tension. In a number of cases {e.g., wrestling, prolonged sitting while 



EARLY SEXUAL GROWTH AND ACTIVITY 


191 


reading a book) both physical tension and psychologic stimulation are 
probably involved. The list includes a number of the non-sexual sources of 
erotic stimulation listed earlier in this chapter, but the following tabulation 
shows items which are responsible for actual ejaculation among these 
early adolescent boys. 


SOURCES OF FIRST SPONTANEOUS EJACULATION 
Chiefly Physical Stimulation 


Sitting at desk 
Sitting in classroom 
Lying still on floor 
Lying still in bed 
Urination 
At toilet 

General stimulation in bath 
Moving water in bath 
General stimulation with towel 
General skin irritation 
Vibration of a boat 


Sliding on chair 
Sliding down a bannister 
Tension in gymnastics 
Chinning on bar 
Climbing tree, pole or rope 
(A rather common source) 
Wrestling with female 
Wrestling with male 
Riding an automobile 
Tight clothing 


Chiefly Emotional Stimulation 

Day dreaming ' Milking a cow 

Reading a book When scared at night 

Walking down a street When bicycle was stolen 

In vaudeville A bell ringing 

In movies An exciting basketball game 

Kissed by female Trying to finish an examination in 

Watching petting • school 

Peeping at nude female Reciting in front of class 

Sex discussion at YMCA Injury in a car wreck 

Beyond earliest adolescence, it is a rare male who ejaculates when no 
physical contact is involved. Many teen-age and even older males come to 
climax in heterosexual petting that may not involve genital contacts ; but 
general body contact, or at least lip contact, is usually included in such 
situations. There are stray cases of males of college age ejaculating under 
the excitement of class recitation or examination, in airplanes during com- 
bat, and under other rare circumstances. There are two cases of older males 
who could reach climax by deliberate concentration of thought on erotic 
situations ; but such spontaneous ejaculation is almost wholly confined to 
younger boys just entering adolescence. 

After the initial experience in ejaculation, practically all males become 
regular in their sexual activity. This involves monthly, weekly, or even 
daily ejaculation, which occurs regularly from the time of the very first 
experience. Among approximately 4600 adolescent males, less than one 
per cent (about 35 cases) record a lapse of a year or more between their 
first experience and the adoption of a regular routine of sexual activ- 



SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


192 


US. POPULATION 


MASTURBATION 

NOCTURNAL 

EMISSION 

INTERCOURSE 

HOMOSEXUAL 

CONTACT 

OTHER 



MASTURBATION 

NOCTURNAL 

EMISSION 

INTERCOURSE 

HOMOSEXUAL 

CONTACT 

OTHER 


■ 


0-8 


MASTURBATION 

NOCTURNAL 

EMISSION 

INTERCOURSE 

HOMOSEXUAL 

CONTACT 

OTHER 


9-12 




MASTURBATION 

NOCTURNAL 

EMISSION 

INTERCOURSE 

HOMOSEXUAL 

CONTACT 

OTHER 



Figure 30. Sources of first ejaculation 

Calculated for total population corrected for U. S. Census distribution, and for boys 
of the grade school (0-8), high school (9-12), and college (13+) levels. 


ity. This means that more than 99 per cent of the boys begin regular sexual 
lives immediately after the first ejaculation. In this respect, the male is 
again very different from the female, for there are many women who go 
for periods of time ranging from a year to ten or twenty years between 
their earlier experiences and the subsequent adoption of regularity m 
activity. The male, in the course of his life, may change the sources of his 
sexual outlet, and his frequencies may vary through the weeks and months, 
and over a span of years, but almost never is there a complete cessation of 
his activity until such time as old age finally stops all response. 




Chapter 6 

TOTAL SEXUAJL OUTLET 

As previously noted, the six chief sources of orgasm for the human male 
are masturbation, nocturnal emissions, heterosexual petting, heterosexual 
intercourse, homosexual relations, and intercourse with animals of other 
species. The sum of the orgasms derived from these several sources con- 
stitutes the individual’s total sexual outlet. 

Since practically all of the sexual contacts of the mature male involve 
emotional changes, all of which represent expenditures of energy, all adult 
contacts might be considered means of outlet, even though they do not 
lead to orgasm. These emotional situations are, however, of such variable 
intensity that they are difficult to assess and compare; and, for the sake of 
achieving some precision in analysis, the present discussion of outlets is 
confined to those instances of sexual activity which culminate in orgasm. 

FREQUENCY OF TOTAL OUTLET 

There are some individuals who derive 100 per cent of their outlet from 
a single kind of sexual activity. Most persons regularly depend upon two 
or more sources of outlet; and there are some who may include all six of 
them in some short period of time. The mean number of outlets utilized by 
our more than 5000 males is between 2 and 3 (means of 2.5 or 2.2) (Table 
39). This number varies considerably with different age groups and with 
different social levels (Figure 35; Chapters 7, 10). 

There are, both theoretically and in actuality, endless possibilities in 
combining these several sources of outlet and in the extent to which each 
of them contributes to the total picture (Figure 31). The record of a single 
sort of sexual activity, even though it be the one most frequently employed 
by a particular group of males, does not adequately portray the whole 
sexual life of that group. Published figures on the frequency of marital 
intercourse, for instance (Pearl 1925), cannot be taken to be the equivalent 
of data on the frequency of total outlet for the married male; for marital 
intercourse may provide as little as 62 per cent of the orgasms of certain 
groups of married males (Table 97). Similarly, studies of masturbation 
among college and younger students are not the equivalents of studies of 
total sexual outlet for such a group. Again, many persons who are rated 
“homosexual” by their fellows in a school community, a prison population, 
or society at large, may be deriving only a small portion of their total outlet 
from that source. The fact that such a person may have had hundreds of 

193 



194 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


heterosexual contacts will, in most cases, be completely ignored. Even 
psychologic studies have sometimes included, as “homosexual,” persons 
who were not known to have had more than a single overt experience. In 
assaying the significance of any particular activity in an individual history, 
or any particular type of sexual behavior in a population as a whole, it is 
necessary to consider the extent to which that activity contributes to the 
total picture. Since all previously published rates on human sexual activity 
have been figures for particular outlets, such as masturbation or marital 
intercourse, the figures given in the present study on total outlet are higher 
than previous data would have led one to expect. 



SAMPLE POPULATION 

u. 

S. POPULATION 

NO. OF 

SOURCES 


%0f 

Cumu- 

Cases 

%0f 

Cumu- 


Cases 

Popula- 

tion 

lated 

Percent 

per 

10,000 

Popula- 

tion 

lated 

Percent 

0 

263 

2.2 

100.0 

199 

1.99 

100.00 

1 

2,169 

18.4 

97.8 

2,579 

1 25.79 

98.01 

2 

3,834 

32.4 

79.4 

3,314 

33.14 

72.22 

3 

3,478 

29.5 

47.0 

2,742 

27.42 

39.08 

4 

1,690 

14.3 

17.5 

974 

9.74 

11.66 

5 

342 

2.9 

3.2 

179 

1.79 

1.92 

6 

33 

0.3 

0.3 

. 13 

0.13 

0.13 

Total 

11,809 


1 

10,000 



Mean 

2.45 =1= 0.01 


2.22 


Median 


2.91 



2.67 



Table 39. Number of sources of outlet in any 5-year period 

Computed for the whole population involved in the present study, and computed 
for a theoretic adult male population with the age distribution found m the U. S, 
Census for 1940. 


The average (mean) frequency of total sexual outlet for our sample of 
3905 white males ranging between adolescence and 30 years of age is 
nearly 3.0 per week. It is precisely 2.88 for the total population of that age, 
or 2.94 for the sexually active males in that population (Table 40, Figure 
32). For the total population, including all persons between adolescence 
and 85 years of age, the mean is 2.74 (Figure 33). 

These average figures, however, are not entirely adequate, for they are 
based upon the particular groups of males who have contributed so far to 
this study. Subsequent analyses will show that there are differences in 
mean frequencies of sexual activity, dependent upon such factors as age, 
marital status, educational, religious, and rurahurban backgrounds, and 



TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 


195 


on other biologic and social factors. In order to be intelligible, any 
discussion of sexual outlet should be confined to a particular group of 
persons whose biologic condition, civil status, and social origins are homo- 
geneous. Most of the present volume is concerned with the presentation of 
data for such homogeneous groups. If there is any advantage in having a 
generalized figure for the population of the country as a whole, that figure 
is best calculated by determining the frequencies for a variety of these 
homogeneous groups, determining the relative size of each of these groups 
in the national census, and then, through a process of weighting of means, 
reconstructing the picture for a synthetic whole (Chapter 3, Tables 7-1 1). 

For this synthesized population, which more nearly represents the con- 
stitution of the nation as a whole, we arrive at a figure of 3.27 per week for 
the total sexual outlet of the average white American male under thirty 
years of age (Table 40). For all white males up to age 85, the corrected 
mean is 2.34 per week. The latter figure is lower because of the inactivity 
of the older males. 

INDIVIDUAL VARIATION 

While approximately 3.3 is .the mean frequency of total outlet for 
younger males, no mean nor median, nor any other sort of average, can be 
significant unless one keeps in mind the range of the individual variation 
and the distribution of these variants in the population as a whole. This 
is particularly true in regard to human sexual behavior, because differences 
in behavior, even in a small group, are much greater than the variation in 
physical or physiologic characters (Table 40, Figures 32, 33). There are a 
few males who have gone for long periods of years without ejaculating: 
there is one male who, although apparently sound physically, had ejacu- 
lated only once in thirty years. There are others who have maintained 
average frequencies of 10, 20, or more per week for long periods of time; 
one male (a scholarly and skilled lawyer) has averaged over 30 per week 
for thirty years (Table 43). This is a difference of several thousand 
times. 

In considering structural characters of plants and animals, such as 
total height in the human, or length of wings, legs or other parts in other 
animals, a maximum that was two or three times the size of the minimum 
would command considerable attention (Bateson 1894, Wechsler 1935, 
Thorndike 1940). One of us has published data (Kinsey 1942) on individual 
variation in populations of insects. The populations represented individ- 
uals of single species, from single localities. There were many characters 
which varied. Extreme wing lengths, for instance, varied between 10 and 
180 micrometer units. This difference of 18 times probably represents as 
extreme a linear variation as is known in any population of adults of any 
species of plant or animal. But differences between the extreme frequencies 
of sexual outlet in the human (Figures 32-33) range far beyond these 




TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 


197 


morphologic differences. Calculation will show that the difference between 
one ejaculation in thirty years and mean frequencies of, say, 30 ejaculations 
per week throughout the whole of thirty years, is a matter of 45,000 times. 
This is the order of the variation which may occur between two individuals 
who live in the same town and who are neighbors, meeting in the same 
place of business, and coming together in common social activities. These 
sexually extreme individuals may be of equal significance, or insignificance, 
in the societal organization. They may be considered as very similar sorts 
of persons by their close friends who do not know their sexual histories. 
It has been notable throughout our field collections that a sample of as few 
as a hundred histories is likely to show a considerable portion of this full 
range of variation. 

These differences in frequency of sexual activity are of great social 
importance. The publicly pretended code of morals, our social organiza- 
tion, our marriage customs, our sex laws, and our educational and reli- 
gious systems are based upon an assumption that individuals are much 
ahke sexually, and that it is an equally simple matter for all of them to 
confine their behavior to the single pattern which the mores dictate. Even 
in such an obviously sexual situation as marriage, there is little considera- 
tion, under our present custom, of the possibility that the two persons who 
have mated may be far apart in their sexual inclinations, backgrounds, 
and capacities. Persons interested in sex education look for a program 
which will satisfy children — meahing all the children — at some particular 
educational level, overlooking the fact that one individual may be adapted 
to a particular, perhaps relatively inactive, sort of sexual adjustment, while 
the next would find it practically impossible to confine himself to such a 
low level of activity. In institutional management, there has been almost 
complete unawareness of these possible differences between inmates. The 
problems of sexual adjustment for persons committed to penal, mental, 
or other institutions, the problems of sexual adjustment for men and 
women in the army, the navy, or other armed forces, are a thousand differ- 
ent problems for any thousand of the persons involved. 

While the curve shows three-quarters (77.7%) of the males with a range 
of variation that lies between 1.0 and 6.5 per week, there is still nearly a 
quarter (22.3%) of the males who fall into extreme ranges (total population, 
U. S. Correction). There are, for instance, 7.6 per cent of all the males 
whose outlets may average 7 or more per week for periods of at least five 
years in some part of their lives. Daily and more than daily arousal and 
sexual activity to the point of complete orgasm must occur among some 
of the friends and acquaintances which any person has. When the data on 
the female are subsequently published, they will show that there is even a 
wider range of variation there, although a larger number of the females 
are in the lower portion of the curve. 



Individual Variation in Total Sexual Outlet 


FRE- 

QUENCIES 

PER 

WEEK 

YOUNGER ages: 

adolescent to 30 

ALL ages: 
adolescent to 85 

Cases 

Sample 

Population 

% 

u. s. 

Population 

% 

Cases 

! 

Sample 

Population 

/o 

u. s. 

Population 

/o 

0.0 

232 

2.0 

1.7 

291 

2.1 

1.3 



192 

1.7 

1.2 

260 

1.8 

1.3 

0.5 

1136 

9.9 

8.3 

1491 

10.6 

12,1 

1.0 

1397 

12.2 

11.3 

1852 

13.2 

14.8 

1.5 

1235 

10.8 

11.2 

1579 

11.2 

11.4 

2.0 

1240 

10.8 

10.4 

1606 

11.4 

13.3 

2,5 

1066 

9.3 

9.6 

1299 

9.2 

9.6 

3.0 

979 

8.5 

7.6 

1194 

8.5 

7.7 

3.5 

910 

7.9 

8.1 

1049 

7.4 

6.2 

4.0 

622 

8.5 

5.2 

717 

5.1 

4.3 

4.5 

455 

4.0 

4.1 

529 

3.8 

3.1 

5.0 

411 

3.6 

3.5 

446 

3.2 

2.3 

5.5 

267 

2.3 

2.2 

298 

2.1 

1.7 

6,0 

249 

2.2 

2.5 

279 

2.0 

2.3 

6.5 

158 

1.4 

1.5 

169 

1.2 

1.0 

7.0 

189 

1.6 

1.9 

208 

1.5 

1.2 

7.5 

122 

1.1 

1.2 

127 

0.9 

0.7 

8.0 

99 

0.9 

1.4 

105 

0.7 

0.7 

8.5 

65 

0.6 

0.7 

69 

0.5 

0.4 

9.0 

33 

0.3 

0.2 

44 

0.3 

0.2 

9.5 

40 

0.3 

0.3 

46 

0.3 

0.3 

10.0 

68 

0.6 

0,8 

71 

0.5 

0.6 

11.0 

61 

0.5 

0.9 

67 

0,5 

0.5 

12.0 

43 

0,4 

0.5 • 

49 

0,3 

0.3 

13,0 

30 

0.3 

0.6 

39 

0.3 i 

0.4 

14.0 

31 

0.3 

0.5 

36 

0.3 

0.4 

15.0 

25 

0.2 

0.4 

33 

! 0.2 

0.3 

16.0 

19 

0.2 

0.3 

23 

0.2 

0.5 

17.0 

18 

0.2 

0.4 

20 

! 0.1 

0.2 

18.0 

10 

0.1 

0.1 

12 

0.1 

0.1 

19.0 

10 

0.1 

0.2 

12 

0.1 

0,1 

20.0 

9 

0.1 

0.2 

11 

0.1 

0.2 

21.0 

5 

— 

0.2 

6 

0.1 

0.1 

22.0 

8 

0.1 

0.1 

10 

0.1 

‘ 0.1 

23.0 

6 

— 

0.1 

6 





24.0 

2 

— 

— 

2 





25.0 

6 

— 

0.1 

6 

— 

0.1 

26.0 

4 

— 

0.2 

1 5 

— 

0.1 

27.0 

3 

— 

0.1 

3 





28.0 

0 

0 

— 

0 

0 



29.+ 

12 

0.1 

0.2 

14 

0.1 

0.1 

Total 

11467 

100.0 

100.0 

14083 

100.0 

100.0 

Mean 

Median 

2.88 0.027 

2.14 

3.27 

2.74 0.024 

1,99 

2.34 


Table 40. Individual variation in frequency of total sexual outlet 

Raw data, based on the available sample, are corrected for a population of the same 
age, marital status, and educational level as that shown for the total population in the 
U. S. Census of 1940. 


198 



TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 


199 


The possibility of any individual engaging in sexual activity at a rate 
that is remarkably different from one’s own, is one of the most difficult 
things for even professionally trained persons to understand. Meetings of 
educators who are discussing sex instruction and policies to be followed in 
the administration of educational institutions, may bring out extreme 
differences of opinion which range from recommendations for the teaching 
of complete abstinence to recommendations for frank acceptance of almost 
any type of sexual activity. No other subject will start such open dissension 
in a group, and it is difficult for an observer to comprehend how objective 
reasoning can lead to such different conclusions among intelligent men and 
women. If, however, one has the histories of the educators involved, it may 
be found that there are persons in the group who are not ejaculating more 
than once or twice a year, while there may be others in the same group 
who are experiencing orgasm as often as ten or twenty times per week, and 
regularly. There is, inevitably, some correlation between these rates and 
the positions which these persons take in a public debate. On both sides 
of the argument, the extreme individuals may be totally unaware of the 
possibility of others in the group having histories that are so remote from 
their own. In the same fashion, we have listened to discussions of juvenile 
delinquency, of law enforcement, and of recommendations for legislative 
action on the sex laws, knowing that the policies that ultimately come out 
of such meetings would reflect the attitudes and sexual experience of the 
most vocal members of the group, rather than an intelligently thought-out 
program established on objectively accumulated data. 

Even the scientific discussions of sex show little understanding of the 
range of variation in human behavior. More often the conclusions are 
limited by the personal experience of the author. Psychologic and psychia- 
tric literature is loaded with terms which evaluate frequencies of sexual 
outlet. But such designations as infantile, frigid, sexually under-developed, 
under-active, excessively active, over-developed, over-sexed, hypersexual, 
or sexually over-active, and the attempts to recognize such states as 
nymphomania and satyriasis as discrete entities, can, in any objective 
analysis, refer to nothing more than a position on a curve which is con- 
tinuous. Normal and abnormal, one sometimes suspects, are terms which 
a particular author employs with reference to his own position on that 
curve. 

The most significant thing about this curve (Figures 32, 33) is its con- 
tinuity. It is not symmetrical, with a particular portion of the population 
set oflF as “normal,” “modal,” “typical,” or discretely different. No 
individual has a sexual frequency which differs in anything but a slight 
degree from the frequencies of those placed next on the curve. Such a 
continuous and widely spread series raises a question as to whether the 
terms “normal” and “abnormal” belong in a scientific vocabulary. At 




Figures 32 - 33 , Individual variation in frequency of total sexual outlet 








TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 


201 


the best, abnormal may designate certain individuals whose rates of activ- 
ity are less frequent, or whose sources of sexual outlet are not as usual in 
the population as a whole; but in that case, it is preferable to refer to such 
persons as rare, rather than abnormal. Moreover, many items in human 
sexual behavior which are labelled abnormal, or perversions, in textbooks, 
prove, upon statistical examination, to occur in as many as 30 or 60 or 75 
per cent of certain populations (see later chapters). It is difficult to main- 
tain that such types of behavior are abnormal because they are rare. 

The term “abnormal” is applied in medical pathology to conditions 
which interfere with the physical well-being of a living body. In a social 
sense, the term might apply to sexual activities which cause social malad- 
justment. Such an application, however, involves subjective determinations 
of what is good personal living, or good social adjustment; and these 
things are not as readily determined as physiologic well-being in an organic 
body. It is not possible to insist that afiy departure from the sexual mores, 
or any participation in socially taboo activities, always, or even usually, 
involves a neurosis or psychosis, for the case histories abundantly demon- 
strate that most individuals who engage in taboo activities make satisfac- 
tory social adjustments. There are, in actuality, few adult males who are 
particularly disturbed over their sexual histories. Psychiatrists, clinical 
psychologists, and others who deal with cases of maladjustment, some- 
times come to feel that most people find difficulty in adjusting their sexual 
lives ; but a clinic is no place to secure incidence figures. The incidence of 
tuberculosis in a tuberculosis sanitarium is no measure of the incidence of 
tuberculosis in the population as a whole; and the incidence of disturbance 
over sexual activities, among the persons who come to a clinic, is no 
measure of the frequency of similar disturbances outside of clinics. The 
impression that such “sexual irregularities” as “excessive” masturbation, 
pre-marital intercourse, responsibility for a pre-marital pregnancy, extra- 
marital intercourse, mouth-genital contacts, homosexual activity, or ani- 
mal intercourse, always produce psychoses and abnormal personalities 
is based upon the fact that the persons who do go to professional sources 
for advice are upset by these things. 

It is unwarranted to believe that particular types of sexual behavior are 
always expressions of psychoses or neuroses. In actuality, they are more 
often expressions of what is biologically basic in mammalian and anthro- 
poid behavior, and of a deliberate disregard for social convention. Many 
of the socially and intellectually most significant persons in our histories, 
successful scientists, educators, physicians, clergymen, business men, and 
persons of high position in governmental affairs, have socially taboo items 
in their sexual histories, and among them they have accepted nearly the 
whole range of so-called sexual abnormahties. Among the socially most 
successful -and personally best adjusted persons who have contributed to 



202 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


the present study, there are some whose rates of outlet are as high as those 
in any case labelled nymphomania or satyriasis in the literature, or recog- 
nized as such in the clinic. 

Clinical subjects who have such unusual items in their histories often do 
present psychopathologies — ^that is why they have gone to the clinics. But 
the presence of particular behavior, or the existence of a high rate, is not the 
abnormality which needs explanation. The real clinical problem is the 
discovery and treatment of the personality defects, the mental difficulties, 
the compulsions, and the schizophrenic conflicts which lead particular 
individuals to crack up whenever they depart from averages or socially 
accepted custom, while millions of other persons embrace the very same 
behavior, and may have as high rates of activity, without personal or 
social disturbance. It has been too simple a solution to discover the sexual 
items in a patient’s history, to consider them symptoms of a neurosis, and 
to diagnose the disturbance as the outcome of the departure from the 
established mores. It is much more difficult to discover the bases of the 
unstable personalities that are upset by such sexual departures, and to 
treat the basic defects rather than to patch up the particular issues over 
which the disturbances occur. Clinicians would have more incentive for 
using such an approach if they were better acquainted with the normal 
frequencies of the so-called abnormal types of activity, and if, at least as 
far as sex is concerned, they could acquire a wider acquaintance with the 
sexual histories of well-adjusted individuals. 

Most of the complications which are observable in sexual histories are 
the result of society’s reactions when it obtains knowledge of an individ- 
ual’s behavior, or the individual’s fear of how society would react if he 
were discovered. In various societies, under various circumstances, and 
(as we shall later show) even at various social levels of the population living 
in a particular town, the sex mores are fundamentally different. The way in 
which each group reacts to a particular sort of history determines the 
“normality” or “abnormality” of the individual’s behavior — in that partic- 
ular group (Benedict 1934). Whatever the moral interpretation (as in 
Moore 1943), there is no scientific reason for considering particular types 
of sexual activity as intrinsically, in their biologic origins, normal or abnor- 
mal. Yet scientific classifications have been nearly identical with theologic 
classifications and with the moral pronouncements of the English common 
law of the fifteenth century. This, in turn, as far as sex is concerned, was 
based on the medieval ecclesiastic law which was only a minor variant of 
the tenets of ancient Greek and Roman cults, and of the Talmudic law 
(Angus 1925, May 1931); Present-day legal determinations of sexual acts 
which are acceptable, or “natural,” and those which are “contrary to 
nature” are not based on data obtained from biologists, nor from nature 
herself. On the contrary, the ancient codes have been accepted by laymen, 



TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 


203 


jurists, and scientists alike as the ultimate sources of moral evaluations, of 
present-day legal procedure, and of the list of subjects that may go into a 
textbook of abnormal psychology. In no other field of science have scien- 
tists been satisfied to accept the biologic notions of ancient jurists and 
theologians, or the analyses made by the mystics of two or three thousand 
years ago. Either the ancient philosophers were remarkably well-trained 
psychologists, or modern psychologists have contributed little in defining 
abnormal sexual behavior. 

The reactions of our social organization to these various types of 
behavior are the things that need study and classification. The mores, 
whether they concern food, clothing, sex, or religious rituals, originate 
neither in accumulated experience nor in scientific examinations of objec- 
tively gathered data. The sociologist and anthropologist find the origins 
of such customs in ignorance and superstition, and in the attempt of each 
group to set itself apart from its neighbors. Pyschologists have been too 
much concerned with the individuals who depart from the group custom. 
It would be more important to know why so many individuals conform 
as they do to such ancient custom, and what psychology is involved in the 
preservation of these customs by a society whose individual members 
would, in most cases, not attempt to defend all of the specific items in that 
custom. Too often the study of behavior has been little more than a ration- 
alization of the mores masquerading xmder the guise of objective science. 

While this problem will be m6t again in other places, the present dis- 
cussion of frequencies of total sexual outlet provides a good opportunity 
for understanding the futility of classifying individuals as normal or abnor- 
mal, or well-adjusted or poorly adjusted, when in reahty they may be 
nothing more than frequent or rare, or conformists or non-conformists 
with the socially pretended custom. 

FACTORS EFFECTING VARIATION 

Morphologic differences between individuals are the product of both 
hereditary and environmental factors. Differences in behavior, on the 
other hand, are dependent not only upon hereditary morphology and upon 
the direct effects of environment on that anatomy, but upon psychologic 
conditioning and social pressures as well. Because of the larger number 
of factors involved, variation in behavior is much greater than variation 
in anatomic structures. 

The most important biologic factors affecting the nature and frequency 
of sexual response in the human animal are the hereditary forces which 
account for the differences between male and female. Within either of these 
sexes, heredity must also account for some of the variation in sensory 
structures and in the mechanisms which are concerned with emotional 
response; but there is little precise information on this point. Variation 



204 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


within the lifetime of a single individual is effected by such biologic factors 
as age, general metabolic level, nutrition (Miles 1919, Jackson 1925), 
vitamins (Biskind and Falk 1943, Moore 1942), general health, changes in 
neurologic conditions, and still other situations. Age is the one biologic 
factor that most strongly affects variation in the sex life of an individual 
and which, therefore, accounts for the differences between populations of 
different age (Chapter 7). Sex hormones are the biologic factors with which 
there has been the most experimentation. In general, an increased avail- 
ability of male hormone (up to the point of its optimum effect) increases the 
frequency of sexual activity (Hamilton 1937, Moore 1942, Pratt 1942, 
Lisser and Curtis 1943, Heller, Nelson and Roth 1943). Less often noted 
in the literature and less widely utilized for experimentation, thyroid 
hormones produce, if anything, more marked results, and our histories 
include some persons who have had the intensity of their sex drive and the 
frequency of their activity considerably increased by the administration of 
thyroid extracts. Since thyroid so directly affects the general metabolic 
level, it is probable that its influence on sexual frequencies is by way of its 
relation to metabolism in general rather than through any immediate 
action of the hormone. The master gland of the hormonal system, the 
pituitary, regulates both thyroid and sex glands and thus (probably in this 
indirect fashion only) affects the sexual activity of the individual. 

Psychologic conditioning accounts for a larger part of the variation in 
behavior in a population. All living organisms, from the lowest to the 
highest, are modified by the experiences through which they pass. This 
modifiability is one of the intrinsic qualities of living protoplasm. In any 
creature with a central nervous system which is as highly developed as that 
found in the vertebrates, particularly in the primates, this conditioning 
becomes a paramount factor in determining the animal’s behavior. 
Whether an individual is located at some lower point or at a higher point 
on the total curve of outlets depends in part upon the experience which he 
has previously had and the incentive which that experience provides for 
a repetition or avoidance of further activity. Whether an individual 
depends upon masturbation or heterosexual intercourse for his pre-marital 
outlet depends in part upon the early experience he happens to have had. 
Whether exclusively heterosexual or exclusively homosexual patterns are 
followed, or whether both heterosexual and homosexual outlets are 
utilized in his history, depends in part upon the circumstance of early 
experience. 

A third group of factors effecting variation in human sexual behavior is 
the sociologic group. As later chapters in this volume will show, the mores 
are the prime forces which produce variation in the sources of sexual outlet 
in different groups. Patterns of sexual behavior are, in an astonishingly 
high percentage of the cases, merely reflections of the patterns of the partic- 



TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 


205 


ular social level to which an individual belongs. In most cases, the 
individual rationalizes his particular pattern and thinks that he himself has 
logically chosen the regimen which is most satisfactory, socially profitable, 
or morally right; but mores which are many hundreds of years old are, 
in reality, the sources of most of these decisions. 

There are, then, a variety of factors which may modify the frequency 
and sources of sexual activity within the lifetime of a single individual; 
but within any limited period of time — ^within a five-year period, for in- 
stance-changes are effected chiefly by physical health and by modifica- 
tions of situations which affect the opportunity for sexual contact. Most 
individuals maintain a surprisingly constant position on the outlet curve 
for periods of several years, changing mostly because of advancing age. 

LOW FREQUENCIES AND SUBLIMATION 

It is not simple to determine the extent to which an individual’s total 
outlet represents something less than the rate to which he would rise if 
there were no restrictions on his behavior. In a few cases, however, it is 
possible to make some analyses. The rates of unmarried males between the 
ages of 16 and 20 average 3.35 (based on 2868 histories, corrected for the 
U. S. Census distribution) while the rates for married males of the same 
age group average 4.83 (Table 60). The difference of 1.5 ejaculations per 
week is to a considerable extent dependent upon the social restrictions on 
pre-marital activity (Chapter 8). k is probable that the biologic capacity 
of the average younger male is even higher than 4.8 per week, for even in 
marriage there is considerable interference with sexual performance. 
Periods of menstruation and pregnancy cause interruption of activity. 
Most males would have intercourse more frequently if their spouses were 
more interested, if other occupations did not interfere, if business routines 
that take precedence over intercourse did not leave one physically and 
mentally fatigued by the time sexual contacts are available. The human 
animal usually demands a certain privacy which is not always available 
when intercourse or other outlets are most desired; society tries to restrict 
all sexual activities to monogamous relations; and moral codes put a 
taint on naany sorts of sexual gratification. It seems safe to assume that 
daily orgasm would be within the capacity of the average human male, 
and that the more than daily rates which have been observed for some pri- 
mate species (Sokolowsky 1923, Bingham 1928, Yerkes and Elder 1936, 
Carpenter 1942, Young and Orbison 1944), could be matched by a large 
portion of the human population if sexual activity were unrestricted. The 
males who are astounded to find that 7.6 per cent of the population does, in 
actuahty, have daily or more than daily outlet are, in most cases, simply 
unaware of their own capacities. Since this percentage of the males already 
has daily rates, in spite of the restrictions on their behavior, it is probable 
that such a percentage of the population would, under optimal conditions, 



206 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


be involved in still more frequent activity during the first five or ten years of 
their adolescent and adult lives. 

In another study we will present data on the relation of sexual and 
physical activity. There is no invariable correlation, and the list of top 
athletes includes persons with both low and high rates of sexual outlet. 
On the whole it is evident that general good health and, therefore, the 
physical activity which engenders good health, may contribute to an in- 
crease in the frequency of sexual performance. Only physical exercise which 
is carried to the point of exhaustion interferes with sexual as well as other 
sorts of reactions. 

Sexual abstinence for short periods of time, such as a few days or weeks, 
is of common occurrence; but average frequencies as low as once in two 
weeks, or lower, occur in only 11.2 per cent of the males under 31 years of 
age (Table 40). Average frequencies ranging between 0.0 and once in ten 
weeks (for any five-year period under 31 years of age) occur in only about 
2.9 per cent of the. population. There is a steady increase in the number of 
low-rating males after age 35. The list may include some whose pruderies 
led them to understate the frequency of their sexual activity; but this is 
more likely to be true among the females, and it is probably not true of 
more than an insignificantly small portion of the male population, for 
most males are inclined to be ashamed of very low rates of activity. On 
the other hand, the list of inactive males includes some persons of such 
superior scientific and other professional training that there can be no 
question that their statements were as complete and accurate as could be 
made. The low-rating males have all sorts of educational, religious, and 
social backgrounds (Table 41). Larger segments of the low-rating popula- 
tion come, however, from persons with lower grade school education 
(many of whom are of lower intellectual capacity and dull sexually as well 
as mentally); from persons who are religiously most active (especially 
devout Catholics and Orthodox Jews); and particularly from males who 
are late in arriving at adolescence (Chapter 9). 

An examination of these cases of low outlet should give some informa- 
tion on the incidence of so-called sublimation. The concept, ascribed by 
the psychoanalysts to Freud (Brill, in Freud 1938), implies that it is possible 
for an individual to divert his sexual energies to such “higher levels” of 
activity as art, literature, science, and other socially more acceptable 
channels. The concept is, of course, much older than Freud. Its affinity to 
Christian, Hebraic, Greek, and more ancient asceticism is betrayed by its 
recognition of social values, and confirmed by the speed with which moral 
leaders of all denominations have adopted the term to cover everything 
that Freud originally intended, and abstinence, self-control, stern suppres- 
sion, and the rest of the ascetic virtues as well. One can hardly object if a 
supposedly scientific concept has been turned into a moral issue, when the 



208 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


179 Males with Low Outlet 


GROUP 

POPULATION 

IN TOTAL STUDY 

LOW RATING 

CASES 

% OF 

POPULATION 

AGES INVOLVED 




Adol.-15 

4102 

138 

3.4 

16-20 

3836 

80 

2.1 

21-25 

2642 

47 

1.8 

26-30 

1405 

25 

1.8 

31-35 

950 

22 

2.3 

EDUCATIONAL LEVEL 




Grades 0-4 

173 

19 

11.0 

5-8 

729 

45 

6.2 

High School 9-12 

724 

25 

3.5 

College 13-16 

1413 

43 ^ 

3.0 

Professional 17 4- 

1063 

47 

4.4 

OCCUPATIONAL CLASS 




1. Underworld 

81 

2 

2.5 

2. Day Laborers 

708 ' 

44 

6.2 

3. Semi-skilled Laborers 

839 

54 

6.4 

4. Skilled Laborers 

287 

21 

7.3 

5. Lower White Collar 

1116 

41 

1 3.7 

6. Upper White Collar 

1288 

45 

3.5 

7. Professional 

595 

29 

4.9 

8. Business Executive 

26 

1 

3.8 

RELIGION 




Protestant: Inactive 

2310 

89 

3.9 

Protestant : Active 

834 

51 

6.1 

Catholic: Inactive 

303 

9 

3.0 

Catholic: Active 

173 

13 

7.5 

Jewish: Inactive 

436 

10 

2.3 

Jewish: Active 

64 

6 

9.4 

Total: Inactive 

3049 

108 

3.5 

Total; Active 

1071 

70 

6.5 

AGE AT ONSET OF ADOLESCENCE 




9-12 

1252 

25 

2.0 

13 

1339 

40 

3,0 

14 

1093 

74 

6.8 

15 

281 

19 

6.8 

164- 

104 

8 

7.7 


Table 41. Social backgrounds of low-rating males 

Every age and every educational, reli^ous, and social background are represented. 
Larger segments of the low-rating population come from poorly educated and religiously 
devout persons, and from males who became adolescent at late ages. 



TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 


209 


(1) Among these 179 males with the lowest rates, there are a few in- 
dividuals (9 males = 5.0%) who are in such poor health, or otherwise so 
incapacitated by structural, hormonal, or other physical deficiencies, that 
all heavy expenditures of energy are impossible or held at a minimum. At 
ages over 45 there is a fair number of cases of impotency, and at younger 
ages there are a few males (4 cases cited in the previous chapter) who have 
been totally impotent throughout their fives for physical or physiologic 
reasons. There are more cases of younger males who are impotent under 
particular situations; but at ages under 36, neither erectal nor ejaculatory 
impotence accounts for more than a few stray cases of low rates of outlet. 
Other physical deficiencies are involved in the 9 cases which belong in this 
fist. 

(2) There is another group of males (at least 52.5% of the above tabula- 
tion) who are apathetic. They never, at any time in their histories, have 
given evidence that they were capable of anything except low rates of 
activity. These are persons who would be described, figuratively, as "‘low 
in sex drive.” Whether the factors are biologic, psychologic, or social, it 
is certain that such persons exist. After these apathetic persons have had 
orgasm, they may go for some days or weeks without further arousal. 
There are few if any psychologic stimuli which will excite them, and even 
when these males deliberately put themselves in erotic situations which 
involve active petting and genital manipulation they may be unable to 
respond more than once in sevei^l weeks. This situation is even more often 
found among females, 30 per cent of whom are more or less sexually un- 
responsive. Such fundamentally apathetic persons are the ones who are 
most often moral (conforming with the mores), most insistent that it is a 
simple matter to control sexual response, and most likely to offer them- 
selves as examples of the possibility of the diversion of probably non- 
existent sexual energies. But such inactivity is no more sublimation of sex 
drive than blindness or deafness or other perceptive defects are sublimation 
of those capacities. 

There is an inclination among psychiatrists to consider all unresponding 
individuals as inhibited, and there is a certain scepticism in the profession 
of the existence of people who are basically low in capacity to respond. 
This amounts to asserting that all people are more or less equal in their 
sexual endowments, and ignores the existence of individual variation. No 
one who knows how remarkably different individuals may be in mor- 
phology, in physiologic reactions, and in other psychologic capacities, 
could conceive of erotic capacities (of all things) that were basically uni- 
form throughout a population. Considerable psychiatric therapy can be 
wasted on persons (especially females) who are misjudged to be cases of 
repression when, in actuality, at least some of them never were equipped 
to respond erotically. 



210 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


(3) In this list of relatively inactive males there are 35 cases (19.6%) 
who were delayed in starting activity, but whose rates were abruptly and 
materially increased as soon as they made their first socio-sexual contacts. 
As their later performances demonstrated, their earlier rates were low only 
because their capacities had not been awakened. Having once been con- 
ditioned by sexual experience, these males subsequently found it difficult 
to get along without regular sexual outlet. Such histories are not cases of 
sublimation. 

(4) There are many cases of males of proved sexual capacity who are 
suddenly forced into relative inactivity by being deprived of opportunities 
for outlet. Sometimes this results in nervous disturbance; but where the 
individuals are effectively removed from sources of erotic arousal, most of 
them are able to adjust to the lower rates. This is best illustrated by the 
many hundreds of histories which we have from men who have been con- 
fined to penal institutions, some of them for periods of as much as twenty 
or twenty-five years. In a prison, there may be opportunity for such out- 
lets as masturbation, nocturnal emission, the homosexual, or a stray 
experience of some other sort; but the sum total of sexual activity is very 
much below that found in similar groups outside of an institution. In a 
short-time prison, the majority of the men do not accept homosexual 
contacts, and there are a great many who, coming from a social level in 
which masturbation is taboo (Chapter 10) and from a social level where 
nocturnal emissions are at a minimum (Chapters 10, 15), may go for long 
periods of months, or for a year or more, without ejaculation. A few of 
these men are nervously disturbed as a result of their lack of outlet; but 
most of them live comfortably enough, apparently because there is little 
erotic arousal which needs to be relieved by orgasm. The men in such 
institutions regularly insist that there is very little if any arousal from con- 
versation, printed pictures, descriptions in literature, or anything short of 
actual contact with a sexual partner. Educated persons are commonly 
misled by the constant discussion of sex for which prisons, armies, factories, 
and other places of partial restraint are notorious. Academically trained 
students are too prone to interpret such situations in terms of their own, 
highly conditioned, responses. For the more poorly educated portion of the 
population, however, there is a minimum of erotic fantasy, and 91.5 per 
cent of all of those committed to penal institutions never go beyond high 
school in their education (U. S. Census 1940). In consequence, these 
prison males do not illustrate sublimation, for they have little or no aroused 
sexual energy which needs dissipation. This is such a special situation that 
prison cases are not included in the above list of low-rating cases, and fre- 
quencies in prison have not entered into any of the calculations of the rates 
of outlet in the present volume. 

There are, however, males who represent cases of deprivation under 



TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 


211 


more usual situations, such as divorce, the illness of the wife, and other 
causes; and these constitute 8.3 per cent of the low-rating list given above, 

(5) Finally there are timid or inhibited individuals in this low-rating list, 
who are afraid of approaching other persons for sexual relations, afraid 
of condemnation were they to engage in such socially taboo behavior as 
masturbation, pre-marital intercourse, or the homosexual; or afraid of 
their own self condemnation if they were to engage in almost any sort of 
sexual activity. This accQjints_for more than half of the low-rating list 
(58.1%). Some of these individuals become paranoid in their fear of moral 
transgression, or its outcome. There ar,e 9 cases of attempted suicide among 
the histories of males who were trying tq^uppress some aspect of their 
sexual activity. These individuals readily acquire and accept every super- 
stitious tale concerning the consequences of masturbation; ascribe every 
pimple and stomach ache, their limitations in height and their failures in 
school or business to their occasional departures from the moral code; and 
seek religious confession, penance, and introverted solitude as means of 
avoiding further sin. Many of these individuals in actuality reduce the 
frequencies of their orgasms considerably below the level of the rest of the 
population. 

If they are better educated persons, and especially if they have some 
command of psychology, these inhibited persons rationalize more adroitly, 
admit that masturbation does no physical harm, but reason that it is bad 
to continue a habit that may ^subsequently make one unfit for normal 
marital relations, decide that pre-marital intercourse similarly unfits one 
for making satisfactory sexual adjustments in marriage, that the homo- 
sexual is a biologic abnormaUty, and that extra-marital intercourse 
inevitably destroys homes. Even among scientifically trained persons, these 
propositions are offered as excuses for their sexual inactivity. Of 58 male 
psychologists who have contributed histories to the present study, 57 have 
defended one or more of these theses, in spite of the fact that no one of 
these conclusions has ever been justified by objective data that would 
satisfy scientists in any field that did not have a moral (traditional) impli- 
cation. Out of 74 male psychiatrists who have contributed, 70 defend 
one or more of these same prejudices. These are all rationalizations, 
clutched at in support of a sexual suppression that is too often mistaken 
for sublimation. 

Recently we have secured histories from a segregated group of males, 
a high percentage of whom are sexually restrained. This has provided an 
unusual opportunity to see the results of suppression on a large scale. The 
group is not at all typical of the American population as a whole. It is 
drawn largely (82.8%) from males in their twenties (Table 42), almost 
wholly from college trained (90.3%) and white collar levels (93.3%), and 
almost wholly from Protestant religious groups (96.3%), with 43.3 per cent 



212 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


of the group actively religious, which is about double the number of actively 
religious persons in the total population on which the present study is 
based. The mean frequencies of total outlet of the segregated group, both 
in the single and the married histories, are between a half and two-thirds 
of the frequencies for corresponding age groups in the total population. 
The incidences of masturbation and of homosexual contacts in the group 
are almost identical with those found in the total population, but the 
incidence of pre-marital intercourse is definitely less (74% of the figure for 
the total population). The group has been honored by several religious 
organizations for its ideahsm and its refusal to allow any interference with 


OUTLET 

CASES IN 

RESTRAINED 

GROUP 

A Restrained Group of 134 Males 
Compared with U. S. Population 

restrained group 

u. s. population 

Total outlet 


Mean frequency 

Mean frequency 

Single males, at age: 




Adol.-15 

130 

2.26 

3.17 

16-20 

, 132 

2.11 

3.30 

21-25 

115 

1.68 

3.04 

26-30 

36 

1.65 

2.94 

Married males, at age: 




21-25 

22 

3.10 

4.14 

26-30 

24 

• 2.58 

3.51 



Accumulative 

Accumulative 



incidence 

incidence 

Masturbation 

124 

% 

92.5 

% 

93.0 

Pre-marital intercourse 

60 

44.8 

^ 85.0 

Homosexual 

44 

32.8 

35.0 


Table 42. Sexual outlet in a restrained group of males 
Compared with the U. S. population of same age and marital status. 


its ideals. Many of these males are belligerently defensive of their sexual 
philosophy. Some of them are vociferous in claiming that they are perfect 
examples of sublimation, and many outsiders look on the group as sexually 
sublimated. However, several of the members of the group were receiving 
psychiatric attention at the time of our interviews, and several psychiatrists 
have reached the conclusion that a high percentage of the whole group is 
neurotic. 

If then, from the list of low-rating males, one removes those who are 
physically incapacitated, natively low in sexual drive, sexually unawakened 
in their younger years, separated from their usual sources of sexual stimu- 
lation, or timid and upset by their suppressions, there are simply no cases 



TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 


213 


which remain as clear-cut examples of sublimation. Whether there is 
partial sublimation among individuals with higher rates of outlet, it would 
be much harder to determine. Whether there is more real sublimation 
among certain groups, as among celibate priests, is a matter that cannot be 
known until we have an adequate sample of histories from such groups. 
Certain it is that among the many males who have contributed to the present 
sample, sublimation is so subtle, or so rare, as to constitute an academic 
possibility rather than a demonstrated actuality. In view of the widespread 
and easy acceptance of the theory, and the efforts that such a large propor- 
tion of the population has made to achieve this goal, one might have 
expected better evidence of its existence, at least among the sexually least 
active 5 per cent of the males in the population. 

mCH FREQUENCIES OF OUTLET 

Since most people have only average rates of sexual outlet, many of 
them will question the accuracy of the data on persons with high frequen- 
cies. Pearl, for instance (1925), considered high frequencies as “extremely 
rare,” although he emphasized the fact that “they do occur often enough 
to show that apparently there really does exist a small but dejSnite ‘sexual 
athlete’ class of men, of which Casanova may be regarded as the classic 
prototype in literature.” However, our large sample shows that, far from 
being rare, individuals with frequencies of 7 or more per week constitute a 
not inconsiderable segment (7,^) of any population. 

There are high-rating persons of every sort, including some who are 
scientifically trained, and other reliable individuals whose records cannot 
be doubted. This high-rating population is described in Table 43. In each 
age group under 30, there are more than four times as many males as in 
any group over 50. More or less equal percentages of the high-rating males 
come from single, married, and previously married groups. Every age, 
educational, and social group is included. It is important to realize that an 
individual may be very active sexually, and of considerable significance 
socially. All religious groups, Protestant, Catholic, and Jewish, are duly 
represented by both their inactive and devout members. Nearly half 
(49,4%) of all the underworld males who have contributed to this study^ 
appear in this high-rating group, and this is further evidence for believing 
that most individuals could be much more active sexually if they were as 
unrestrained as the group that openly and regularly defies the law and the 
social convention. Fewer high-rating males come from the college level, 
but a somewhat larger number comes from the group that has professional 
training. Upper white-collar classes are rather less often represented. The 
boys who are earliest adolescent, by age twelve at the latest, are the ones 
who most often have the highest rates of outlet in the later years of their 
lives. 



462 High-Rating Males 


GROUP 

POPULAT. 

IN TOTAL 

STUDY 

OUTLETS 
7-13 PER WK. 

OUTLETS 
14-20 PER WK. 

OUTLETS 

21 + PER WK 

TOTAL WITH 
HIGH RATES 

Cases 

% 

Cases 

% 

Cases 

0/ 

/ 0 

Cases 

% 

AGES INVOLVED 










Adol.-15 

4102 

224 

5.5 

45 

1.1 

13 

0.3 

282 

6.9 

16^20 

3836 

209 

5.4 

45 

1.2 

17 

0 4 

271 

7.1 

21-25 

2642 

148 

5.6 

33 

1.3 

16 

0.6 

197 

7.5 

26-30 

1405 

76 

5.4 

21 

1.5 

10 

0.7 

107 

7.6 

31-35 

950 

27 

2.8 

12 

1.3 

6 

0.6 

45 

4.7 

36-40 

690 

16 

2.3 

6 

0.9 

4 

0.6 

26 

3.8 

41-45 

473 

9 

1.9 

6 

1.3 

0 


15 

3.2 

46-50 

320 

9 

2.8 

3 

0.9 

0 


12 

3.8 

51-55 

206 

3 

1.5 

1 

0.5 

0 


4 

1.9 

56-60 

117 

2 

1 1.7 

0 


0 


2 

1.7 

61 + 

134 

2 

1.5 

0 


0 


■ 2 

1.5 

MARITAL STATUS 










Single , 

8159 

545 

6.7 

113 

1.4 

35 

0.4 

693 

: 8.5 

Married 

2665 

179 

6.7 

56 

2.1 

30 

1.1 

265 

! 9.9 

Post-Marital 

754 

45 

6.0 

14 

1.9 

8 

1.1 

67 

8.9 

EDUCATIONAL LEVEL 










Grades 0-4 

173 

17 

9.8 i 

10 

5.8 

3 

1.7 

19 

11.0 

5-8 

729 

84 

11.5 

33 

4.5 

11 

1.5 

109 

15.0 

H. S. 9-12 

724 

88 

12 2 

19 

2.6 

9 

1.2 

99 

13.7 

College 13-16 

1413 

121 

8.6 

14 

1.0 

4 

0.3 

126 

8.9 

Professional 17+ 

1063 

100 

9.4 

11 

1.0 : 

6 

0 6 

109 

10.3 

OCCUPATIONAL CLASS 










1. Underworld 

81 

31 

38.3 

15 

18.5 

7 

8.6 

40 

49.4 

2. Day Laborers 

708 

87 

12.3 

32 

4.5 

12 ' 

1.7 

109 

15.4 

3. Semi-skilled 

839 

102 

12.2 

46 

5.5 

18 

2.2 

135 

16.1 

4. Skilled La- 




• 






borers 

287 

34 

11.8 

8 

2.8 

0 


35 

12.2 

5. Lower White 










Collar 

1116 

GO 

o 

9.7 

18 

1.6 

7 

0.6 

119 

10.7 

6. Upper White 










Collar 

1288 

106 

8.2 

15 

1.2 

6 

0.5 

115 

8.9 

7. Professional 

595 

72 

12.1 

7 

1.2 

5 : 

0.8 

74 

12.4 

AGE, ONSET ADOL. 










9-10 

91 

13 

14.3 

1 

1.1 

1 

1.1 

13 

14.3 

11-12 

1161 

147 

12.7 

43 

3.7 

16 : 

1.4 

175 

15.1 

13 

1339 

117 

8.7 

23 

1.7 

6 

0.4 

131 

9.8 

14 

1093 

94 

8.6 

17 

1.6 

9 

0.8 

103 

9.4 

15 

281 

30 

10.7 

2 

0.7 

1 

0.4 

31 

11.0 

16+ 

104 

7 

6.7 

1 

1,0 

0 

0.0 i 

8 1 

7.7 

RELIGION 










Protestant, Inac- 










tive 

2310 

242 

10.5 

54 

2.3 

20 

0.9 

271 

11.7 

Protestant, Active 

834 

54 

6.5 

12 

1.4 

3 

0.4 

62 

7.4 

Catholic, Inactive 

303 

54 

17.8 

11 

3.6 

5 

1.7 

62 

20.5 

Catholic, Active 

173 

10 

5.8 

4 

2.3 

0 

0.0 

14 

8.1 

Jewish, Inactive 

436 

50 

11.5 

6 

1.4 

5 

1.1 

53 

12.2 

Total Inactive 

3049 

346 

11.3 

71 

2.3 

30 

1.0 

386 

12.7 

Total Active 

1007 

64 

6.0 

16 

1.5 

3 

0.3 

76 

7,1 


Table 43. Social backgrounds of high-rating males 


Every age and every educational, religious, and social background are represented. Higher 
percentages come from younger age groups, from religiously less active groups, and from 
the underworld. 


214 



TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 


215 


In explanation of these high rates of outlet, it is to be noted that it is a 
common pattern for many persons to engage in intercourse every night 
or practically every night in the week; and there are many married persons, 
especially at lower social levels, who have intercourse quite regularly both 
in the evening upon retiring and in the morning upon awakening. Where 
the occupation allows the male spouse to return home at noon, contacts 
may also occur at that hour of the day and, consequently, there is a regular 
outlet of fourteen to twenty-one times per week. In the same fashion, 
masturbation, the homosexual, and still other sorts of sexual activities may 
acquire daily or more than daily frequencies. 

An even larger portion of this high-rating group secures its outlet in 
multiple ejaculations during a limited number of sexual contacts. The 
existence of multiple orgasm in the pre-adolescent male has been discussed 
in Chapter 5. A similar situation occurs, although less frequently, in the 
adult (Table 48, Figure 36). Most males occasionally ejaculate more than 
once; but there are males who regularly do so, practically every time there 
is a socio-sexual contact. Of the white males who have contributed to the 
present study and who have had experience in intercourse, 380 have had a 
history of regular, multiple ejaculation at some period in adolescence or in 
adult years. Sometimes these ejaculations, totaling two or three or more, 
are spread over several hours in a single evening, with more or less con- 
tinuous sex play; but in a fair number of cases it is habitual for a male to 
ejaculate two or more times in gontinuous intercourse and while maintain- 
ing a continuous erection (it is mentioned in Kahn 1939:110), Some 
physiologists have questioned the possibility of such a performance. Since 
multiple ejaculation in the male depends upon glandular secretion, there 
are complications which are not involved in the female, where multiple 
orgasm is better known. Nevertheless, scepticism over the possibility of 
repeated response in the male merely emphasizes the incapacity of even 
scientifically trained persons to comprehend that others may be made 
differently from themselves. Both the testimony of the performing hus- 
bands and the collaborative record obtained from their wives or other 
female partners, leave no doubt that multiple orgasm, usually with ejacula- 
tion, is the regular routine in a fair number of cases. Data from males with 
homosexual experience indicate that in such relations it is not at all infre- 
quent for a younger male to proceed to a second or a third orgasm in a 
matter of five or ten minutes. Most of these multiple climaces occur in 
younger males, but not all of them do so, as the age distribution in Table 
48 will show. Very few adult males are able to reach more than four or five 
climaces in any limited period of time; an occasional teen-age boy will 
reach six or more; and a quarter of the pre-adolescents for whom we have 
any record of orgasm were able to go beyond five, and in some cases to as 
many as ten, twenty, or more in a few hours’ time (Table 33). Wherever 
there is multiple orgasm, the total frequencies of outlet are multiplied, and 



216 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


these cases account for nearly 30 per cent of the population which lies on 
the higher portions of the curve. 

Among the more active cases in our histories, there are male prostitutes 
whose capacity to perform may determine the level of their income. Male 
prostitutes may be involved in heterosexual prostitution, which is rare, or 
in homosexual prostitution, which is much more frequent. In male prosti- 
tution, the prostitute usually experiences orgasm. This is in contrast 
to the situation among female prostitutes, most of whom go into prostitu- 
tion for the sake of the money that may be earned. In most cases the 
female prostitute is not aroused and does not experience orgasm during a 

SIX MOST ACTIVE MALES 

Maximum frequencies during 30 continuous years, in best authenticated cases 

Marital status during maximum activity 
Married (4), single (2) 

Religion 

Active Protestant (1), inactive Protestant (4), inactive Jewish (1) 

Educational level in years 
3, 16, 17, 20 (3 cases) 

Occupations 

WPA and labor, physician (2), scientific worker, educator, lawyer 
Ages at onset of adolescence 

11 (2 cases), 12, 13 (2 cases), 14 
30-year period involved 

11-^0 (2 cases), 12-41, 13-42, 16-45, 21-50 
Years of maximum frequency 

At ages 11-15, 12-15, 13-15, 21-25, 36-40 (leases) 

Average rates per week for 30-year period 
10.6, 11.7, 13.6, 14.0, 17.8, 33.1 
Range in rate during 30-year period 

5.3 to 19.6, 11.6 to 31.6, 9.0 to 13.0, 14.7 to 15.7, 10.0 to 21.5, 25.6 to 37.8 
Multiple ejaculation 

1-2, 2 when younger, 2-5, 3 and 4, in each coitus. Two cases never multiple 
Chief sources of outlet 

Masturbation, marital intercourse, and extra-marital intercourse 
Masturbation, marital intercourse, and homosexual contacts 
Masturbation and extra-marital intercourse 
Masturbation and pre-marital intercourse 
Masturbation and homosexual outlets 
Marital intercourse only 

professional contact. In male prostitution, on the contrary, in some of the 
techniques that are employed, capacity to achieve erection and ejaculate is a 
requisite part of the arrangement. Some male prostitutes ejaculate five, six, 
or more times per day with regularity over long periods of years. While the 
amount of semen per ejaculation is thereby reduced, there is usually 
emission, even with such frequent orgasms. The validity of the data on 
this point depends not only upon the records of the several hundred male 
prostitutes who have contributed their histories, but also upon the obser- 
vations of persons who have had contact with them. In a few cases, there 



total sexual outlet 


217 


are records made by persons who have observed the actual performance of 
particular male prostitutes from hour to hour, over periods of time, and 
there is no question that there is frequent arousal and actual ejaculation 
of semen five or more times per day in some of these cases. One such set 
of observations concerns a 39-year-old Negro male who had averaged 
more than three per day from 13 to 39 years of age, and at the latter age 
was still capable of 6 to 8 ejaculations when the occa^sion demanded. The 
average frequencies of such an individual carry the curve of total sexual 
outlet to unusually high points. 

The six white males with the highest long-time averages for a continuous 
30-year period deserve further examination, although it should be under- 
stood that there is a continuous list of others who grade into these highest 
cases. The males included in this list all went to considerable pains to give 
details for each of the periods involved; one is a lawyer, one is an educator, 
three of them are scientifically trained persons, two of whom had kept 
diaries or other records; and we have had such extended contact with 
three of these individuals that we feel considerable reliance can be placed 
on their data. The six cases are strikingly different in regard to ages 
involved, in the variability of the rate during the 30-year period, and in the 
sources of most of the sexual outlet. 

Outside of penal institutions and cloistered halls, there are ever-present 
stimuli to heterosexual response. For the educated portion of the male 
population, for instance, there? are persons of the opposite sex, female 
wearing apparel which emphasizes and suggests sexual situations, the 
constant portrayal of these things in magazines, in moving pictures, on 
billboards, in decorative art, in the plots of printed fiction and stage 
drama, in the emphasis given to radio performances, in advertisements 
everywhere, in most poetry and songs, and, more subtly but even more 
effectively, in all those forms and ceremonies which are accepted as 
courtesies between the sexes, and in the social traditions connected with 
marriage. There is, in consequence, constant arousal and regular sexual 
activity in most males, particularly younger males who are conditioned by 
any experience, or by the vicariously shared experiences of their fellows. 
While all males must have known of the regularity of sexual activity in 
their own histories, the significance of the fact for the population as a 
whole has never been fully appreciated. The assumption that the unmarried 
male has only occasional outlet, or that he may go for long periods of time 
without any sexual activity, is not in accord with the fact. The assumption 
that there can be such sublimation of erotic impulse as to allow an appre- 
ciable number of males to get along for considerable periods of time with- 
out sexual activity is not yet substantiated by specific data. For most males, 
whether single or married, there are ever-present erotic stimuli, and sexual 
response is regular and high. 



Chapter 7 

AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


In physiology, endocrinology, genetics, and still other fields, biologists 
often go to considerable pains to restrict their experimental material to 
animals of particular species, to particular age groups, and to individuals 
that are reared on a uniform diet and kept under strictly controlled labora- 
tory conditions. Different hereditary strains of a single species may give 
different results in a physiologic experiment; and, in many laboratories, 
stocks are restricted to the progeny of particular pairs of pedigreed ances- 
tors. In studies of human behavior, there is even more reason for confining 
generalizations to homogeneous populations, for the factors that affect 
behavior are more abundant than those that affect simpler biologic charac- 
ters, and there are, in consequence, more kinds of populations to be 
reckoned with. Nevertheless, restrictions of psychologic and sociologic 
studies to clearly defined groups have rarely been observed (McNemar 
1940), perhaps because we have not, heretofore, known what things effect 
variability in a human population and how important they are in deter- 
mining what people do. ^ 

There are at least eleven factors which are of primary importance in 
determining the frequency and sources of human sexual outlet. They are 
sex, race, age, age at onset of adolescence, marital status, educational 
level, the subject’s occupational class, the parental occupational class, 
rural-urban backgrounds, religious affliations, and the extent of the 
subject’s devotion to religious affairs. The effects of these factors on the 
sexual histories of white males are discussed in the present volume. In 
view of the conclusions that these analyses now afford, it becomes apparent 
that generalizations concerning any aspect of human sexual behavior are 
uninterpretable unless they are limited to populations which are clearly 
defined in regard to the more important of the eleven items listed above. 

In the sexual history of the male, there is no other single factor which 
affects frequency of outlet as much as age. Age affects the source of sexual 
outlet only indirectly, by way of its relation to marital status, to the avail- 
ability of social contacts, to the liability to physical fatigue, and to the 
psychologic fatigue that comes as a result of the repetition of a particular 
sort of activity. But age more directly affects frequency of outlet. Age is so 
important that its effects are usually evident, whatever the marital status, 
the educational level, the religious background, or the other factors which 

218 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 219 

enter the picture. It is logical, therefore, to begin the present analyses with 
a consideration of this factor. 

ADOLESCENT SEXUAL ACTIVITY 

As we have previously indicated (Chapter 5), there is sexual activity in 
the pre-adolescent male which may involve definite erotic arousal and 
actual orgasm; but the onset of regular sexual performance is usually 
coincidental with the onset of adolescence. Throughout the remainder of 
this volume, descriptions of sexual activity will apply to age periods that 
begin with adolescence and which extend, in the first instance, through 
15 years of age, and which are five year periods from that point through 
the remainder of the individual’s history. 

Over 95 per cent of the adolescent males are regularly active by 15 years 
of age (Table 44). Over 99 per cent of the adolescent and older males are 
active throughout the whole period from 16 to 45. In those 30 years, only 
1 or 2 per cent of the male population is without regular and usually fre- 
quent outlet. After 45 there is a gradual but distinct drop in the number of 
active cases. These generalizations apply to all white males, whether single 
or married, and whatever their educational level or social background. 

Maximum Activity. The maximum sexual frequencies (total outlet) occur 
in the teens. Frequencies then drop gradually but steadily into old age 
(Table 44, Figure 34). Considering the active, single males in the popula- 
tion, the maximum mean frequencies are almost 3.4 per week (calculated 
for the U. S. population), which is almost exactly every other day in the 
week, month, or year (Tables 45, 60, Figures 50-52). This rate is reached 
between adolescence and 20 years of age. 

The means for the married males begin at their highest point, 4.8 per 
week, between 16 and 20 (Tables 45, 60, Figures 50-52). Few males are 
married prior to 16, and there is not enough material to calculate statisti- 
cally significant averages for any married group prior to that age. It is 
probable that in a population which married at an earlier age, the highest 
frequency on the curve would come in the earlier adolescent group ; but, 
in our society as it is, the high point of sexual performance is, in actuality, 
somewhere around 16 or 17 years of age. It is not later. The data which 
have already been given on the sexual capacity of the pre-adolescent boy 
(Chapter 5) indicate that the peak of capacity occurs in the fast-growing 
years prior to adolescence; but the peak of actual performance is in the 
middle or later teens. 

The earliest serious attempt to determine the age of maximum sexual 
activity, and the effect of age on sexual performance in the human male, 
was made by Pearl (1925). For his study, he had data from 213 men (aver- 
age age 64.53 years) who felt they could recall the frequencies of marital 
intercourse in their earher histories. The point of maximum sexual activity 



220 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Total Outlet: Frequency Per Week 


AGE 

GROUP 

CASES 

TOTAL SAMPLE POPULATION 

ACTIVE IN SAMPLE 

POPULATION 

u. s, 

POPULATION 

Range 
(minus 1) 

Mean 

Me- 

dian 

%0f 

Total 

Mean 

Me- 

dian 

Mean: 

Total 

Popul. 

Mean: 

Active 

Popul. 

Adol. 

3905 

0.0-29-j- 

2.86=i= 

2.11 

95.1 

3.00=1= 

2.26 

3.17 

3.36 

-15 



0.05 



0.05 




16-20 

3750 

0.0-29-1- 

2.87 =i= 

2.17 

99.3 

2.89=1= 

2.19 

3.32 

3.37 




0.05 



0.05 




21-25 

2502 

0.0-29+ 

2,85± 

2.10 

99.6 

2.86=1= 

2.11 

3.35 

3.40 




0.06 



0.06 




26-30 

1310 

0.0-29+ 

3.01 =i= 

2.24 

99.5 

3.03=1= 

2.25 

3.35 

3.38 




0.09 



0.09 




31-35 

879 

0.0-29+ 

2.64=b 

1.91 

99.7 

2.65=1= 

1.92 

2.89 

2.90 




0.10 



0.10 




36-40 

628 

0.0-22.0 

2,36=1= 

1.73 

99.5 

2.37=1= 

1.73 

2.36 

2.36 




0.10 



0.10 




41-45 

440 

0.0-15.0 

1.98=i= 

1.41 

99.1 

2.00=t: 

1.42 

1.96* 

1.98* 




0.11 



0 11 




46-50 

285 

0.0-12.0 

1.78=1= 

1.10 

97.5 

1.82=1= 

1.15 

1.75* 

1.78* 




0.12 



0.12 




51-55 

173 

0.0-10.0 

1.50=1= 

0.90 

96^0 

1.57=1= 

0.96 






0.14 



0.14 




56-60 

106 

0. 0-9.0 

1.20=1= 

0.73 

95.3 

1.26=t 

0.79 






0.15 



0.15 




61-65 

58 

0. 0-4.0 

0.84=t: 

0.52 

81.0 

1.04=t= 

0.71 






0.16 



0.19 




66-70 

30 

0. 0-3.0 

0,65=1= 

0.30 

73.3 

0.88=1= 

0.48 






0.24 



0.31 




71-75 

12 

0.0-0. 5 

0.13=1= 

0.00 

41.7 

0.30=1= 

0.30 






0.07 



0.14 




76-80 

4 

0.0- 

0.01=1= 

0.00 

25.0 

0.05 

0.10 





scant 

0.01 







81-85 

2 

0.0 

0.00 

0.00 

0.0 

0.00 

0.00 



Total 

14084 

0.0-29+ 

2.74± 

0.02 

1.99 

91,9 

2.80=±= 

0.02 

2.04 



AdoL 

-30 

11467 

0.0-29+ 

2.88=1= 

0.03 

2.14 

98.0 

2.94=1= 

0.03 

2.20 




Table 44. Total sexual outlet in relation to advancing age 


These data are based on the total population, including single, married, and pre- 
viously married groups. For each group calculated separately, see Table 60. Data for 
the U. S. population are based on a theoretic group with the marital status and edu- 
cational levels shown in the U. S. Census for 1940. ^Starred items are corrected for 
marital status only. 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


221 


for this population was located in the 30-39 year period. For the younger 
ages, Pearl recorded definitely lower frequencies; but he concluded, as we 
have with our own data, that “the low frequency exhibited in this [younger] 
age period is in part and probably mainly an expression of an essentially 
social factor — lack of opportunity — rather than of anything physiological.” 
Of the men who were married in their twenties, 67 essayed to recall fre- 
quencies in that period; and 9 of the men who were married in their teens 
supplied data for that age. On these limited bases, Pearl concluded that 
“with approximate equality of opportunity at the different ages the peak of 
activity is in the 20-29 decade and that thereafter there is a steady decline” ; 
but after inspecting the curve, he theoretically adds that “with unrestricted 



I I I - I - I . - I I I - I ^ I 1 

-15 -20 -25 -30 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 -50 -65 -70 -75 -50 -85 


AGE GROUPS 

Figure 34. Frequency of total outlet in relation to age 

Based on total population, including single, married, and previously married groups. 
Broken lines represent raw data; the solid black line represents the mean corrected for 
the U. S. Census distribution. 

legitimate opportunity the peak of sex activity is prior to age 20.” Our own 
abundant data push the peak of the curve back, as Pearl predicted, into 
the late teens. Unfortunately, the conclusions which are more often quoted 
from the Pearl study are those based on his total population, with its 
maximal frequency between 30 and 39 years; whereas the curve which he 
derived from the smaller sample of married males, and his prediction that 
the maximum activity occurs before the twenties, prove to be the more 
correct. 

Social Significance. The identification of the sexually most active period 
as late adolescence will come as a surprise to most persons. General opinion 
would probably have placed it in the middle twenties or later. Certainly the 



222 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


average college student and the town boy of corresponding age will be 
startled to learn that their younger brothers who are still in high school 
surpass them in capacity and ofttimes in performance. By law, society 
provides a source of regular sexual outlet in marriage, in part because it 
recognizes the sexual need of the older male; but it fails to recognize that 
the teen-age boys are potentially more capable and often more active than 
their 35-year old fathers. Even among physicians and biologists, there has 
been . a general opinion that sexual capacity develops gradually in early 
adolescence, reaches its maximum in the thirties or forties (the ‘"prime of 
life”)j passes a peak somewhere in a period which is considered a male 
climacteric, and drops abruptly into the inactivity and complete impotence 
of old age. It so happens that much of this picture is correct for the female, 
but it is certainly not the pattern in the male. The preoccupation of so many 
of the previous sex studies with the female has too often led to inter- 
pretation of the male by analogy, rather than by way of data taken directly 
from him. 

This considerable activity and greater potentiality of the adolescent 
male pose a number of sociologic problems. In the normal course of events, 
the primitive human animal must have started his sexual activities with 
unrestrained pre-adolescent sex play, and begun regular intercourse well 
before the onset of adolescence. This is still the case in the other anthro- 
poids (Hamilton 1914, Kempf 1917, Bingham 1928, Nowlis 1941), in some 
of the so-called primitive human societies which have not acquired particu- 
lar sex taboos (Malinowski 1929, Ford 1945), and among such of the 
children in our society as escape the restrictions of social conventions 
(Chapter 5). The near-universality of adolescent sexual activity in our own 
Western European civilization down through the eighteenth century is 
poorly understood by those who have not made a study of earlier litera- 
ture; but there is every indication in that literature, both sober and erotic, 
that the high capacity of the younger male was recognized and rather 
widely accepted until near the Victorian day in England. The problem of 
sexual adjustment for the younger male is one which has become especially 
aggravated during the last hundred years, and then primarily in England 
and in America, under an increasing moral suppression which has coincided 
with an increasing delay in the age of marriage. This has resulted in an 
intensification of the struggle between the boy’s biologic capacity and the 
sanctions imposed by the older male who, to put it objectively, is no longer 
hard-pressed to find a legalized source of sexual contact commensurate 
with his reduced demand for outlet. 

The fact that the unmarried male still manages to find an outlet of 3.4 
per week demonstrates the failure of the attempt to impose complete 
abstinence upon him. The sources of this outlet must be a matter of 
bewilderment to those who have supposed that most males remained 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


223 


continent until marriage. Nocturnal emissions do not provide any con- 
siderable portion of the orgasms (Chapter 15), in spite of the fact that 
many persons have wished that to be the case. Masturbation is a more 
frequent outlet among the upper social level males where, during the last 
two or three decades, it has been allowed as a not too immoral substitute 
for pre-marital intercourse; but most of the less-educated 85 per cent of the 
population still consider masturbation neither moral nor normal For the 
mass of the unmarried boys, intercourse still provides the chief sexual 
activity (Chapter 10). This means that the majority of the males in the 
sexually most potential and most active period of their lives have to accept 
clandestine or illegal outlets, or become involved in psychologic conflicts 
in attempting to adjust to reduced outlets. With the data now available, 
biologists, psychologists, physicians, psychiatrists, and sociologists should 
be enabled to make better analyses of the problem which has heretofore 
been imposed on this unmarried male in his middle and late teens, and 
in his twenties. 

The situation is complicated by the fact that the average adolescent girl 
gets along well enough with a fifth as much sexual activity as the adoles- 
cent boy, and the frequency of outlet of the female in her twenties and 
early thirties is still below that of the average adolescent male. As mothers, 
as school teachers, and as voting citizens, women are primarily responsible 
for the care of these boys ; and, to a large degree, they are the ones who 
control moral codes, schedules for sex education, campaigns for law en- 
forcement, and programs for combating what is called juvenile delin- 
quency. It is obviously impossible for a majority of these women to under- 
stand the problem that the boy faces in being constantly aroused and 
regularly involved with his normal biologic reactions. 

The mean rate of outlet for the women who are young mothers and high 
school teachers lies between 0.7 and 2.1 per week (as indicated by pre- 
liminary calculations from our unpublished material on the female). 
Many of these women, including some high school biology teachers, 
believe that the ninth or tenth grade boy is still too young to receive any 
sex instruction when, in actuality, he has a higher rate of outlet and has 
already had a wider variety of sexual experience than most of his female 
teachers ever will have. Whether there should be sex instruction, and what 
sort of instruction it should be, are problems that lie outside the scope of 
an objective scientific study; but it is obvious that the development of any 
curriculum that faces the fact will be a much more complex undertaking 
than has been realized by those who think of the adolescent boy as a 
beginner, relatively inactive, and quite capable of ignoring his sexual 
development. 

Institutional Problems. The legal approach to this problem is, as usual, 
even less realistic. By making illegal aU pre-marital sexual activities except 



224 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


nocturnal emissions and solitary masturbation, English and American 
law forces most boys, as indicated above, into illicit activity. The chief 
exceptions are largely in that group that goes on to college, and which, 
coincidentally, accepts masturbation as a chief source of outlet. Precise 
incidence figures for the various types of sexual behavior which are illegal 
are given in later chapters of the present volume. On a specific calculation 
of our data, it may be stated that at least 85 per cent of the younger male 
population could be convicted as sex offenders if law enforcement officials 
were as efficient as most people expect them to be. The stray boy who is 
caught and brought before a court may not be different from most of his 
fellows, but the public, not knowing of the near universality of adolescent 
sexual activity, heaps the penalty for the whole group upon the shoulders 
of the one boy who happens to be apprehended. This situation presents a 
considerable dilemma for law enforcement officials and for students of the 
social organization as a whole. 

The problem of sexual adjustment for a younger male who is confined 
to a mental, penal, or other sort of institution is even more difficult than 
the problem of the boy who lives outside in society. Administrators who 
have these younger males in their care are generally bewildered and at a 
loss to know how to handle their sexual problems. In many cases, the 
situation is simply tolerated or ignored, and the administrator would pre- 
fer not to be aware of the actualities. For this, many people would con- 
demn him; but the problem in an institution for teen-age boys is far more 
complex than the public or the administration or scientific students have 
realized. It is obvious that lifetime patterns of sexual behavior are greatly 
affected by the experiences of adolescence, not only because they are the 
initial experiences, but because they occur during the age of greatest activ- 
ity and during the time of the maximum physical capacity of the male. 
This is the period in which the boy’s abilities to make social adjustments, 
to develop any sort of socio-sexual contacts, and to solve the issues of a 
heterosexual-homosexual balance, are most involved. Since younger boys 
have not acquired all of the social traditions and taboos on sex, they are 
more impressionable, more liable to react de novo to any and every situation 
that they meet. If these adolescent years are spent in an institution where 
there is little or no opportunity for the boy to develop his individuality, 
where there is essentially no privacy at any time in the day, and where all 
his companions are other males, his sexual life is very likely to become 
permanently stamped with the institutional pattern. Long-time confine- 
ment for a younger male is much more significant than a similar period of 
confinement for an older adult. 

The situation is aggravated by the more recent development of the 
juvenile court. Abundant as the merits of such an institution may be, there 
are complications involved when a court assumes control of a juvenile for 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


225 


a long period of years, until he is twenty or twenty-one, without, at the 
same time, considering the problems of sexual adjustment for its ward. 
The practice of the juvenile court is based on a reahzation that a child may 
need long-time training; but it ignores these other aspects of the child’s 
development. The juvenile court protects many a boy from the more 
severe sentences of the adult laws ; but it sometimes holds a juvenile for 
several years in a correctional institution, or under probation with the 
court, when the crime involved would have brought only a few months’ 
sentence on an adult criminal charge. Adult institutions often have young 
inmates who have falsified their age in order to draw the lesser time of a 
penal commitment. The juvenile court structure is disguised by a verbiage 
which avoids references to “convictions,” “sentences,” “penalties,” “years 
to serve,” “prisons,” or “penal institutions.” But in spite of the legal 
fiction, the fact remains that teen-age boys may, by order of a court, be 
held in custody, sometimes for several years, in institutions which may be 
no less repressive and punitive than the average of adult prisons. It is 
doubtful if many of these committing judges ever consider the juvenile’s 
sexual adjustment when he is sent to such an institution. Within recent 
years there has been a movement to extend the jurisdiction of the juvenile 
court to persons as old as eighteen or twenty. There are commendable 
objectives back of such moves, but no one seems to have considered the 
sexual problems that will arise from the commitment of a still larger 
portion of the teen-age population to what are in essence long-time insti- 
tutions. 

The problem is not solved by the common practice of releasing juveniles 
from institutions on long-time parole; for the terms of the parole are, in 
most states, practically as strict in regard to sexual activities as the rules 
of the institution itself. We have numerous histories of boys who have 
been paroled from such institutions to elderly persons, often on farms, 
who have no understanding of the problems of sexual adjustment of a 
younger boy, who do not comprehend the significance of his socio-sexual 
development during that period, and who beheve that such a boy should 
be kept from making even the simplest sort of social contacts with individ- 
uals of the opposite sex. If any large portion of the male population had 
been raised under such conditions, the imphcations of the situation would 
be apparent to everyone ; but since the boys who get into institutions repre- 
sent a small portion and a socially limited portion of the whole population, 
most people do not have firsthand contacts with them, and have not, 
therefore, considered the problem of sexual adjustment for institutional- 
ized boys. 

Boys who live in private boarding schools, and even boys who attend 
pubhc or private day schools that are restricted to the single sex, face some 
of the same sexual problems as the boys in a penal institution. 



226 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


SEXUAL AGING 

Having reached its peak in adolescence, sexual activity in the male 
drops steadily from then into old age (Tables 44-45, Figures 34-37). As 
far as human sexuality is concerned, aging begins at least with the onset 
of adolescence; and if the capacity (rather than the performance) of the 


Total Outlet, Marital Status, and Age 




Total Outlet: Sample Population 

Total Outlet: 

U. S. Population 

AGE 

GROUP 

CASES 

TOTAL population 

active population 

total 

POPUL. 

ACTIVE 

POPULAHON 



Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 

Mean 

Freq. 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 


Single Males 


Adol. 

-15 

3012 

2.91 ±0.05 

2.18 

95.1 

3,06 

2.33 

3.17 

94.2 

3.36 

16-20 

2868 

2.88 ±0.05 

2.19 

99.2 

2.90 

2.22 

3.30 

98.8 

3.35 

21-25 

1535 

2.67 ±0.07 

1.95 

99.1 

2.70 

1.97 

3.04 

97.9 

3.11 

26-30 

550 

2.63 ±0.11 

1.90 

99.3 

2.65 

1.92 

2.94 

98.6 

2.98 

31-35 

195 

2.38 ±0.21 

1.58 

99.0 

'‘2.40 

1.60 

2.44 

99.2 

2.46 

36-^0 

97 

2.07 ±0.21 

1.36 

97.9 

2.12 

1.39 

2.00 

98.5 

2.04 

41-45 

56 

1.79 ±0.28 

0.98 

96.4 

1.85 ■ 

1.05 




46-50 

39 

1.88 ± 0.43 

1.00 

92.3 

2.04 

1.13 





Married Males 


16-20 

272 

4.67 

=fc 

0.29 

3.21 

100.0 

4,67 

3.21 

4.83 

100.0 

4. 

83 

21-25 

751 

3.90 

=b 

0.14 

2.81 

100.0 

3.90 

2.81 

4.14 

100.0 

4. 

14 

26-30 

737 

3.27 


0.12 

2.47 

100.0 

3.27 

2.47 

3.51 

100.0 

3. 

51 

31-35 

569 

2.73 

=b 

0.11 

2.08 

100.0 

2.73 

2.08 

2.90 

100.0 

2. 

90 

36-40 

390 

2.46 

=h 

0.13 

1.89 

99,7 

2.47 

1.89 

2.42 

99.9 

2. 

42 

41-45 

272 

1.95 


0.12 

1.61 

100.0 

1.95 

1.61 

1.95 

100.0 

1. 

95 

46-50 

175 

1.79 

=fc 

0.16 

1.18 

98.9 

1.81 

1.20 

1.80 

98.1 

1. 

83 

51-55 

109 

1.54 


0.18 

1.00 

98.2 

1.57 

1.04 

1,54 

97.2 

1. 

58 

56-60 

67 

1.08 

db 

0.12 

0.79 

98.5 

1.09 

0.81 






Table 45. Total sexual outlet, marital status, and age 
For explanations, see the legend with Table 51 on masturbation. 


pre-adolescent is taken into account (Chapter 5), it seems more correct to 
think of aging as a process that sets in soon after the initiation of growth. 
The sexagenarian — or octogenarian — ^who suddenly becomes interested 
in the problems of aging is nearly a lifetime beyond the point at which he 
became involved in that process. 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


227 


It will be interesting to know how many of the other physical and 
physiologic functions of the human animal reach their prime before the 
twenties. There are a few studies of physiologic aging, and the data (just 
as with sexual aging) show a steady degeneration of capacities from the 
age of the youngest child studied. This, for instance, has been shown for 
such phenomena as basal heart rate, resting oxygen intake and intake dur- 
ing maximum work, respiratory quotient, and carbon dioxide and lactic 
acid relations during work (data and references in Robinson 1938). In 
everyday affairs, it is to be noted that armies and navies, and others who 
depend on manpower to accomplish work, know that the male in his late 
teens has physical quality, nervous coordination, and capacity for recovery 
that are beyond those of the even slightly older man. But research on 
aging has concerned itself primarily with very old individuals, and too 
often failed to consider such fundamentals as might be seen only in the 
beginnings of the processes. Aging studies need to be re-oriented around 
the origins of biologic decline, and that will mean around pre-adolescence 
or early adolescence in regard to at least some aspects of human physiology. 

From the early and middle teens, the decline in sexual activity is remark- 
ably steady, and there is no point at which old age suddenly enters the 
picture. The calculations become more significant when the single and 
married males are analyzed separately (Table 45, Figures 50-52). There 
are no calculations in all of the material on human sexuality which give 
straighter slopes than the data showing the decline with age in the total 
outlet of the single males, or the similar curve showing the decline in outlet 
for the married males. Starting from a high point of 3.2 for the single 
males, or 4.8 for the married males, in the middle teens, the mean for both 
groups drops steadily to about the same point, 1.8 per week at 50 years of 
age, to 1.3 per week at 60 years, and to 0.9 per week at 70 years of age. 

Individual males may show variations from this picture, but departures 
from a steady decline are exceptions in the population as a whole. There 
are some clinical studies (Norbury 1934, Mead and Stith 1940, Heller 
and Myers 1944^ Bauer 1944, Werner 1945, et al.) which seem to show that 
some males reach a period in middle life that may be recognized as a 
climacteric, accompanied by an abrupt reduction in the frequency of 
sexual activity; but our own data show no such phenomena for the popula- 
tion as a whole, nor for most of the individuals in the population. 

The decline in sexual activity of the older male is partly, and perhaps 
primarily, the result of a general decline in physical and physiologic 
capacity. It is undoubtedly affected also by psychologic fatigue, a loss of 
interest in repetition of the same sort of experience, an exhaustion of the 
possibilities for exploring new techniques, new types of contacts, new 
situations. Evidence of this is to be found in numerous cases of older males 
whose frequencies had dropped materially until they met new partners, 



228 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Age and Number of Sources of Outlet 


AGE 

GROUP 

c/ 

VSES 

% OF Population Utilizing Each Number of Sources 

number of sources 

mean no. 

OF sources 


0 

1 

1 

‘ 2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

Adol.-15 

3,378 

5.4 

25.3 

36.7 

22.0 

8.3 

2.0 

0.3 

2.10 =t 0.02 

16-20 

3,206 

0.8 

8.6 

27.5 

34.1 

22.3 

6.1 

0.6 

2.89 i 0.02 

21-25 

2,106 

0.5 

10.5 

28.6 

35.3 

22.8 

2.2 

0.1 

2.77 ± 0.02 

26-30 

1,062 

0.6 

15.6 

35.3 

33.8 

13.0 

1.6 

0.1 

2.48 ± 0.03 

31-35 


704 

0.4 

23.0 

37.5 

32.1 

6.1 

0.8 

0.1 

2.24 ± 0.04 

36-40 


511 

1.2 

30.4 

36.5 

27.4 

3.3 

1.2 

0.0 

2.05 0.04 

41^5 


358 

1.7 

36.3 

33.5 

25.4 

2.5 

0.6 

0.0 

1.92 ± 0.05 

46-50 


235 

3.0 

38.8 

38.7 

17.4 

1.7 

0 4 

0.0 

1.77 =t 0.06 

51-55 


151 

3.2 

43.8 

31.1 

19.9 

1.3 

0.7 

0 0 

1,74 =i= 0.07 

56-60 


98 

7.2 

50.2 

26.6 

12.3 

3.7 

0.0 

0.0 

1.56 =t O.IO 

AdoL-60 

11,809 

2.2 

18.4 

32.4 

29.5 

14.3 

2.9 

0.3 

2.45 

U.S.PopuL 

10,000 

2.0 

25.8 

33.1 

27.4 

9.7 

1.8 

0.1 

2.22 





Accumulated % with Each Number of Sources 

AGE 


CASES 


number OF sources 



GROUP 





















median no. 











OF sources 





1 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 


Adol.-15 

3,378 

94.6 

69.3 

32.6 

10.6 

2.3 

0.3 

2.53 

16-20 


3,206 

99.2 

90.6 

63.1 

29.0 

6.7 

0.6 

3.38 

21-25 


2,106 

99.5 

89.0 

60.4 

25.1 

2.3 

0.1 

3.30 

26-30 



1,062 ! 

99.4 

83.8 

48.5 

14.7 

1.7 

0.1 

2.96 

31-35 



704 

99.6 

76.6 

39.1 

7.0 

0.9 

0.1 

2.71 

36-40 



511 

98.8 

68.8 

31.9 

4.5 

1.2 

0.0 

2.51 

41-45 



358 

98.3 

62.0 ; 

28.5 

3.1 

0.6 

0.0 

2.36 

46-50 



235 

97.0 

58.2 ^ 

19.5 

2.1 

0.4 

0.0 

2.22 

51-55 



151 

96.8 

53.0 

21.9 

2.0 

0.7 

0.0 

2.11 

56-60 



98 

92.8 

42.6 

16.0 

3.7 

0.0 

0.0 

1.87 

Adol.-60 

11,809 

97.8 

79.4 

47.0 

17.5 

3.2 

0.3 

2.91 

U. S. PopuL 

10,000 

98.0 

72.2 

39.1 

11.7 

1.9 

0.1 

2.67 


Table 46. Age and number of sources of outlet 

Effect of age on the number of different kinds of sexual outlet (masturbation, dreams, 
intercourse, etc.) utilized in each age period. Based on the whole population involved 
in the study. Calculations of means for the U. S. population are based on a theoretic 
population with the age distribution found in the U. S. Census for 1940. 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


229 


adopted new sexual techniques, or embraced totally new sources of outlet. 
Under new situations, their rates materially rise, to drop again, however, 
within a few months, or in a year or two, to the old level. How much of the 
over-all decline in the rate for the older male is physiologic, how much is 
based on psychologic situations, how much is based on the reduced avail- 
ability of contacts, and how much is, among educated people, dependent 
upon preoccupation with other social or business functions in the pro- 
fessionally most active period of the male’s life, it is impossible to say at 
the present time. 



ADOL-15 I6'Z0 ZI-Z5 Z6-50 31-55 56-40 41-45 46-50 51-55 56-60 

AGE GROUPS 

Figure 35. Number of sources of outlet in relation to age 


In addition to the decrease in frequency of total outlet, there is a more 
or less corresponding decrease in frequency for each type of outlet (Tables 
51-59, Figures 38-49, 53-88). 

The number of sources contributing to the total outlet is highest in the 
16-20 year period. After that, some of the sources of outlet are abandoned 
in some of the histories. From the teens into old age there is a steady 
decline in number of sources utilized (Table 46, Figure 35). The mean 
number of sources of outlet for the older teen-age males is 2.9 (a median 
of 3.4), and there is a fair number of individuals (6.7%) of that age wh® 
have five or six kinds of outlet. By 60 years of age, the mean number of 
sources has dropped to 1.6 (the median is 1.9), and none of these 60-year 
olds has more than four sources of outlet. 

Throughout the life span, there is a steady decline in erotic responsive- 
ness (Table 47). As measured by reactions to particular stimuh, each his- 




230 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


tory in the present study has been rated on a scale which allows some 
comparison of persons of different degrees of responsiveness. Ratings for 
the entire white male population average, for instance, 16.4 at 26-30 years 
of age. The ratings then steadily drop, until they reach a median erotic 
rating of 3.6 between 66 and 70 years of age. 

Frequencies of morning erection show some decline from younger to 
older age groups (Table 47). The frequency is probably highest in pre- 
adolescent or early adolescent boys, where we do not have sufficient data. 


Age Affecting Physical and Physiologic Characters 
Medians 


AGE 

GROUP 

j 

EROTIC 

RATING 

MORNING 
ERECTIONS, 
FREQUENCIES 
PER WEEK 

DURATION 

OF 

ERECTION, 

MINUTES 

ANGLE 

OF 

ERECTION 
(HIGHER ANGLES 
INDICATED BY 
LARGER FIGURES) 

MUCOUS 

SECRETION 

(GREATER 

ABUNDANCE 

INDICATED BY 
LARGER figure) 

Adol.-15 

13.64 

0.97 

12.00 

0.74 

0.00 

16-20 

14.51 

1.40 

42.88 i 

1.02 

1.07 

21-25 

15.82 

1.41 

54.43 i 

1.06 

1.37 

26-30 

16.35 

1.77 

53.09 

1.10 

1.26 

31-35 

13,92 

2.05 

47.24 

0.94 

1.08 

36-^0 

12.60 

1.68 

40.62 

0.91 

0,97 

41-45 

10.31 

1.47 

31.07 

0.95 

0.72 

46-50 

8.73 

1.33 

29.02 

0,78 

0.57 

51-55 1 

6.44 

1.29 

21.62 

0.81 

0.80 

56-60 

8.17 


26.67 


0.00 

61-65 

4.75 

1.18 

1 19.50 

0.81 

0.00 

66-70 

3.60 

i 0.50 

7.00 

0.64 

0.00 

71+ 

0.00 

I 

0.00 


0.00 


Table 47. Age affecting physiologic capacities 

Data for angle of erection and for mucous secretion were coded, and calculations 
based on the figures so obtained. 


The highest recorded median frequency is 2.05 per week between 31 and 
35 years of age. By age 70 the median frequency is down to 0.50, and it 
drops still lower in older groups. There are a number of cases of persons 
who were able to record the amount of decrease in frequency of morning 
erections in their individual histories. There are some data that indicate 
that the frequency of morning erection is correlated with general physical 
vigor and, consequently, with frequency of sexual activity (e.g,, Hamilton 
1937), and that the steady decline in morning erections over the life span 
is therefore some measure of the decline in intensity of the sex drive in 
the male. 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


231 


There is evidence of greater speed in reaching full erection during earlier 
years, and slower erection during later years, although this has been a 
difficult matter on which to secure calculable data. We have already 
drawn attention to the high sensitivity of pre-adolescent boys (Chapter 5). 
Older adults are definitely slower than youths in their teens and twenties. 
A number of our adults were able to estimate the changes which had 
occurred in the course of their lives. This gradual loss in speed of erection 
of the male becomes evident ten or twenty years before he becomes 
totally impotent* 

The length of time over which erection can be maintained during con- 
tinuous erotic arousal and before there is an ejaculation, drops from an 
average of nearly an hour in the late teens and early twenties to 7 minutes 
in the 66-70 year old group (Table 47). Under prolonged stimulation, as 
in heterosexual petting or group activities or in protracted homosexual 
activities, many a teen-age male will maintain a continuous erection for 
several hours, even when the physical contacts are at a minimum and, in 
some cases, even after two or three ejaculations have occurred. Very few 
middle-aged males, and no older ones, are capable of such a performance. 
A considerable loss in ability to maintain an erection becomes evident some 
years before the onset of complete impotence. 

In any age group there is considerable variation in the angle at which 
the erect penis is carried on the standing male. The average position, 
calculated from all ages, is very' slightly above the horizontal, but there 
are approximately 15 to 20 per cent of the cases where the angle is about 
45° above the horizontal, and 8 to 10 per cent of the males who carry the 
erect penis nearly vertically, more or less tightly against the belly. The 
angle of erection is, in general, higher for males in the early twenties, and 
lower in more advanced ages (Table 47). Average angles become definitely 
reduced in males past fifty. It has been difficult to secure quite dependable 
estimates of angles from the subjects in this study, and it is probable that 
the changes in medians shown in the table do not express the full extent 
of the change with advancing age. There are records of 106 older males 
who recalled a change through the years of their own histories ; and these 
cases indicate a more considerable drop in angle, even from near the 
vertical to the horizontal or, at later ages, to something below a horizontal 
position. 

With advancing age there is a steady reduction in the amount of pre- 
coital mucus which is, in a portion of the population, secreted from the 
tirethra during sexual arousal and before ejaculation. In each age group 
there are about a third of the males who do not secrete such a mucus. 
Usually the secretion forms only a single clear drop; but for some males 
it amounts to several drops, or it is enough to wet the whole glans of the 
penis, or enough to drip. The greater abundance is found in the twenty- 



232 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

GROUP 

TOTAL 

POPULATION 

ACTIVE IN 

COITUS OR 

HOMOSEXUAL 

Capacities for Multiple Orgasm 

ORGASMS per CONTACT: 

NUMBER OF CASES INVOLVED 

1-2 

2 

2-3 

3 

4 

! 5 + 

Total 

Pre-adol. 

182 


17 


18 

10 

56 

101 

Adol.-15 

792 

53 

66 

14 

14 

8 

3 

158 

16-20 

2092 

91 

1 155 

32 

28 

i 10 

2 

318 

21-25 1 

2886 

65 

115 

30 

22 

6 

2 

240 

26-30 

1225 

43 

44 

15 

7 

2 

1 

112 

31-35 

866 

21 

20 

10 

2 

3 

0 

56 

36-40 

630 

13 

6 

7 

2 

1 

0 

29 

41-45 

431 

3 

3 

2 

1 

1 

0 

10 

46-50 

278 

2 

3 

2 

1 

1 

0 

9 

51-55 

172 

2 

3 

1 

1 

1 

0 

8 

56-60 

101 

1 

1 

0 

1 

0 

0 

3 

Total 

9655 

294 

433 

113 

97 

43 

64 

1044 





ORGASMS PER 

CONTACT. 





% OF ACTIVE POPULATION 

INVOLVED 

AGE 

TOTAL 









POPULATION 








tjfKUUlr 











1-2 

2 

2-3 

3 

4 

5+ 

Total 

Pre-adol. 

182 


9.3 


9 9 

5.5 

30 8 

55.5 

Adol.-15 

792 

6.7 

8.3 

1.8 

1.8 

1.0 

0 4 

20.0 

16-20 

2092 

4.3 

7.4 

1.5 

1.3 

0.5 

0.1 

15 2 

21-25 

2886 

2.3 

4.0 

1.0 

0 8 

0.2 

0.1 

8.3 

26-30 

1225 

3.5 

3.6 

1.2 

0.6 

0.2 

0.1 

9.1 

31-35 

866 

2.4 

2.3 

1 2 

0 2 

0.3 

0.0 

6.5 

36-40 

630 

2.1 

1.0 

1 1 

0.3 

0.2 

0.0 

4.6 

41-45 

431 

0.7 

0.7 

0.5 

0.2 

0.2 

0.0 

2 3 

46-50 

278 

0.7 

1.1 

0.7 

0.4 

0.4 

0.0 

3.2 

51-55 

172 

1.2 

1.7 

0.6 

0.6 

0.6 

0.0 

4.7 

56-60 

101 

1.0 

1.0 

0.0 

1.0 

0.0 

0.0 

3.0 


Table 48. Multiple orgasm and age 

Capacity to have multiple orgasm in each sexual contact rapidly decreases with age 
the capacities of the pre-adolescent males and males in their teens being far beyond those 
of older adults. Table includes 380 males who regularly have multiple climax m inter- 
course. 





AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


233 


year old males, and there is a steady decline among the older males (Table 
47). There are a few males who have been able to indicate the amount of 
reduction in their histories; but the record accumulated for the current 
ages (at time of reporting) gives a more definite picture of the decline The 
amount of mucus varies in any individual with the intensity of the erotic 
arousal, and it is probable that the lessened secretion of the older male is 
as much a measure of a reduction in the degree of arousal, as it may be of 
degenerating glands. 

The capacity to reach repeated climax in a limited period of time defi- 
nitely decreases with advancing age. Occasional multiple climax occurs in 



Figure 36 Capacity for multiple orgasm ia relation to age 


most of the histories, but regular multiple climax is characteristic of only 
a smaller number of males. The capacity is highest among those pre- 
adolescent boys (55.5%) who have sufficient sexual contact to test their 
capacities (Chapter 5), but multiple climax is still frequent among males 
(15% to 20%) in their teens and twenties (Table 48). While a few males 
(perhaps 3%) retain this capacity until they are 60 or older, most men 
lose it by 35 or 40 years of age. 

Individuals differ in the way in which they age just as they differ in their 
frequencies and in their choices of sexual outlet Generalizations which 
are based on averages of any sort must always be tempered with an under- 
standing of the range of variation in each age group Data on means and 
medians must not be confused with data on particular individuals, many of 
whom represent wide departures from any average. 


234 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


It is important to note that the range of variation in physical and 
behavioral characters is greatest in the youngest groups and is gradually 
reduced in successive periods (Table 49). This means that older populations 
are more homogeneous than younger groups. This is true in regard to the 
frequency of total sexual outlet, and in connection with most but not all 
of the individual sources of outlet. 

Masturbation, nocturnal emissions, total pre-marital intercourse, and 
animal intercourse follow the general picture in having their maximum 


Maximum Frequencifs per Week 
(Not Including the One Most Extreme Case in Each Group) 


AGE 

GROUP 

CASES 

STUDIED 

TOTAL OUTLET 

MASTURBATION 

NOCTURNAL 

EMISSIONS 

PRE-MARITAL 

INTERCOURSE 

MARITAL 

INTERCOURSE 

ANIMAL 

INTERCOURSE 

PETTING TO CLIMAX 

INTERCOURSE WITH 

PROSTITUTES 

HOMOSEXUAL 

EXTRA-MARITAL 

INTERCOURSE 

Adol. 












-15 

3012 

29.0 

23.0 

12.0 

25.0 


8.0 

3.5 

2.0 

7.0 


16-20 

2868 

28.0 

15.0 

6.5 

25.0 

25.0 

4.0 

4.5 

4.0 

10.0 

7.5 

21-25 

1535 

29.0 

12.0 

6.5 

25.0 

29.0 

1.0 

7.0 

7.0 

11. 0 

18.0 

26-30 

550 

29.0 

9.0 

4.0 

16.0 

25.0 

0.1 

4.0 

4.0 

15.0 

6.0 

31-35 

195 

29.0 

7.0 

3.0 

13.0 

20.0 


1.0 

3.0 

4.5 

4.0 

36-40 

97 

22.0 

7.0 

2.0 

8.5 

20.0 


0.5 

2.5 

4.0 

4.0 

41-45 

56 

15 0 

7.0 

1.0 

6.5 

14.0 


0.5 

1.5 

5.0 

2.0 

46-50 

39 

14.0 

6.0 

1.0 

3.5 

14.0 


0.1 

2.5 

5.0 

2.5 

51-55 

173 

7.0 

1.5 

1.0 


6.0 



1.0 

0.1 

2.0 

56-60 

106 

4.5 

0.5 

0.5 


3.0 




0.1 

2.0 

61-65 

58 

4.0 




5.0 





2.0 


Table 49. Range of variation and age 

Data based on histories of single (unmarried) males, except for marital and extra-marital 
intercourse. The lower limits of the ranges are 0 or near 0, and the maximum case is therefore 
a measure of the range of variation in each case. Differences between the least active and 
most active individuals in each age group decrease with advancing age, Le., the range of 
varaition becomes less, the homogeneity of the population increases, with advancing age. 
Only the last 4 sources have the maximum cases in anything but the youngest groups. 


range of variation in the youngest years, and narrower ranges in the older 
years. On the other hand, pre-marital petting, pre-marital intercourse with 
prostitutes, homosexual activity, and extra-marital intercourse reach their 
maximum range of variation ten or more years beyond adolescence. The 
magnitudes of the ranges in these latter cases increase through the first 
age groups (in spite of reductions in sample size). The latter cases, it is to 
be noted, include more or less taboo activities. In these cases, the restric- 
tion of these ranges in the younger groups is probably due to the impact of 
the social tradition ; and the achievement of maximum range and maximum 
mean frequency at a later period represents the gradual emancipation of 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


235 


the individual from the social tradition, and his final acceptance of a 
pattern which suits him (Chapter 21). Many of the individual histories 
support such an interpretation. After reaching the maximum range, each 
of these outlets then follows the rule in having the range of variation drop 
in successive age periods. 

OLD AGE AND IMPOTENCE 

We have the histories of 87 white males (and 39 Negro males) past 60 
years of age. The number is too small to allow statistical analyses of the 
sort employed for the other age groups. Nevertheless, there is such interest 
in the sexual fate of the older male that it seems valuable to summarize the 
data even for these few cases. 

The most important generalization to be drawn from the older groups 
is that they carry on directly the pattern of gradually diminishing activity 
which started with 16-year olds. Even in the most advanced ages, there 
is no sudden elimination of any large group of individuals from the 
picture. Each male may reach the point where he is, physically, no longer 
capable of sexual performance, and where he loses all interest in further 
activity; but the rate at which males slow up in these last decades does 
not exceed the rate at which they have been slowing up and dropping out 
in the previous age groups. This seems astounding, for it is quite contrary 
to general conceptions of aging processes in sex. The mean frequencies of 
these older white males who are still active range from 1.0 per week in the 
65-year old group to 0.3 in the 75-year olds, and less than 0.1 in the 
80-year old group (Table 44). 

At 60 years of age, 5 per cent of these males were completely inactive 
sexually. By 70, nearly 30 per cent of them were inactive. From there on, 
the incidence curve (as far as our few cases allow us to judge) continues to 
drop. There is, of course, tremendous individual variation. There is the 
history of one 70-year old white male whose ejaculations were still aver- 
aging more than 7 per week. Among the Negro males, there was one aged 
88 who was still having intercourse with his 90-year old wife, with frequen- 
cies varying from one per month to one per week. In the latter case, both 
of the spouses were still definitely responsive. 

Heterosexual intercourse continues longer than any other outlet, but 
masturbation still occurs in some of the histories of men between 71 and 
86 years of age, and nocturnal dreams with emission persist into the 76-80 
year period. Among these cases, there is no male over 75 who has more than 
a single source of outlet. Erotic response at age 75 has a rating which is 
one-quarter of the mean rating for age 65. 

Among these particular males, the mean frequency of morning erections 
had been 4.9 per week in the earlier years of their hves. In the 65-year 
period, it had dropped to 1.8, and at 75 years of age it had dropped to 0.9 



236 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

TOTAL 1 

POPULATION 

CASES 

IMPOTENT 

% 

IMPOTENT 

INCREMENT 

[ /o 

10 

4108 

0 

0 


15 

3948 

2 

0.05 

0.05 

20 

3017 

3 

0.1 

0.05 

25 

1627 

6 

0.4 

0.3 

30 

1025 

8 

0.8 

0.4 

35 

741 

10 

1.3 

0.5 

40 

513 

10 

1.9 

0.6 

45 

347 

9 

2.6 

0.7 

50 

236 

16 

6.7 

4.1 

55 

134 

9 

6.7 

0.0 

60 

87 

16 

18.4 

11.7 

65 

44 

11 

25.0 

6.6 

70 

26 

7 

27.0 

2.0 

75 

11 

5 

55.0 

28.0 

80 

4 

3 

75.0 

20.0 


Table 50. Age and erectile impotence 

An accumulative incidence curve ; based on cases which are more or less totally and, 
to all appearances, permanently impotent. 



Figure 37. Age of onset of impotence 
Percent of total population which is impotent is shown for each age. 




AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


237 


per week. Morning erections usually persist for several years, even as long 
as five or ten years, after a male has become completely impotent in other 
situations. 

The data on impotence will command especial interest. True ejaculatory 
impotence (incapacity to ejaculate even when aroused and in erection) is a 
very rare phenomenon (in 6 out of 4108 cases). Erectal impotence, on the 
other hand, is not uncommon. It appears occasionally in younger cases 
and is, of course, the ultimate outcome of the sexual picture in a portion of 
the older histories. Early erectal impotence occurs in only a few cases 
(0.4 per cent of the males under 25, and less than 1 per cent of the males 
under 35 years of age). In only a small portion of these is it a hfelong and 
complete incapacity. Sometimes the situation is complicated by a normal 
development of erotic responsiveness without an ability to perform. In 
some of these males, ejaculation may occur without erection as a result of 
the utilization of special techniques in intercourse. In many older persons, 
erectile impotence is, fortunately, accompanied by a decUne in and usually 
complete cessation of erotic response. 

Out of 4108 adult males on whom adequate data are available, there are 
66 cases which have reached more or less permanent erectile impotence. 
Ruling out instances of temporary incapacity in younger individuals, the 
ages involved in onset of permanent impotence and the incidence data 
for each of the subsequent age groups are shown in Table 50 and Figure 37. 

It will be seen that there are stray cases of impotence between adolescence 
and 35 years of age. Between 45 and 50, more males become incapacitated, 
and after 55 the number of cases increases rapidly. By 70 years of age, 
about one-quarter (27.0%) of the white males have become impotent; by 
75 more than one-half (55.0%) are so; and 3 out of the 4 white males in 
the 80-year group are impotent. Two Negro males were still potent at 80, 
We have three histories of Negroes 88 years of age, and one aged 90. One 
of these males had been impotent for fifteen years. Two had not tried to 
have intercourse for some years, but morning erections made them believe 
they would still be potent if aroused; they were, however, no longer 
responding to erotic stimulation. The oldest potent male in our histories 
was the 88-year old Negro, who was still having regular intercourse with 
his 90-year old wife. Only a portion of the population ever becomes 
impotent before death, although most males, but not all of them, would 
become so if they all lived into their eighties. 

A problem which deserves noting is that of the old men who are appre- 
hended and sentenced to penal institutions as sex offenders. These men are 
usually charged with contributing to delinquency by fondling minor girls 
or boys; often they are charged with attempted rape. Among the older sex 
offenders who have given histories for the present study, a considerable 



238 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


number insist that they are impotent, and many of them give a history of 
long-standing impotence. A few of these men may have falsified the record, 
and many courts incline to the belief that all of them perjure themselves. 
We find, however, definite evidence in the histories that many of these men 
are in actuality incapable of erection. The usual professional interpretation 
describes these offenders as sexually thwarted, incapable of winning atten- 
tion from older females, and reduced to vain attempts with children who 
are unable to defend themselves. An interpretation which would more 
nearly fit our understanding of old age would recognize the decline in 
erotic reaction, the loss of capacity to perform, and the reduction of the 
emotional life of the individual to such affectionate fondling as parents 
and especially grandparents are wont to bestow upon their own (and other) 
children. Many small girls reflect the public hysteria over the prospect of 
‘‘being touched” by a strange person ; and many a child, who has no idea 
at all of the mechanics of intercourse, interprets affection and simple 
caressing, from anyone except her own parents, as attempts at rape. In 
consequence, not a few older men serve time in penal institutions for 
attempting to engage in a sexual act which at their age would not interest 
most of them, and of which many of them are undoubtedly incapable. 

MASTURBATION AND AGE 

Masturbation is primarily a phenomenon of younger and unmarried 
groups, although it does occur in a fair number of the married histories. 
Later analyses will show that the incidence and frequency of masturbation 
are particularly affected by social backgrounds and correlated with educa- 
tional levels and occupational status (Chapter 10). 

The highest incidence for masturbation among single males (in the popu- 
lation taken as a whole) lies between 16 and 20 years of age, when 88 per 
cent is involved (Table 51, Figures 53-58). If the population is broken 
down into three groups on the basis of the amount of schooling they receive 
before they finally leave school (Table 82), it becomes apparent that the 
highest frequencies of masturbation really occur in the period between 
adolescence and age 15. The incidence steadily drops from that point. 
About half of the single population (53.8%) is still masturbating at 50 
years of age. Among married males, the highest incidence (42T%) occurs 
between 21 and 25 years of age, and the figures for these males similarly 
drop steadily into old age. In the middle fifties, hardly more than a tenth 
of the married males (11.4%) is involved. Masturbation is the first major 
source of outlet to disappear from the histories. A stray male is still in- 
volved at 75 years of age, but there is no complete masturbation to orgasm 
in any of the older histories. 

Individuals differ tremendously in the frequencies with which they 
masturbate. There are boys who never masturbate. There are boys who 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


239 


masturbate twice or thrice in a lifetime; and there are boys and older 
youths who masturbate two and three times a day, averaging 20 or more 
per week throughout periods of some years. The population is most 
variable (the range of masturbatory frequencies is greatest, the differences 
between the least active and most active males are greatest) in the 11-15- 
year old group (Table 49). From this point on, the population becomes 
more homogeneous (there is a steady decline in range of frequencies) with 
advancing age. The highest rating individual at 15 years of age has a mas- 
turbatory rate which is two and a half times that of the highest rating in- 
dividual at 30 years of age, and four times the rate of the highest individual 
at 50 years of age. 

For the single population, the maximum average frequencies of mastur- 
bation are in the very youngest group (Table 51, Figures 53-58). In this 
group the boy who is masturbating at all ejaculates more than twice a week 
from this source (a mean of 2.1 and a median of 1.8 for the active popu- 
lation). By the middle teens the frequencies have dropped to approximately 
two-thirds of the figures in the younger group, and they continue to drop 
steadily into old age. By 50 years of age, there is about half as much mas- 
turbation among single males as in the younger adolescent boys. The decline 
in frequencies is dependent upon the fact that masturbation is, to a certain 
extent, a substitute for heterosexual or homosexual intercourse which 
replaces it in older groups ; but it is to be emphasized that throughout the 
lives of many males, including married males especially of upper social 
levels, masturbation remains as an occasional source of outlet that is 
deUberately chosen for variety and for the particular sort of pleasure 
involved. Among the married males who do masturbate the frequencies 
are usually not high, averaging about once in three weeks for the active 
population as a whole. The frequencies are much higher for married males 
of upper educational levels. For the total married population, the mean 
frequencies are highest in the youngest age groups, dropping steadily into 
old age; but in the active portion of the married population, the frequen- 
cies hardly vary between 16 and 55 years of age. Here the effect of age is 
not in the direction of reducing rates among individuals who do mastur- 
bate, but by way of reducing the number of males who are involved. 

In the youngest adolescent group, considering the population as a whole, 
the average boy is drawing nearly two-thirds (60.2%) of his total sexual 
outlet from masturbation. The figures drop steadily into old age. By 50, 
the average unmarried male who has any masturbation in his history de- 
rives only about a third (37.9%) of his outlet from that source. Among 
the married males who draw at all on this outlet, about 8 per cent of 
the total number of ejaculations comes from masturbation between 16 
and 20 years of age; and, interesting to note, the figure rises in the later 
married years until it reaches 16 per cent at 50 years of age. 

9 





240 


41-45 56 0.61 ± 0.19 0.08 35.19 60.7 1.01 ± 0.29 0.37 43.66 ± 6.31 

46-50 -39 0.62 ± 0.25 0.07 33.13 53.8 1.15 ± 0.45 0.50 37.88 ± 7.05 



Married Males 


(S-ctfonT-icocsio 


oo 

<N CO 

rj- fo 

CN 



m 

•t:J- rt 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CO CO 


d 

o o 

O 

O 

d 

d o 



t-H io 

00 

CN 

o 

VO rf 


r-'* 

cN vd 

CN 

CN 

to 

CO ^ 


«n 

CO 

CO 

CN 




cn 

o r- 

C3N 

to 

CO 

CO CO 


C3^ 

Tt to 

r-- 

On 


to t-H 


d 

'4' 

CO 

CN 

CN 

CN CN 


Tgh r- VO 



to 

to CO 



,— ( 1—1 


o 

O 

O O 


d 

o o 

O 

o 

o 

O O 


m 

r- 

to 

VO 

Tl- 

CN 00 

VO 

to 

ov 0\ 


VO 

rH 

On t-^ 

00 


o o 


t-H 

CN 

CN Tt 

r- 

■H 

41 41 

41 

41 

41 

41 41 

41 

1—4 

to o 



VO 

tci- 00 

vO 

as 

l-H to 

NT 

VO 

On 

00 

os 

CN <N 

CN 

CN 

CN 

00 

d 


1— 1 l-H 

T-< 


l-H 

T-H t-H 

CN 



o 

O 

o 

o o 

On 


1— 1 i-H 


1— ( 

1— 1 

t-H t-H 

O 

d 

d d 

d 

d 

o 

o o o 

00 



to 

VO 

NO ’-t 

O 

o 

o o 

o 

O 

o 

o ^ 


d 

o o 

o 

o 

o 

d o 

d 

■H 

41 41 

41 

41 

41 

41 41 

41 

cn 

to CN 

VO 

to 

Tt 

O 00 

CN 

1 Ti- Tt- 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CO CO 

CN 

o 

o o 

O 

O 

d 

d d 

d 

o 

oo ON 

to 

r' 

r- 

On 


as 


to 

VO 

CN 

O to 

On 

CO 

rT Tt 

Tt- 

CO 

CO 

CO CN 


CO 

CN C3N 

CO 

CN 


T-H to 

VO 

so 

to T~t 

O 

CN 

NO 

HH CO 

O 

CO 

«0 VO 

VO 

to 

«o 

to VO 

tT 


Q O 

o 

Q 

o 

O O 

8 

o 

O O 

o 

o 

o 

O O 

d 

d o 

o 

o 

d 

O O 

d 

CO 

CN CN 

(N 

CN 

cN 

CN CO 

CN 

o 

O O 

o 

O 

O 

O O 

O 

O 

o o 

o 

o 

d 

o o 

d 


41 41 

41 

41 

41 

41 41 

41 


o 

VO 

CO 

rH 

ON O 


1— t 

CN CN 

T— ( 

»-H 

1— I 

O HH 

o 

d 

o o 

o 

o 

d 

o o 

d 

cs 

^ r- 

ON 

O 

CN 

«0 ON 



to CO 

VO 

ON 


l> o 

VO 

cs 


to 

CO 

<N 



o 

to o 

to 

O 

to 

O to 

o 

VO 

:^2 

CO 

ri 

22 

to to 

VO 

vi 


CN CN 

CO 

CO 

Tt- 

to 

•o 


0 O S 

bJO Cl, cr* 

*5 c'S -(-> 

1 

<2 %° 

i 

d n ^ 

d 2 d 

w 2 

*3 ^ 

'B 2 a> 

.S 

^ 2 w 

j ^ 

t 

W) p -*-* 


oo *2 

a B 

g 

“ o 

o 

ii 


_ o 

CD 

CO 

O 

T3 T3 



td i: 

& g o 


.3 d.o 

I &o 

d ^ 
d o 45 
bJ0*d3 y 
« 2 

■oil 

§ .9 p 

9 t'O 

O o S 

^ • s S 

^ 3 

g-^ I: 

w p_ 
CD c2 
d .d CD 
CLi ^ 43 
£ 

§l-l 

i^l 

P cS > 

ft E.S 


5 "i •- 

^ l> 

V 3 ^ 

s'. c) 

'C? 4> 

O d 

'|.|i 

l^i 

P 4> 3 

= §8 • 


241 



Nocturnal Emissions and Age 


t-- m VO ^ o i> 

0 > «rv CO 00 VO 



ov »o 


0-’ 

T— H 

VO 

1 

T-X T-t 


T-l 

o4 



VO CO 

''t 

04 

O' 

00 


CO CO 

CO 

CO 

04 

r-H 

• 

o o 

O 

O 

o 

o 

• 

n OV 

Os 

CO 


. 

OS O 

O 

ON 

Tl- 

VO 


CO 

r- 

VO 

VO 

wo 


«0 T-i 

o 

04 

O' 


<o 

CO 

ON 

CO 




00 


O'* 

WO 


-Tt- CO ■Ti- 

04 

0" 

O 



(N 

04 

1-1 



d o 

O 

O 

o 

O 


CO CO 

1 

00 

r-( 

CO 

ON 

00 lO 

00 

CO 

wo 

VO 


o o o 

t-H 

04 

CO 

WO 

tl -H 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

0\ 04 


00 

00 

04 

oo 


Os 

so 


'Vt 

VO T-< 

CO 

04 

T-S 

ON 

Os 

C4 04 

cs 

04 

04 



rH CO 

CO 

04 

wo 

O oo 

<N (N 

r4 

04 



O 

o o o 

O 

o o o 

04 

<N 

04 

CO 

CO 

wo 

o o 

O 

O 

o 

O 

O 

o o o 

O 

o 

O 

d 

41 41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

CO O 

o 

wo 


O l> 

Tf rt 

CO 

04 

04 


o o 

o 

O 

o 

o o 

04 CO 

oo 

O' 

CO 00 04 

C4 O 

o 

oo O 

ON 

00 

wo oo 

oo 

O' 

0" 

w^ 


O Os 

O 

O'- 


CO 

CO 

oo (N 


VO 

i-H 

OS 

O' 

l> 

04 

o 

00 

WT 








*-< 04 <N 

o 

r- 

CO 

O 

O ^ 

1—1 


o 

O 

o 

o o 

O 

o 

o 

O 

o 

I— 4 

04 

04 

04 

04 

CO 

o o 

O 

o 

o 

O O 

d d 

o 

o 

o 

O 

O 

■H 41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

CNl cs 

CO 

00 Os 

04 

00 

<N CO 

CO 

04 


.—1 

o 

o o 

o 

O 

o 

d 

d 

04 00 

wo 

o 

wo 

O' 

VO 


CO 

wo 

ON 

a\ 

uo 

O 00 

wo 

wo 




CO CN 

r-t 






VO 

V O *o O «o O »o 

CN CN CO m *«:1- ’Vt 

TO ’-t rs fS CO CO Tj- 

<c 


O' wo O VO 

Os 

oo Os VO 


I-H rt o wo 

Os 

O O' o 


WO WO VO VO 

WO 

VO OS o 






CO T-H CO T-( 

On 

00 wo 00 


04 04 04 04 


T-l f-H r-l 


o o o o 

O 

o d o 


CO 00 wo 

r-H 

r-H Os 


CO 00 O 

04 

^ ^ o 


WO WO WO WO 


CO CO 


04 CTs CO 


wo O' O 


wo o VD O' 

04 

wo <0s wo 


04 CO CO CO 

CO 

CO 04 CO 


04 04 CO r-t 

00 

O' VO VO 



o 

o o o 


o o o o 

O 

d d o 


CO -Tf T-H O 

O 

^ 

CO 

oo wo wo O' 


O 00 ON 

o 

o o o o 

o 

^ ^ 

vd 

41 41 41 41 

41 

41 41 41 

41 

O' On On QO 

o 

Os O O 

ON 

wo -Tf rt r-i 

00 

ON 

O' 

WO VO VO O' 

VO 

0^ OS 0'^ 

CO 





Os Os 00 00 

oo 

00 O' O' 

O' 

o o o o 

o 

o o o 

o 

o o o o 

o 

o o o 

d 

Tj- 04 04 04 

04 

04 04 CO 

04 

O Q O O 

O 

o o o 

O 

d d d d 

d 

d d d 

d 

41 41 41 41 

41 

41 41 41 

41 

wo 04 O 

o 

VO wo wo 

o 

04 04 0^4 04 

04 

r— 1 T— 1 


o o o o o 

d d d 

d 

ON o VO VO 

CO 

TO o o 

rt 

04 d 04 Q 
wo WO VO 'o 

CO 

TO oo Tj* 

CO 

wo 

wo Tj- '(t 

04 

w^ 00 Os 

O 

O th 04 

CO 

00 VO C4 wo 


wo ON TO 

O' 

04 CO Tj- Tj- 


Tf CO Tt* 

CM* 

>— 1 CO CO CO 


^ o o 

O 

o o o o 

o 

o o o 

o 

o o o o 

o 

d d d 

d 

04 -rH r-l I— 1 


— ^ 04 

zr 

o o o o 

o 

o o o 

o 

o o o o 

o 

d d d 

d 

41 41 41 41 

41 

41 41 41 

41 

CO Tt 04 


ON «> O' 

CO 

r-( T-< T— 1 f— 1 


o o o 

O 

o o o o 

o 

d d d 

d 

04 ^ t" ON 

o 

<S wo On 


t'' wo CO VO 

OS 

r' O' o 

VO 

04 O' t" wo 

CO 

04 ’'H -rH 


O WT o w^ 


wo O wo 

o 

04 04 CO CO 


wo wo 

IvAi 

VO 

VO -H Qj 

VO 

vi 

04 04 CO 

CO 

'«ct- Tf wo 

wo 


242 


Table 52. Nocturnal emissions and age 
For explanations, see the legend with Table 51 on Masturbation. 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


243 


Age is, obviously, a factor which affects masturbation in most of its 
aspects. Its influences are to be noted in the steady reductions in the num- 
bers of persons involved (the incidences); in similar reductions in the fre- 
quencies (rates per week) with which masturbation occurs, both in the 
single and in the married portions of the population ; in the reduction in 
the range of frequency. The percentage of the total outlet which is supplied 
by masturbation is reduced in the total population, both single and married ; 
but among those who continue to draw on this source, masturbation 
remains a fairly constant portion of the outlet during the single years, even 
if they extend into old age. For the married males who masturbate it is an 
increasingly important source of orgasm with the advancing years. 

NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS AND AGE 

Nocturnal emissions enter the picture somewhat later than other sources 
of sexual outlet (Table 52, Figures 59-64). In only a small number of cases 
do they appear at the very beginning of adolescence. Even in those cases 
where dreams provide the first source of ejaculation, pubic hair and other 
physical characteristics usually indicate that the individual became adoles- 
cent a year or more before the first emission. There are 4 cases of persons 
who were past 40 before they had their first nocturnal emission. Neverthe- 
less, dreams to climax are primarily a phenomenon of the teens and the 
twenties. 

The highest incidence of nocturnal emissions is about 71 per cent among 
single males 21 to 25 years of age. By 50, only about a third of the males 
still experience such dreams. By 60 years of age only 14 per cent still has 
them. It is interesting to note that dreams as an occasional source of 
ejaculation still appear in the histories of men as old as 86. 

In the youngest age group there are a few individuals who dream to 
climax with average frequencies which run as high as 12 per week, although 
there are many males who average only a few times per year. The maximum 
frequencies drop rapidly in successive age groups. At 30 years of age, the 
maximum is only a third as high, and by 50 years of age it is only a twelfth 
as high, as at age 15. 

Among single males of the active population, nocturnal emissions occur 
with the highest frequency between adolescence and 30 years of age. 
Among the married males the highest frequency is between 16 and 30 years 
of age. The highest average frequency, for those single males who have any 
nocturnal emissions at all, is about once in three weeks (0.3 per week); 
for the married males, it is once in four weeks (0.23 per week). In both 
groups there is a decline in frequencies after thirty. Beyond 50 years of 
age, nocturnal enoissions do not average more than four or five per year, 
for those individuals who have any at all. 



AND Age 





cS 


2 


S 

to 

S 


os o ^ 




VO T-H cs m o 

vs 



rt VO os 00 O 

c- 







fN in o m 

CM 



CM OJ fO (N 

CM 



o o o o o 

o 



rn OO >o T-i 00 

vs 



OO ^ 00 CO O 

T-I 



CO (N CM (N 

r-l 



^ VO CM CM OO 
CO O r-H io 

CO 



T-1 CM CO CM 'fH 

tH 



Tt OO OO VO CM 

O O O O O O 



o d o o d d 



CM Ov CM r- OS 

CO 

T-I 


VO Tj* 00 O 

o CO 


O O O T-I CO 

00 

vs 


■H -H -H -H -H 

-H 

41 


CM -H ^ On 

o 


o 

vs VO CM 'O 

o 


vs 

Os CO CM CO 

CO 

c- 

d 

1— H l-H T-i 




vs O ’-1 T}* Tt 

8 

00 

vs 

C- OO Os OO On 

00 

C' 

O O O O O 


o 

o 

1 oooooooo 1 

1 CM CM CO vs IT) 

c- 

vs 


o o o o o o o 
d d d d d d d 


■H -H -H 4i -H 

41 

41 


00 • OO CM vs 


CO 

vs 

CM CO CO CO CJ 

CM 

T-I 

o 

d o o o o 

o o o 

CO m C' vs VO 

CO 

C' 


C' OO CM CO CO 

T-I 

o 

vs 

T-I CO Tj- CO CM 

T-I 



OS C- VO ON VO 


CM 

VO i-H r-l t-H VS 


00 

T-I 

T^ VO CM 


o o 

O Q O O O 

o o o o o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o o 

o o o o o 

o o o 

O T-I ^ CM T-^ 


T-, 

o 

o o o o o 

o 

o 

o 

o o o o o o o 

d 

•H -H -H 

41 

41 

41 

vs CM VO T-H VO 

CM 

T— 1 


05 T-H T— 1 T— 1 

o o o 

o o o o d 

d d d 

CM OO VO O vs 

c- 

VO 

OS 

tH VO CO vs os 

O 00 VS VS TH 
CO CM T-I 

Os 

vs 

CO 

vs 

V O VO O VO 

o 

vs 

o 

1. CM CM CO CO 

32 

VO 

vA 

T3 T-1 CM CM CO 

<I 

CO 




o 

tS) 

cS 

Ti 

c 

c3 

>< 

c3 


to 

a 


ei 

H 


244 


For explanations, see legend with Table 51 on Masturbation. 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


245 


In the early twenties, the average single male (total population) derives 
about one-twelfth (8.3%) of his total outlet from nocturnal dreams. From 
there on, this outlet is of decreasing importance in the total picture. For 
those single males who have any dreams at all (active population), they 
are of greatest importance (21.5% of the total outlet) in the middle thirties. 
Among married males they are of lesser significance, accounting for about 
3 per cent of the total ejaculations of the total population throughout the 
life span. They steadily rise in importance among the males in the active 
portion of the married population, representing 5 per cent of the outlet of 
the younger married males, and 10 per cent of the outlet of the older 
married males who ever do dream to the point of cHmax. 


PETTING TO CLIMAX, AND AGE 

Pre-marital heterosexual histories often involve a considerable amount 
of physical contact without actual intercourse. About 88 per cent of the 
total male population has such petting experience prior to manage (Table 
134, Figure 117). There are some males (and a smaller number of females) 
who respond to such stimulation, whether generalized or more specifically 
genital, to the point of complete orgasm. Such petting, as it is usually 
called, is not entirely new with the younger generation; but frank and fre- 
quent participation in physical stimulation that is openly intended to 
effect orgasm is definitely more abundant now than it was among older 
generations. A great deal of the petting does not proceed to orgasm, but 
more than a quarter of all the males (28%) pet to that point prior to mar- 
riage (Table 135, Figure 118). The incidence of the phenomenon is still 
higher at upper educational levels (Chapters 10 and 16) where more than 
half of the males (58%) are ultimately involved. The highest incidence in 
any single age period is 31.8 per cent during the 21 to 25-year period, and 
the figures drop steadily from there until the time of marriage — or until 
they disappear in the old age of still unmarried males. 

The highest frequencies recorded for any individual male average 7.0 
per week, in the 21 to 25-year group, after which the maximum cases drop 
quickly to 0.5 per week after 35 years of age (Table 49). 

The frequencies with which males reach orgasm in pre-marital petting 
are relatively low, in all age groups (Table 53, Figures 38-43). This is one 
phenomenon where frequencies are not highest in the youngest group. 
Calculated in any way (as means, or medians, for the total, or for only the 
active portion of the population) the maximum performance is in the 21 to 
25-year old group, where the mean of the active population is about once 
in three weeks (0.30 per week). The averages (means for the active popu- 
lation) then drop a bit from 26 to 40 years of age, and more abruptly there- 
after. 



PERCENT OF POPULATION PERCENTS FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


PETTING TO CLIMAX 







PETTING TO CLIMAX 

M£AN FREaUENCieS 

TOTAL POPULATION 







RA 

FI6 

H DATA 
CORRECTI 

. 38 

OM 










y 

SIM< 



















I I ^ ! I 1 XZ„r--:„rT.d- ^ 1 

-J5 -20 -25 -50 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 


ACE GROUPS 


Z5 


20 


IS 


10 







PETTING TO CLIMAX 

PEIlCeNT OF TOTAL OUTtCT 

TOTAL POPULATION 







RA\ 

mmm u S 

FIG 

H DATA 
CORRECT 

39 

ON 



\ 







SINGLE 











^ J 









n 



— 

— 




-15 -20 -25 -50 -55 -40 -45 -50 -55 - 60 



ACE GROUPS 


Figures 38-40. Relation of age to frequency, incidence, and significance of 

petting to climax 


Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 


246 





FREQUENCY PER WEEK PERCENTS FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


PETTING TO CLIMAX 







SINGLE 

\ 

\ 

\ 

^ 







/ 

/ 

"'s. 












PETTING TO CLIMAX 

MEAN FREaUENCIES 

ACTIVE POPULATION 


RAW DATA 
U 5 CORRECTION 


-15 -20 -25 -50 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 

AGE GROUPS 








SINGLE 

y 

/ 





1 

! 





PETTING TO CLIMAX 

PERCENT OF TOTAL OUTLET 

ACTIVE POPULATION 



RAW data 

mmm v S CORRCCTIOM 

FIG. 42 I 

— 

/ 

y 

y 





A 

\ 

\ 

\ 





\ 

\ 

\ 

N 




-15 -20 -25 -30 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 

AGE GROUPS 


PETTING TO CUMAX 

MEDIAN FREaUENCIES 

ACTIVE population 

\A 

— — RAW DATA 

I FIG. 43 I 






















"SINGLE^ 




i 

1 


-15 -20 -25 -30 -55 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 

AGE GROUPS 

Figures 41-43. Relation of age to frequency and significance of petting to climax 
Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 

247 





Non-marital Intercourse and 


w 



00 O O ^ VO 00 • 

^ ^ Os O ON r-i . 


<N CO • 

• 


00 o m NO »o VO • ■ 

On oo oo Ti- T-i CO • • 

^ ^ ^ ^ ^ O • • 


to CO m <N * 


o o 00 rsj to • • 

^ NO VO VO to • 


VO VO ^ VO VO O 
to »-i ON r-- ON o 

to On O CO O VO 
r-l CO CO cn (N 


O <N «0 ON (N OO • • 

00 m <N ON • • 


O T-H ^ o o o • • 


H 

k 

c/3 

Si 

cs 


S 

feJD 

.s 


oo 

tn 


CO 

r4 

oo 

VO 

CO 


00 


oo 

CO 

o 

oo 

O 


O 



CO 

to 

VO* 

On 

-H 

-H 


•H 

-H 

■H 

-H 

■H 

<s 

00 

On 

VO 

o 

T— 1 

00 

00 

r-H 

CN 


(N 

o 

04 

(N 

00 

VD 

to 


o 


tn 

VO* 

d 

CO 

CO 

CO 



CO 


Tt- 


OtOfOONCOCNM-tN 

vO'^TfT}-”<;i*co't3’CN 

d d d d d d d d 


or^ONr— oovoi>-vo 

d d d d d d d d 
41-H-H-H-H-H-H-H 

r'ONcScnr^ONr^Ti' 

VOcOCDCNOr^Oto 

d d d d d d 


fnT^totr)■^vooom 

■^(NOOCNt^cO^T-i 

(Ntototoiotototo 


<N ro VO CO Th O O 

,— ( rt XT <N OO ^ 


'Ct to ON 00 VO O CO 
r-i CS CN CN <N CN CS 


oo 

Tf- 


OcNvor-vocNtNr-H 

oooooooo 
d d d d d d d d 


CO to r~- 1 

o o o o > 


I ON O ON 

. O >-H o 


oooooooo 

•H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H 

Oc0l>C0T-t(NOC0 

'^r^r^c^vo'^t'^cN 

d d d d d d d d 


oiootTiOtor^vooN 
^VOcOtOONONtriCO 
O 00 to to ^-1 

CO CS 1— t 


■^OtoOtoOtOO 

(NrScoco'sJ-'^to 

ONOriNiT-lJr-HVO 

"O^CN<Ncocort'^ 

< 


OO VO CN CO r-' ON ' 


CO 00 04 O 'Tj- t— < ro 
CN 04 r- to to Tj- Tt 


. o O O O O 


00 CO CO On CN T— I ^ 


ONONVOtotO-’^tOVO 


O O O O O O O 


^^^^cvirocotoON 


OOOOOOOOO 


OOOOOOOOO 


I o O O O O O O 


■'Ct-Tt-OTj-00tOC4cO'< 


ONr^'sttOt^tOOOONCS 


OOOOOOOOO 

OOOOOOOOO 


OOOOOOOOO 


r-vor4r4«/^<NTftoto 

OOOOOOOOO 

d d d d d d d d d 
-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H 


OOOOOOOOO 


cocnOvotCN'^OOt^ 

C^ r-j cn 


OtnOtTNOtnotoO 

tNcNcncOTf'^totnvo 

vi J. vi ^ 5 I J. Mi 

'rH(S(NcncO'^t'?l'»o«ri 


248 


Table 54. Non-marital intercourse with companions, and age 

Showing pre-marital intercourse for single males, extra-marital for married males. For the additional intercourse which those same males have with 
prostitutes, see Table 55. For further explanations, see the legend with Table 51 on Masturbation. 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


249 


Between and 25 years of age, the average male (total population) 
derives 3.0 per cent of his total sexual outlet from petting to climax. 
Leaving out the males who never do reach orgasm in petting, the statement 
can be made that about 6 per cent of the total outlet is so derived by the 
average male between 16 and 20 years of age, and this figure builds up 
to 17.5 per cent at 40 years of age. Since this source drops steadily in im- 
portance for the total single population, but rises in importance for 
individuals who are actively engaged in this activity, it is evident that 
the percentage of the population which is involved steadily decreases in 
older age groups. Petting is less important as a source of outlet than any 
other sexual behavior except intercourse with animals of other species. It 
is much more significant as a means of education toward the making of 
socio-sexual adjustments. 

The low incidence and frequency of petting to climax in the older single 
groups may be correlated with the usually low rates of all sexual activities 
at that age, and with the fact that a large number of older, single males are 
apathetic, sexually inhibited, socially timid, or heterosexually disinclined. 
The present data, however, may be merely an expression of the fact that 
petting as a source of outlet has acquired vogue only in more recent dec- 
ades. It is possible that some years hence those members of the present 
younger generation who are still unmarried may account for an increase 
in the frequency of this activity at older levels. 

PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE AND AGE 

It is probable that heterosexual intercourse would provide the major 
source of pre-marital outlet if there were no restrictions on the activity of 
the younger male. There is, however, no other sort of activity that is so 
markedly affected by the tradition of the social level in which the individual 
is raised (Chapter 10). The incidence and frequencies of pre-marital inter- 
course are very low for the more educated portion of the population; but 
for lower educational levels this remains as the chief source of outlet 
before marriage. Data on pre-marital intercourse must, therefore, be inter- 
preted in connection with the other factors which are treated in the present 
volume. 

The highest incidence of pre-marital intercourse comes in the late teens, 
where nearly three-quarters (70.5%) of the total U. S. population is in- 
volved (Table 54, Figures 71-76). From that point the incidence drops, 
but still stays high. In every age group between 16 and 50, more than half 
(from 70.5% down to 51.3%) of the single males engage in heterosexual 
intercourse. 

The variation in frequency of pre-marital intercourse in any group is at 
its maximum between adolescence and 25 years of age (Table 49). From 
that point, the range becomes increasingly restricted in each older popu- 



Intercourse with Prostitutes: 


ooo^o^'Or'J■^oom 

mr^ooc^mooiort 


41 r 

* "o e 


^ "S 

o 

\o p 3 

O 




»o 

o\ 

m 'i—i <s 




CN 

<N 

VO VO 

wo 



d 

O 

o o o 

o 




o\ 

m 00 VO 

. 



1-H 

VO ^ 

wo 




■rt 

^ ^ JO 

w> 




'sO 

Os VO r- 

CN 



'sO 


<N m Tf 



d 

tri 

VO Cs <N 

VO 





t— ( 




<N 

<N 

O 00 ^ 




O 


CN cn 

CO 

• 


d 

O 

O O O 

o 

• 

• 


O 

T— < cn VO 


CO 

VO 

m 

so 

CN (N cn 


ON 

ON 


o 

r-K CN CO 

wo 


00 

•n 

41 

41 41 41 

41 

41 

41 

cn 


ON CN CO 

ON 

CO 

-vt 

t-h 

o 

T-i 00 r- M- CN 

r-* 


00 

CN oo 

On 

00 

CN 



r-( CN CN 

CO Tl- 

wo 

00 

r-* 

On vn r-* CN 

CO 

r-- 

o 

O 

O CN CN CO 

CO 

CN 

d 

o 

o o o 

o 

o 

d 

ro 

<N 

CO VO 00 

ON 

o 

o 

O 

O 

o o o 

o 


CN 

' d 

d 

d o o 

o 

o 

d 

•H 

41 

41 41 41 

41 

41 

41 


cn 

<n CO «/N 



o 

<N 

r4 

CO VO VN 

wo 


VO 

d 

d 

O O O 

o 

o 

d 

<N 

oc 

r-( 00 wo 

wo 

CO 



d 

ON ON d 

00 

CJV 

00* 


m 

CN CN Tf Tt 

CO Nt- 

o\ 

00 

CN Tt 

CO 

wo 


cn 


ON ^ VO 

o 

VO 


d 

ci 

CO VO CJN 

CO 

o 

wo 




t— 1 

r— t 


8 

O Q O O 
O O O O 

§ 

o 

o 

8 

d 

o 

o o o 

o 

o o 

o 

T— ( 

rH CN ^ 

wo 

wo 


o 

o 

o o o 

o 

o 

r-H 

d 

o 

d d o 

o 

d 

d 

41 

41 

41 41 41 

41 

41 

41 


r-- 

O VO CN 

VO 

00 

<7s 

o 

o 

^ ^ CN 

CN 


CN 

o o 

o o o o 

o 

d 

<s 

oo 

wo O w^ 

c-* VO o\ 1 


VO 

CO wo ON 

On wo 

CO 

o 

00 

WO WO r-i 




cn 

cs 

1-H 





CO Tt' 

wo 

wo 00 

ON 

CO 

CO 








T-H 


VO 

wo 

r- 

00 CN 

ON 


wo 


CO 

CN 

CN 

^ CN 


<N 

CN 


d o o 

o o o o 

o 


ON 

wo 

CN 

On O 

N" 

ON 

,-H 


wo 

Ov 


N* CN 


t- 

ON 


r-s 


rH 







wo 

o 

CN CN 


t-h 

wo 



r-t 

CO 

T— ( 

t-t 

o 

VO 


r— < 


r-S 


Y*<p-4 


r-H 


wo 

wo 

N* 

CO CN 

CN 

CN 



o 

o 

o 

o o 

o 

o 

o 


o o o 

o o 

o 

o 

d 


wo VO 

CN 

ON CO 

wo 

N- 

CO 

wo 

■(— ( 

o^ o 

T-H CO 


ON 

r- 

CO 


o 


T-I CN 

CO 

t-* 

o 

oo 







t-h 

1— H 

41 

41 

41 

■« ■« 

41 

41 

41 

41 

r- 

00 

CO 

o 

wo 

ON 


8 

wo 

CN 

On O 
t-h wo 

CN 

Nr 

wo* 

00 

CN 

wo 

On 


r- 


oo oo 

C7^ 

CN 

r- 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o o 

o 

CN 



o 

o 

o 

d o 

o 

o 

o 

o 


wo 

s 

-Ct- rj- N- 


Os 

wo 

»— 1 

o 

o o 

o 

o 

o 

t-h 

o 

o 

o 

o o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

41 

41 

41 

41 41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

00 


ro 

o o 

oo 

wo 


wo 

CN 

cN 

CN 

CN CN 


CN 

CN 

CN 

o 

o 

o 

o o 

o 

o 

o o 

CO 

ON 

CO 

r-< wo 

wo 

l> 

CO 

wo 

Tl- CN 


T-S o 

oo 

wo* 


T-( 

t— ( 








CN CN 

N* wo 

00 

o 

CO 

wo 

00 

r- 

00 

00 00 


00 

H-H 

C" 

o o o o o o o 



o 

8 

o 

88 

o 

o 

o 


o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o o o 

o o 

o 

o 

(N 



. 

8 

o 



o 

o 

o 

o o 

o o o 

o 

o 

o 

d d 

o 

o 

o 

o 

41 

41 

41 

41 41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

CO 

CO CN CN 

cN 


CN 

CN 

o 

o o 

o o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o o 

o o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

CN 



ON O 

CN 

wo 

On 

r-- 

wo 

CO 

VO ON 


o 

o 

VO 

CN 


«> 

wo CO 

CN 


IPH 



esc4fnco'^'^«n 

OVsirivilTiijT-iVO 

^T-<r4<NfOfn'^Tr- 

< 


OioO'nO'nOw^O 

vi ^ ^ a. 3 2 .A j. vi 


250 


Table 55. Intercourse with prostitutes, and age 
For explanations, see the legend with Table 51 on Masturbation. 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


251 


lation. At 15 years of age, the most active male is having pre-marital inter- 
course with ejaculation on an average of 25 times per week. At 30 years of 
age, the extreme male still has a rate of 16 per week; but by 50 years of 
age, the maximum frequency is down to 3.5 per week. 

For the total population, the highest frequency of pre-marital inter- 
course occurs in the 16-20 year group, where the mean is about one and a 
third times (1.32) per week. If the calculations are made on the active males 
only, the highest average frequency is in the youngest group, between 
adolescence and 15 years of age, where the corrected figure is almost 
exactly 2 per week. It again becomes evident that the youngest boy has 
the greatest capacity and the highest frequency of activity if he has the 
opportunity to exercise it. From age 16 on, the frequencies drop, and those 
males who are still unmarried at 50 engage in intercourse only a quarter 
as often (0.5 per week) as the active teen-age boys. 

With advancing age, the average unmarried male draws a somewhat 
decreasing proportion of his total outlet from pre-marital intercourse. 
The average teen-age boy (of the active population) derives nearly half of 
all his outlet from intercourse. The average 50-year old, unmarried male 
derives nearer a third of his outlet from heterosexual intercourse (the data 
based on interpolations from the uncorrected figures in Table 54). This 
follows the now familiar pattern of each outlet beginning at its peak in the 
middle teens, and going down in rate with advancing age. 

Pre-marital intercourse may be had either with companions or with 
professional prostitutes. Among unmarried males, an increasing portion 
of the intercourse is derived in later years from paid contacts (Table 55, 
Figures 77-82). In the adolescent to 15-year group, less than 1 per cent of 
the boys with pre-marital intercourse depend solely upon prostitutes, and 
14.6 per cent have intercourse with both companions and prostitutes. By 
50 years of age, a seventh of the males (14.3%) who have pre-marital inter- 
course depend entirely upon prostitutes, and more than a half of them 
(62.0%) have intercourse with both companions and prostitutes. 

The individual males who have the highest frequencies of pre-marital 
intercourse with prostitutes are found in the group between 21 and 25 years 
of age (Table 49). In both younger and older age groups, the maximum 
frequencies are lower (/.e., the range of variation in those populations is 
less). 

For this active portion of the population, the frequency of intercourse 
with companions is greatest between adolescence and 15, after which the 
frequencies drop steadily into the oldest ages; but intercourse with prosti- 
tutes increases in frequency until it reaches its maximum (over 0.6 per 
week) between 26 and 35 years of age. This increase in frequency is not an 
effect of aging, but a social effect. Younger males find it easier to secure 
intercourse with girls of their own age and social level. The older male 



Marital Intercourse and Age 




For explanations, see the legend with Table 51, on Masturbation. 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


253 


finds it more convenient and less dangerous to secure intercourse from 
professional sources. This custom may not be followed by succeeding 
generations, who have been less accustomed to going to prostitutes at any 
age (Chapter 11). 

For the younger males, between 16 and 20, prostitution provides only 
4 per cent of the total outlet for the population as a whole, and about 1 1 
per cent of the outlet for those who actually frequent prostitutes. But by 
50 years of age, prostitutes provide nearer a sixth (approximately 16%) of 
the total outlet for the still single males, and more than half (about 53%) 
of the total outlet for the males who do go to prostitutes. Since payment for 
sexual contacts is much more frequent among males of particular social 
levels, the data need the breakdown which will be given them in subse- 
quent chapters on social factors affecting patterns of sexual behavior. 

MARITAL INTERCOURSE AND AGE 

Marital intercourse is the one activity which is least affected by any of 
the social factors except marital status itself. The data given here are 
based on males who are living with either legal or common-law wives. 

Between 16 and 40 years of age, practically all of the males (more than 
99%) who are married find some outlet in marital intercourse (Table 56, 
Figures 44-49). From 45 on, there are a few males who discontinue such 
intercourse even though they remain wedded and live with their wives. 
By 60 years of age about 6 per cent of the married males are no longer 
active. Our limited series of older histories shows 83 per cent of the males 
having intercourse with their wives at ages 60-65, and 70 per cent having 
it between 66 and 70. We have so few histories of still older married males 
that we cannot make a further statement. 

In all the age groups between 16 and 30, there are individuals who have 
intercourse with frequencies as high as 25 or more per week (Table 49). 
Such high frequencies are not found in older groups. There the range of 
variation becomes narrower ; and by 50 years of age the maximum average 
rate for any individual is 14 per week. By 60 the maximum has cut down 
to 3 per week. 

It is particularly instructive to compare average frequencies in marital 
intercourse for successive age groups (Table 56, Figures 44-49). Between 
16 and 20, the boy who is married has a higher rate of total sexual outlet 
(4.8 per week) than the males of any other group, and most of that outlet 
(over 85%) is derived from his marital intercourse. The frequencies of the 
intercourse in this teen-age group average near 4 (3.9) per week. From 
there on, the mean frequencies drop in each successive five-year period. 
The decline is at an astonishingly constant rate, from the youngest to the 
oldest ages. By 60 years of age, the average frequency is about once (0.9) 
per week. 



PERCENT OF POPULATION PERCENTS FREOUENCY PER WEEK 


254 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


MARITAL INTERCOURSE 



AGE GROUPS 



































FIG. 

1 

— — R^W 

■■■ U S 

45 

DATA 

CORRECTIO 







MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

PCRC£NT OF TOTAL 00TL6T 

TOTAL POPULATION 


0 I 1 1 1 ^ \ I 

“15 “20 -25 -30 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 


AGE GROUPS 



Figures 44-46. Relation of age to marital intercourse 
Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 





FREQUENCY PER WEEK PERCENTS FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


255 


MARITAL INTERCOURSE 




























MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

MEAN FREQUENCIES 

ACTIVE POPULATION 


RAW DATA 

mm V S CORRECTION 


-20 -25 -30 -35 -40 -45 

ACE GROUPS 


-55 -60 


RAW DATA 

mmm U. S. CORRECTION 

MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

PERCENT OF TOTAL OUTLET 

ACTIVE POPULATION 


-55 -40 

ACE CROUPS 


MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

MEDIAN FREQUENCIES 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

— RAW DATA 

FIC. 49 


-20 -25 -50 -55 -40 -45 -50 -5 

ACE CROUPS 

Figures 47-49. Relation of age to marital intercourse 
Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 






Total Intercourse and Age 


2 

o 

§ 

i 

H 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 


00 rj- T-t VO . . 

m 0^ VO OO VO . . 

m O >G <N On * ' 

irj VO 

o 

Ph 

c/i 

3 

D 

S 

Ph 

Mean 

Freq. 


00 Tf O'! m • * 

On CO oo »o 04 • • 

• 





ill 

Vi 

Oi 

ID 

< 

Incid. 

% 


Cv l> O rt 04 CO • • 

On ro 04 O • • 

m r- o- o- r- oo • • 

0 

u 

u 

s 

1 

O 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 


l> 04 m Tf 04 04 . . 

r- ( 00 m -VT O' 

O o4 O vn »o m * * 

04 Tj- to Tt rt- Tj- 

o 

ps 

Mean 

Freq. 


ON in 0 - "tt ’-t o • • 

O' fo m cn ^ O * • 

O ^ r'H ^ ^ ^ . . 


i 

p 

CLi 

o 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 


36.18 ± 1.61 
35.41 =fc 0.77 
38.75 ± 0.99 
49.29 =i: 1.78 
58.46 ± 2.80 
57.68 4.14 

66.56 =t 5.34 
66.33 =i= 6.77 

o 

5 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

CO 

VOT-Ifomcoo-ONO 

‘O'^'^J-O'O'tGTl*'^ 

d d d d d d d d 

D 

1 

u 

si 

CO 

Ph 

u:i 

< 

Mean 

Frequency 

s 

O 

Ta 

B 

ON‘/^0' 04 00 04>G04 
000’--i’~'0404 04 

o o o o d o o d 

v.0i0*<}'»~i004'7t''^ 

tno4O4‘/NiGfO^0N 

u 

CO 

ci 

0 

O 

U 

pi 


Incid. 

/o 


o-voo>^r^ovoo' 

^roONOto-Otnvo 

04‘0'OVO'00''VONO 

I 

l-H 

< 

h 


%0f 

Total 

Outlet 


T— (’F-^coO'^mmoo 
tnONmrn-r-tooO'-tn 

'!tO'm'<!tvof^roO 

F-<o4commcococo 

f2 

P 

Ph 

G 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


oo4r^F-toaNfONO 

OOOo4mo4o4T-F( 

d d d d d d d d 


TOTAL P 

Mean 

Frequency 


0.34 i 0.02 
0.67 ± 0.03 
0.75 ± 0.04 
1.00 ± 0.08 

1 02 =*= 0.13 
0.92 ± 0.16 
0.75 0.17 

0.61 ± 0.16 

cases 


«nONF-'<0''cnOF-4VD 

04cn<NO<NT-ivom 

ON O' F-I VO 04 
fo cn 04 

AGE 

GROUP 


tG 

’T'O‘/0O'GO‘OO 
'. 04 04mmTf''«i*to 

’oviJ.voJ.Jdvi 

-dirHoioammrtTt 

< 


^OC^O^'vOcOT^-OT^• . 


04 t-t O 


m 


m 



CfS 

On 

ON 

ON 

ON 

ON 


r-H OO 

04 

04 





lo O' < 

O' 

cn 

o 

On 



CO m 

04 

04 

04 

r-H 

t-h 


O O oo 

On 

O 

ON 

O 



O O ON 

ON 

ON 

ON 

OO 

NO 


O O ON 

ON 

ON 

On 

ON 

ON 


r-l T— ( 







04 VO vn 

NO 

On 

00 

m 


04 VO O 

O' 

04 

O' 

04 



04 


CO 

cn 



ON ON On 

On 

ON 

On 

ON 

ON 


CO O' 

04 

00 


O' 

oo 


«G O' r-^ 

O' 

04 

O 

00 


cn CO 

04 

04 

04 

F'H 

t-f 


^ On 

VO 

ON 

ON 

04 


1 

CO VO vn 

O 

sO 

O' 

04 



O O O 

o 

o 

o 


— 

04 

-H -H -H 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

-t O' 

to 

r' 

NO 

CG 

o 


O) ‘G T-H 


CO 

VT) 

m 

<G 

m 

ON On 

On 

d 


o 

o 


On OO CO 

00 

ON 

ON 

O' 

ON 

ON 

CO rf VO 

ON 

rn 

NO 

vO 

O' 

04 

O' Tt ^ 

00 

O' 



On 

CO 

04 04 04 



T- 


o 

o 

O' rl- 
04 — 

O 

::: 



O' 

cn 

o o o o o 

d 

d 

o 

d 

-H ll 41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

1". o I-I 

o 


ON 

On 

rn 

04 

04 Ov 


C4 

00 

o- 

»G 


m 04 

04 

04 





O ON 

O 


O' 

o 

VO 

cn 

d Ov On 

ON 

ON 

On 

oo 

O' 

O' 

O On ON 

On 

ON 

On 

ON 

ON 

On 








F-i VO m 

NO 

04 

O 

O' 

cn 

On 

CO ON CO 

r-- 

f> 

O 

VT) 


O 

^ ON 00 

CO 

On 

ON 

ON 

CO 

04* 

ON 00 CO 

oo 

00 

oo 

OO 

00 

ON 

00 Tj- NO 

On 

CN 

NO 


rf 

ON 

4-' Tt ^ 

oo 

t> 


1— t 

ON 

O- 

04 04 04 





d 

d 

O' ^ 

O 



Tt NO 

cn 

04 fh t-h 

r-< 

r-i 

r-H 


F-H 


d d d 

o 

o 

d 

d 

o 

d 

41 41 41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

4> NO O 

oo 

04 

ON NO On 

On 

1 04 -vT ON 

04 

00 

4> O 1 

m 04 

04 

04 





ON NO 04 

CO 

04 NO 

40 

cn 

cn 

r'' NO ON 

04 

^ O 

ON 

04 

O' 

04 O' 4> 

NO TT CO 


T— ( 



0‘o0<^0«o0‘o0 

1 1 I I f I I 1 I 

'Ot-h\O^^Oi-h'Ot-mVO 

'.-i<N<Ncofn'^'^'n»o 


256 


Table 57. Total heterosexual intercourse and age 
For explanations, see the legend with Table 51, on Masturbation. 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


257 


On the other hand, the average male draws a nearly constant proportion 
of his total outlet throughout his life from marital intercourse. He gets 
from 85 to 89 per cent of his outlet from that source between 16 and 55, 
after which there is only a slight drop in the significance of marital inter- 
course. Since his rate has gone down while he continues to draw a constant 
portion of his total outlet from the intercourse, it is obvious that the de- 
cline in frequency of this activity must occur at precisely the same rate as 
the decline in the frequency of his total outlet. 

Although biologic aging must be the main factor involved, it still is not 
clear how often the conditions of marriage itself are responsible for this 
decline in frequency of marital intercourse. Long-time marriage provides 
the maximum opportunity for repetition of a relatively uniform sort of 
experience. It is not surprising that there should be some loss of interest in 
the activity among the older males, even if there were no aging process to 
accelerate it. 

EXTRA-MARITAL INTERCOURSE AND AGE 

Extra-marital intercourse, partly with companions and partly with 
prostitutes, occurs among 23 to 37 per cent of the males in each of the 
five-year periods. It is highest among the teen-age males, where 36.8 per 
cent of the population is involved (Tables 54, 55, Figures 71-76). The ac- 
cumulated number of males who have such intercourse at any time in their 
lives is, of course, much higher (Chapter 19). The active incidence figure 
stays remarkably constant between 21 and 60 years of age, with only a slight 
trend toward a decline in the older years. The absence of an aging effect 
on the incidence of the outlet is unique among all kinds of sexual activity. 

The range of variation in any five-year population is greatest between 
21 and 25 years of age (18 per week for the most active individuals), and 
the maximum goes down rapidly after that (Table 49). By 60 years of age, 
the most active individual has extra-marital intercourse only twice per 
week. 

Mean frequencies for the males who are actively involved in extra- 
marital intercourse go down more or less steadily from about 1.3 per week 
in the late twenties to about once in four weeks for the sixty-year olds. 

The percentage of the total outlet which is derived from extra-marital 
intercourse is highest in the 16-20-year period (9.6 per cent of the outlet 
of the total population), after which there is a drop at least to age 45. Then, 
only about 5 per cent of the outlet of the total married population comes 
from this source. Considering only the males who are having some extra- 
marital intercourse, the figures first drop and then rise — 18.4 per cent of the 
outlet in the teen-age group, 12.3 per cent of the outlet in the 30-year 
group, and possibly 41 per cent of the outlet in the 60-year group comes 
from this intercourse with females not their wives. The rise in significance 
of extra-marital intercourse, both in the total population and particularly 



Homosexual Outlets and Age 



VO m O «n rvj 
r- fo <N 

t>- r' o o 

<-H T-t CO cn Tf 


r-t to O Ov • • 

oo 00 m VO VO O • * 


O O ^ r-^ • • 


fo O ‘Ai CO O ; ; 

r~. T-( r«. lo ro 00 • • 

fS m <N rn fO ro • • 


m ^ CO T-w ^ 

O OC CO VO VO O 


feO 

.s 

OQ 


O'! VO Vi CO *—1 
CN <N cn Vi Tt 


O O O O O O 


^CNViO-'-^CS'^OO 

r-coovcorOf<i<NO 


o o ^ 


I 04 Vi r- (On 


-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H 

mviviooc^r-v) 

»-HCN'^0(7i’«-(^fS 

csmvi^'VO^^<N'<^^ 

T-iT-^raco'^'^'^vi 


I O VO O Vi 
I ^ On VO Vi (N 


OOOOOOOr-t 


oooooooo 

•O (N OV oo 00 ( 

Vi VO O VO < 

d d tJ 


oovovir-icocviVicOi 

TtT-Hrj-viodr^vi 

cNCS-r-icNcnTi-r^cn 


GvO 00 ’f-(xl-'^r~<Tl" 

ooi>< 7 N(r)<Dvi^Vivo 


888888S8 
d d d d d d d d 


oooooooo 

ri'mvor^'rHT|-Tj*'PH 
T— (T-lT-(COVi’Ni--^VO 

d d d d d d d d 


cscoviovir-voov 

T-HVOcoviONOvvicn 
O 00 V) Vi N-( 

CO <N ’-H 


' O Vi O Vi o Vi o 

0323:5:333 

’O'-HCNCScOcOTtrJ- 

< 


tTi os r-< Tt Tf ^ 
04 VO (On r-< O m O 


OO Xj- VO (Ov Cv <N VO 


V r~ 00 r4 P'j 
r 4 r<i Tt '(t C 4 Vi 

r-i‘ d d d d d d 


m VO (On 00 00 t- 1 rj 
d d VO* (N CN '-H 


C4 O O O O O O 


t Tt cn ri ^ ( 

, o O o o o o 


o o o o o o o 


rl CN C^ <N O 

^ -H -H 41 ^ -fl -H 
o o oo o Vi o 

CN VO cn Vi Vj IV) t-h 


fS ov CD ctn O O 
(Ti o O O T-H 


O O O O O o o 


O O o O O O 

4] 41 4i -H 41 41 41 

O (T) 00 r- VC 
04 Vi Tf 

^* d d d d d 


Vi Vi VO OV 00 CS 
00 d CO (N <N 


^ CO o r- i> VO ^ 
cnoovovovoootj* 

d d d d d o 


8888888 

d d d d d d d 


Tt ^ ^ r-N v-( <N 

o o o o o o 

d d d d d d 

41 41 41 41 41 41 

O CO r4 CM r4 

o o o o o 

d d d d d d 


c4 ^ r-- ov o cs VI 

i> Vi CO VO ov 

CN Vi CO <N 


O Vi O Vi O Vi O 

CN (N cn CO Tt Vi 

a vi a 3 3 vA 

■'-*( CN CS CO CO 


258 


Table 58. Homosexual outlets and age 
For explanations, see the legend with Table 51 on Masturbation. 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


259 


among males who are actually having such experience, is matched only by 
the increased significance which pre-marital intercourse plays in the lives 
of the older single males. Among married males, the rise in importance of 
extra-marital intercourse is chiefly at the expense of marital intercourse 
which contributes less and less to the total picture. The other outlets, 
masturbation, nocturnal dreams, and the homosexual, are not so modified 
by the extra-marital intercourse. 

From about 7.5 per cent to 14.5 per cent of the extra-marital intercourse 
is had with prostitutes (Table 55, Figures 77-82). The figures on the avail- 
able histories fluctate from group to group, without a discernible aging 
effect, at least up to age 60. The percentage of married males involved with 
prostitutes drops steadily from 19.5 per cent among the young 20-year olds, 
to 7.9 per cent at age 50. The frequencies among the active males stay 
quite constant (between 0.18 and 0.27) at all ages, without any definite 
trend. For males who have any extra-marital intercourse with prostitutes, 
the contacts account for about 3.6 per cent of the total sexual outlet at 
earlier ages. The significance of such intercourse increases with advancing 
age among these males who are actively involved. It finally approaches a 
figure which is nearly a fifth (18.4%) of the total outlet of these males in 
their fifties. This increase in percent of total outlet derived from extra- 
marital intercourse with prostitutes is in striking contrast with the lowered 
incidences, frequencies, and significances, with advancing age, of most 
other types of sexual activity. The meaning of this will be discussed in 
Chapter 20. 

The individuals who have extra-marital intercourse with prostitutes 
most frequently are in the youngest age group, 16 to 20 years of age; but 
since the percentage of the total sexual outlet which is drawn from that 
source is lowest in the youngest group, and rises gradually to 50 years of 
age, it is apparent that prostitutes are important in replacing other sources 
of outlet among older males. 

HOMOSEXUAL ACTIVITY AND AGE 

Homosexual activity in the human male is much more frequent than is 
ordinarily realized (Chapter 21). In the youngest unmarried group, more 
than a quarter (27.3%) of the males have some homosexual activity to the 
point of orgasm (Table 58, Figures 83-88). The incidence among these 
single males rises in successive age groups until it reaches a maximum of 
38.7 per cent between 36 and 40 years of age. 

High frequencies do not occur as often in the homosexual as they do in 
some other kinds of sexual activity (Table 49). Populations are more homo- 
geneous in regard to this outlet. This may reflect the difficulties involved 
in having frequent and regular relations in a socially taboo activity. Never- 
theless, there are a few of the younger adolescent males who have homo- 



Animal Contacts and Age 



’So 


<S Vd 
Os Os 

o o 

r-. Tf 


so r-( 

-ct 


Os C> 
m VO 

oo ro 

CM r-H 


d d 

o d 


Os 

CM 


VO t;!- 

^ O 


r-- 00 

r-( Tf 

^ o 


o o 

o o 


m Os 
fS Ol 
O O 

fO r-H 
88 


d d 

O O 


o r- 

Os VO 

’-V C' 


o ^ 

CO t1- 


■H -H 

^ -H 


oo oo 
00 oo 

ro r- 
r-H so 


VO OS 

1— ( Tt 


M3 Tl- d- ^ 
r' OS Os O 

o o o ^ 


o o 

o o 


r- 

CM 

o ^ 

CM VO 
^ CM 

«o 

Cl 

d d 

d o 

d 

■H 

-H 

■H 

00 Ti- 
ro VO 

c^i O O 

CO ro CO 

o o 

o o o 

Os «o 

O VO 


VO CO 

o 

CM 

oo VO 
VO 

CO 

CO so 

^ o 


O O O O 


O O 

o o 

88 


o o 

d o 


VO «o CM <N 
8888 


d d d d 


-H -H 

1 -H 


Ol OS 
CM r-i 

O O 

CO CM 
88 


o o o o 


CM 00 
t-h so 

O oo 

CO CM 

1535 

550 

195 

Adol.-15 

16-20 

21-25 

26-30 

31-35 


i> 

OJ 

C 

a 

Vi 

o 

iS 

a 

0 

u 

’d 

1 

c 

lO 


d 

H 


d 

4D 


X) 

d 


d 

d 

d 

a> 

u 

tS 


260 



AGE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


261 


sexual frequencies of 7 or more per week, and between 26 and 30 the maxi- 
mum frequencies run to 15 per week. By 50 years of age the most active 
individual is averaging only 5.0 per week. 

For single, active populations, the mean frequencies of homosexual 
contacts (Table 58, Figures 83-88) rise more or less steadily from near once 
per week (0.8 per week) for the younger adolescent boys to nearly twice 
as often (1.7 per w^eek) for males between the ages of 3 1 and 35. They stand 
above once a week through age 50. 

In the population as a whole, among boys in their teens, about 8 per 
cent of the total sexual outlet is derived from the homosexual. Calculating 
only for the single males who are actually participating, the average active 
male in his teens gets about 18 per cent of his outlet from that source, and 
the figure is increasingly higher until, at 50 years of age, the average male 
who is still single and actively involved gets 54 per cent of his outlet from 
the homosexual This, and pre-marital intercourse with prostitutes, are 
the only sources of outlet which become an increasing part of the sexual 
activity of single males. For most other kinds of outlet, as we have shown, 
the figures drop with advancing age. Since there is a steady decline in 
frequency of total sexual outlet for the average male, and since there is an 
increase both in frequencies and in percentage of total outlet derived from 
the homosexual, it is obvious that this outlet acquires a definitely greater 
significance, and a very real significance, in the lives of most unmarried 
males who have anything at all to do with it. There is considerable con- 
flict among younger males over participation in such socially taboo activ- 
ity, and there is evidence that a much higher percentage of younger males 
is attracted and aroused than ever engages in overt homosexual activities 
to the point of orgasm. Gradually, over a period of years, many males who 
are aroused by homosexual situations become more frank in their accep- 
tance and more direct in their pursuit of complete relations (Chapter 21), 
although some of them are still much restrained by fear of blackmail. 

Homosexual contact as an extra-marital activity is recorded by about 
10 per cent of the teen-age and young 20-year old married males. By 50 
years of age, it is admitted by only 1 per cent of the still married males, 
but this latter figure is undoubtedly below the fact. Average frequencies 
fluctuate between once a week to once in two or three weeks for the married 
males who have any such contacts; and there is no distinct age trend. 
From 4 to 9 per cent of the total outlet of these married males is drawn 
from the homosexual source, but again there is no apparent age trend. 

ANIMAL CONTACTS AND AGE 

Contacts between the human and animals of other species are largely 
confined to the rural portions of the male population. Rural-urban back- 
grounds are, consequently, the most important factors in determining the 



262 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


incidence and frequency of this sort of outlet. Animal contacts include the 
usual type of heterosexual intercourse and also anal and oral techniques 
which provide orgasm for the human males. The present calculations in- 
clude all three types of relations. 

There are some data which indicate that the frequency of animal con- 
tacts varies tremendously in different parts of the United States. The fig- 
ures given here apply primarily to the northeastern quarter of the country. 

About 6 per cent of the total male population is involved in animal 
contacts during early adolescence (Table 59). This is the highest inci- 
dence figure at any age. The figure drops to about 1 per cent in the single 
population over 20 years of age. If only the unmarried rural population is 
considered, the incidence figures range from 11 per cent at 11-15 years of 
age to 4 per cent at 25 years of age. Differences in social level affect this 
activity, and the figures need further analyses on that basis (Table 124). 

The maximum frequencies in animal intercourse (for the most active 
individual) go up to 8.0 per week for the population between adolescence 
and 15 years of age (Table 49). The most active cases in the next age group 
drop to 4.0 per week, and to 0.1 per week by 30 years of age. 

Animal intercourse has the lowest frequencies of all the sources that 
contribute to sexual outlet. For the total population (including persons 
who never engage in the activity) the average never rises above once or 
twice in a year; but for those who actually utilize this source of outlet, 
the frequency is about twice in three weeks (0.69 per week) in the late 
teens. There are too few active cases to generalize for older groups. The 
average boy who is having animal intercourse draws 7 per cent of his total 
outlet from fhat source in his early teens, and nearly 15 per cent of his 
outlet from that source between ages 21 and 25. There are cases of animal 
intercourse that extend with some frequency into the fifties, and in one 
case past 80 years of age. In general, the picture is one of decreasing inci- 
dence, decreasing frequency, and decreasing significance in the later years ; 
but the cases are so few and the rural-urban factors are so significant, that 
these data are not readily interpreted without further breakdown. 

POST-MARITAL OUTLETS AND AGE 

Among the males who have been married, but whose marriages have 
been terminated by death, separation, or divorce, there is sexual activity 
which in frequency is considerably above that of the single male, and nearly 
as high as among the married males. The effects of aging are, of course, 
apparent in this group. The detailed record is given in Chapter 8 on Marital 
Status and Sexual Outlet. 



Chapter 8 

MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 

Among the social factors affecting sexual activity, marital status is the 
one that would seem most likely to influence both the frequencies and 
the sources of the individual’s outlet. The data, however, need detailed 
analyses. 

SOCIAL AND LEGAL LIMITATIONS 

In social and religious philosophies, there have been two antagonistic 
interpretations of sex. There have been cultures and religions which have 
inclined to the hedonistic doctrine that sexual activity is justifiable for its 
immediate and pleasurable return; and there have been cultures and reli- 
gions which accept sex primarily as the necessary means of procreation, 
to be enjoyed only in marriage, and then only if reproduction is the goal 
of the act. The Hebrews were among the Asiatics who held this ascetic 
approach to sex; and Christian sexual philosophy and English- American 
sex law is largely built around these Hebraic interpretations, around Greek 
ascetic philosophies, and around the asceticism of some of the Roman 
cults (Angus 1925, May 1931). 

A third possible interpretation of sex as a normal biologic function, 
acceptable in whatever form it is manifested, has hardly figured in either 
general or scientific discussions. By English and American standards, such 
an attitude is considered primitive, materialistic or animalistic, and be- 
neath the dignity of a civilized and educated people. Freud has contributed 
more than the biologists toward an adoption of this biologic viewpoint. 

Since English-American moral codes and sex laws are the direct out- 
come of the reproductive interpretation of sex, they accept no form of 
socio-sexual activity outside of the marital state; and even marital inter- 
course is more or less limited to particular times and places and to the 
techniques which are most likely to result in conception. By this system, 
no socio-sexual outlet is provided for the single male or for the widowed or 
divorced male, since they cannot legally procreate; and homosexual and 
solitary sources of outlet, since they are completely without reproductive 
possibilities, are penalized or frowned upon by public opinion and by the 
processes of the law. 

Specifically, English-American legal codes restrict the sexual activity of 
the unmarried male by characterizing all pre-marital, extra-marital, and 
post-marital intercourse as rape, statutory rape, fornication, adultery, 

263 



264 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


prostitution, association with a prostitute, incest, delinquency, a contri- 
bution to delinquency, assault and battery, or public indecency — all of 
which are offenses with penalties attached. However it is labelled, all 
intercourse outside of marriage (non-marital intercourse) is illicit and sub- 
ject to penalty by statute law in most of the states of the Union, or by the 
precedent of the common law on which most courts, in all states, chiefly 
depend when sex is involved. In addition to their restrictions on hetero- 
sexual intercourse, statute law and the common law penalize all homo- 
sexual activity, and all sexual contacts with animals; and they specifically 
limit the techniques of marital intercourse. Mouth-genital and anal con- 
tacts are punishable as crimes whether they occur in heterosexual or homo- 
sexual relations and whether in or outside of marriage. Such manual 
manipulation as occurs in the petting which is common in the younger 
generation has been interpreted in some courts as an impairment of the 
morals of a minor, or even as assault and battery. The public exhibition of 
any kind of sexual activity, including self masturbation, or the viewing of 
such activity, is punishable as a contribution to delinquency or as public 
indecency. 

There have been occasional court decisions which have attempted to 
limit the individual’s right to solitary masturbation; and the statutes of at 
least one state (Indiana Acts 1905, ch. 169, § 473, p. 584) rule that the 
encouragement of self masturbation is an offense punishable* as sodomy. 
Under a literal interpretation of this law, it is possible that a teacher, 
biologist, psychologist, physician or other person who published the 
scientifically determinable fact that masturbation does no physical harm 
might be prosecuted for encouraging some person to “commit masturba- 
tion.” There have been penal commitments of adults who have given sex 
instruction to minors, and there are evidently some courts who are inclined 
to interpret all sex instruction as a contribution to the delinquency of 
minors. In state controlled penal and mental institutions, and in homes 
for dependent children, the administrations are authorized to establish 
rules of sexual behavior which go beyond the definitions of courtroom law 
It is the usual practice in such institutions to impose penalties, including 
physical punishment, for masturbation, and we have histories from at least 
two institutions which imposed equally severe penalties for nocturnal 
emissions. The United States Naval Academy at Annapolis considers evi- 
dence of masturbation sufficient grounds for refusing admission to a can- 
didate (U. S. Naval Acad. Regul., June 1940). It is probable that the courts 
would defend the right of the administrators of institutions to impose such 
ultimate restrictions upon the sexual outlets of their charges. 

Concepts of sexual perversion depend in part on this same reproductive 
interpretation of sex. Sodomy laws are usually indefinite in their descrip- 
tions of acts that are punishable; perversions are defined as unnatural acts, 
acts contrary to nature, bestial, abominable, and detestable. Such laws are 



MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


265 


interpretable only in accordance with the ancient tradition of the English 
common law which, as has already been indicated, is committed to the 
doctrine that no sexual activity is justifiable unless its objective is procrea- 
tion. 

Official church attitudes toward contraception and abortion similarly 
stem from the demand that there be no interference with reproduction. 
They are consistent in denying the use of contraceptives in marriage and in 
intercourse which is outside of marriage, for intercourse outside of mar- 
riage is illegal and not a legitimate source of procreation. Medical and 
presumably scientific data which are adduced in support of the objections 
to contraception and abortion, are rationalizations or confusions of the 
real issue, which is the reproductive value of any kind of sexual behavior. 

In addition to establishing restrictions by way of the statutory and com- 
mon law, society at large, and each element in it, have developed mores 
that even more profoundly affect the frequency of sexual activity and the 
general pattern of behavior. Some of the community attitudes fortify cer- 
tain of the legal interpretations, even though no segment of society accepts 
the whole of the legal code, as its behavior and expressed attitudes demon- 
strate (Chapter 10). Often the social proscriptions involve more than 
is in the law, and the individual who conforms with the traditions of the 
social level to which he belongs, is restricted in such detail as the written 
codes never venture to cover. Group attitudes become his “conscience,” 
and he accepts group interpretations, thinking them the product of his 
own wisdom. Each type of sex act acquires values, becomes right or wrong, 
socially useful or undesirable. Esthetic values are attached: limitations 
are set on the times and places where sexual relations may be had; the 
social niceties (and the law) forbid the presence of witnesses to sexual acts ; 
there are standards of physical cleanhness and supposed requirements of 
hygiene and sanitation which may become more important than the grati- 
fication of sexual drives; the forms of courtesy between men and women 
may receive especial attention when sexual relations are involved; the 
effect of the relations upon the sexual partner, the effect upon the subse- 
quent sexual, marital, or business relations with the partner, the effect 
upon the subject’s own self esteem or subsequent mental or physical happi- 
ness, or conflict — may all be involved in the decision to have, or not to 
have, a socio-sexual relation. While the decision seems to rest upon per- 
sonal desires, ideals, and concepts of esthetics, the individual’s standards 
are very largely set by the mores of the social level to which he belongs. In 
the end, their effect is strongly to limit his opportunity for intercourse, or 
for most other types of sexual activity, especially if he is unmarried, wid- 
owed, separated, or divorced. 

A lower level male has fewer esthetic demands and social forms to 
satisfy. By the time he becomes an adolescent, he has learned that it is 



266 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Total Outlet, Marital Status, and Age 



Total Sample Population 

age 


CASES 



MEAN FREQUENCY 

GROUP 








Single 

Married 

Post- 

marital 

Single 

Married 

Post- 

mantal 

Adol.-15 

3012 




2.91 



16-20 

2868 

272 

46 


2.88 

4.67 

4.08 

21-25 

1535 

751 

119 


2.67 

3.90 

3.70 

26-30 

550 

737 

182 


2.63 

3.27 

2.93 

31-35 

195 

569 

158 


2.38 

2.73 

1.93 

36-40 

97 

390 

128 


2.07 

2.46 

1.69 

41-45 

56 

272 

96 


1.79 

1.95 

1.49 

46-50 

39 

175 

63 


1.88 

1.79 

1.26 

51-55 


109 

42 


1 

1.54 

1.17 

56-60 


67 




1.08 


i 

Active Cases in Sample Population 

age 


INCIDENCE % 


MEAN FREQUENCY 

GROUP 








Single 

Married 

Post- 

marital 

Single 

Married 

Post- 

marital 

Adol.-15 

95.1 




3.06 



16-20 

99.2 

100.0 

97.8 


2.90 

4.67 j 

4.17 

21-25 

99.1 

100.0 

98.3 


2.70 

3.90 

3.76 

26-30 

99,3 

100.0 

97.3 


2.65 

3.27 

3.01 

31-35 

99.0 I 

100.0 

94.9 


2.40 

2.73 

2.04 

36-40 

97.9 

99.7 

97.7 


2.12 

2.47 

1.73 

41^5 

96.4 

100.0 

93.7 


1.85 

1.95 

1.59 

46-50 

92.3 

98.9 

93.7 


2.04 

1.81 

1.35 

51-55 


98,2 

88.1 



1.57 

j 1.33 

56-60 


98.5 




1.09 



Corrected for U. S. Population 


total population I 

I 


ACTIVE POPULATION 


age 








GROUP 

MEAN FREQUENCY 

INCIDENCE % 

MEAN FREQUENCY 


Single 

Married 

Single 

Married 

Single 

Married 

Adol.-15 

3.17 


94.2 



3.36 


16-20 

3.30 

4.83 

98.8 


100.0 

3.35 

4.83 

21-25 

3.04 

4.14 

97.9 


100.0 

3.11 

4.14 

26-30 

2.94 

3.51 

98.6 


100.0 

2.98 

3.51 

31-35 

2.44 

2.90 

99.2 


100.0 

2.46 

2.90 

36-40 

2.00 

2.42 

98.5 


99.9 

2.04 

2.42 

41-45 


1.95 



100.0 


1.95 

46-50 


1.80 



98.1 


1.83 

51-55 


1.54 



97.2 


1.58 


Table 60. Total outlet in relation to marital status and age 

Data for the U. S. population are based on the sample population which is corrected 
for the distribution of educational levels which is shown in the U. S. Census for 1940. 
For sigmas of means, median frequencies, etc., see the tables in Chapter 7. 





WEEK FREQUENCY PER WEEK FREQUENCY 


MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


TOTAL OUTLET 



TOTAt OUTLET 

MCOIAN frReOUENCICS 

ACTIVE 'POPULATION 
I RAW DATA f 


P<I5T- MARITAL 


A6E GROUPS 


Figures 50-52. Relation of age and marital status to frequency of total outlet 
Solid lilies represent the U. S. Corrections. 



















268 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


possible to josh any passing girl, ask for a simple social date, and, inside 
of a few minutes, suggest intercourse. Such financial resources as will 
provide a drink, tickets for a movie, or an automobile ride, are at that level 
sufficient for making the necessary approaches. Such things are impossible 
for most better educated males. Education develops a demand for more 
elaborate recreation and more extended social contacts. The average col- 
lege male plans repeated dates, dinners, expensive entertainments, and 
long-time acquaintances before he feels warranted in asking for a complete 
sexual relation. There is, in consequence, a definitely greater limitation on 
the heterosexual activities of the educated portion of the population, and 
a higher frequency of solitary outlets in that group. Upper level males 
rationalize their lack of socio-sexual activities in terms of right and wrong, 
but it is certain that the social formalities have a great deal to do with 
their chastity. 

In any case, at any social level, the human animal is more hampered in 
its pursuit of sexual contacts than the primitive anthropoid in the wild; 
and, at any level, the restrictions would appear to be most severe for males 
who are not married. One should expect, then, that the sexual histories of 
unmarried males would contrast sharply with the histories of married 
adults; and that, at the end of two thousand years of social monitoring, 
at least some unmarried males might be found who follow the custom and 
the law and live abstinent, celibate, sublimated, and wholly chaste lives. 
Scientists will, however, want to examine the specific data showing the 
effect of marital status on the human male’s total sexual outlet, and on his 
choice of particular outlets (if he has any) in his single, married, or post- 
marital states. 

TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 

The mean frequencies of total sexual outlet for the married males are 
always, at all age levels, higher than the total outlets for single males; but, 
as already pointed out (Chapter 6), essentially all single males have regular 
and usually frequent sexual outlet, whether before marriage, or after being 
widowed, separated, or divorced. Of the more than five thousand males 
who have contributed to the present study, only 1 per cent has lived for as 
much as five years (after the onset of adolescence and outside of old age) 
without orgasm. 

As previously recorded, the mean frequency of the total outlet for the 
single males between 16 and 20 is (on the basis of the U. S. Corrections) 
about 3.3 per week (Table 60, Figures 50-52). The mean frequency of 
total outlet for the married male is about 4.8 per week, which is 47 per 
cent above the average outlet of the single male At 30, the frequencies 
for the married males are about 18 per cent above those of the single 
males, and approximately this relation holds for some period of years. 
Beyond 40 years of age, the single males may actually exceed the married 



MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


269 


males in their sexual frequencies. In adolescent years, the restrictions upon 
the sexual activity of the unmarried male are greatest. He finds it more 
difficult to locate sources of outlet and he has not learned the techniques 
for approaching and utilizing those sources when they are available. 
Nevertheless, his frequency between adolescence and 16 does average 
about 3.0 per week and between 16 and 20 it amounts to nearly 3.4 per 
week. This represents arousal that leads to orgasm on an average of about 
every other day. By the time he is 30, the single male has become much 
more efficient in his social approaches and does not lag far behind the 
married individual in his performance. Considering the physical advantage 
which the married individual has in securing intercourse without going 
outside of his own home, it is apparent that the older single male develops 
skills in making social approaches and finding places for sexual contacts 
which far exceed the skills of married persons. Beau Brummels and Casa- 
novas are not married males. A few of the married males who are involved 
in promiscuous extra-marital activity are the only ones whose facilities 
begin to compare with those found among unmarried groups. It is notable 
that in the male homosexual, where long-time unions are not often main- 
tained and new partners are being continually sought, there are many per- 
sons who preserve this same facihty for making social contacts for long 
periods of years. 

The differences that exist between the total activities of the younger 
married male and the younger unmarried male are, to some degree, a 
measure of the effectiveness of the social pressures that keep the single 
male’s performance below his native capacity; although the lower rates 
in the single males may depend, in part, upon the possibility that less 
responsive males may not marry so young, or may never marry. On the 
other hand, the fact that the single male, from adolescence to 30 years of 
age, does have a frequency of nearly 3.0 per week is evidence of the ineffec- 
tiveness of social restrictions and of the imperativeness of the biologic 
demands. For those who like the term, it is clear that there is a sexual 
drive which cannot be set aside for any large portion of the population, 
by any sort of social convention. For those who prefer to think in simpler 
terms of action and reaction, it is a picture of an animal who, however 
civilized or cultured, continues to respond to the constantly present sexual 
stimuli, albeit with some social and physical restraints. 

In addition to the differences in frequencies of total outlet between 
married and single males, there are minor differences in incidence and in 
range of variation in the groups. Between adolescence and 15 years of age, 
95 per cent of the unmarried boys have some sort of sexual outlet. From 
16 to 35 years of age, 99 per cent or more of these males are engaging in 
some form of sexual activity (Table 60). Among the married males, a full 
100 per cent is sexually active between 16 and 35 years of age. Beyond 



PERCgNT OF POPULATIOM PERCENT^ FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


270 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


MASTURBATION 



-<5 -20 -25 -30 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 



0 1 : 2 ::::,:: T.l , 1 I I I "M I \ 

-15 -20 -25 -30 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 


ACE GROUPS 



AGE GROUPS 

Figures 53-55. Relation of age and marital status to masturbation 
Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 





FReaUENCY PER WEEK PERCENTS FREaUENCY PER WEEK 


MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


271 


MASTURBATION 




A<3E GROUPS 


2.5 

2.0 

1.5 

1.0 

0.5 

0 

Figures 56-58. Relation of age and marital status to masturbation 







masturbation 

M EDI AM FAEDUEN 

ACTIVE POPULAT 

CIES 

ON 

SIN6LE 

\ 

\ 






H'Z: RA\ 

FIG, 

N PATA 

5S 


\ 

\ 

\ 










— Sc : 

•V. 


■ *s. 






POST'M/ 

MARRifi 

VRiTAL 

cr:-: 

D 

— 

— 

' 








(S -20 “25 “30 *“55 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 

AG 6 OROyPS 


Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 





Masturbation, Marital Status, and Age 


Total Sample Population 


age 


cases 


MEAN 

FREQUENCY 

% OF 

total outlet 

group 











Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar. 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar. 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar. 


Adol.-15 

3012 



2.02 




70.33 



16-20 

2868 

272 

46 

1.46 


0.17 

0.36 

51.28 

3.63 

9.01 

21-25 

1535 

751 

119 

1.10 


0.21 

0.28 

41.95 

5.52 

7.61 

26-30 

550 

737 

182 

0.94 


0.20 

0.25 

36.40 

6.19 

8.72 

31-35 

195 

569 

158 

0.73 


0.16 

0.16 

31.29 

6.03 

8.35 

36-40 

97 

390 

128 

0.76 


0.13 

0.17 

37.32 

1 5.22 

9.71 

41-45 

56 

272 

96 

0.61 


0.11 

0.20 

35.19 

5.64 

13.28 

46-50 

39 

175 

63 

0.62 


0.09 

0.23 

33.13 

5.11 

18.39 

51-55 


109 

42 



0.10 

0.14 


6.35 

11.49 

56-60 


67 




0.04 



4.06 



Active Cases in Sample Population 


AGE 

GROUP 

^ incidence % 

mean frequency I 

% OF 

TOTAL outlet 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar. 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar. 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar. 


Adol.-15 

85.4 



2.36 




77.94 



16-20 

88.4 

39.0 

56.5 

1.66 


0.43 

0.64 

60.23 

9.91 

17.13 

21-25 

80.7 

47.8 

44.5 

1.37 


0.45 

0.62 

54.40 

12.15 

26.25 

26-30 

77.1 

47.9 

44.0 

1.22 


0.42 

0.58 

47.34 

12.50 

24.00 

31-35 

71.3 

45.5 

38.6 

1.02 


0.36 

0.42 

40.43 

12.44 

28.25 

36^0 

62.9 

36.7 

42.2 

1.20 


0.35 1 

0.39 

47.06 j 

12.41 i 

30.59 

41-45 

60.7 

32.7 

3^.3 

1.01 


0.34 

0.59 

43.66 

12.66 

32.06 

46-50 

53.8 

30.9 

44.4 

1.15 


0.30 

0.52 

37.88 

14.94 

36.57 

51-55 


25.7 

33.3 



0.38 

0.41 


18.18 

29.43 

56-60 


19.4 




0.22 



20.86 



Corrected for U. S. Population 


total population 


active population 


AGE 

GROUP 

MEAN 

FREQUENCY 

% OF TOTAL 
OUTLET 

incidence % 

mean 

FREQUENCY 

% OF TOTAL 
OUTLET 


Sin- 

Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 

Sm- 

Mar- 


gle 

ried 

gle 

ried 

gle 

ried 

gle 

ried 

gle 

ried 

Adol.-15 

1.86 


60.22 


87.2 


2.14 


71.02 


16-20 

1.23 

0.14 

38.50 

2.95 

87.7 

37.1 

1.40 

0.38 

47.76 

8.29 

21-25 

0.06 

0.17 1 

29.52 

4.40 

73.6 

42.1 

1.16 

0.42 

^3.12 

9.40 

26-30 

0.75 

0.16 

26.94 

4.57 

73.9 

36.5 

1.02 

0.43 

39.12 

11.34 

31-35 

0.59 

0.11 

24.94 

3.79 

67.6 

32.8 

0.88 

0.34 

34.03 

10.25 

36-40 

0.58 

0.07 

29.09 

2.95 

57.7 

22.2 

1.01 

0.33 

41.30 

13.17 

41-45 


0.05 


2.43 


15.0 


0.32 


11.80 

46-50 


0.05 


2.53 


13.6 


0.33 


16.29 

51-55 


0.03 


2.13 


11.4 


0.34 


12.09 


Table 61. Masturbation in relation to marital status and age 


Data for the U. S. population are based on the sample population which is corrected 
for the distribution of educational levels shown in the U. S. Census for 1940. For sigmas 
of means, median frequencies, etc., see the tables in Chapter 7. 



MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 273 

35, the incidence figures drop for single males, and at a somewhat faster 
rate than for married males. The differences are not great. 

The range of variation in frequency of outlet in any particular age 
group is also nearly identical for single and married males. In both popu- 
lations (Table 49), there are individuals who engage in sexual activity only 
a few times a year, and there are some who engage in sexual activities 
regularly 3 or 4 or more times per day (29 or more per week). The lower 
average rates for single males are not dependent upon the fact that there 
are no high-rating individuals in that group, but upon the fact that there 
is a larger number of the single males who have lower rates, and a larger 
number of married males who have higher rates. At least half of the 
younger married males have outlets which average 3 or more per week, 
whereas only a third of the single males fall into that category. 

Throughout both single and married histories, there is a steady decline 
in total sexual outlet in successive age groups (Chapter 7). After 30 years 
of age this decline in any 5-year period (Figures 50-52) is very nearly as 
great as the differences between married and single males of the same age 
group. Age is eventually as important as all of the social, moral, and legal 
factors which differentiate single from married histories. 

SOURCES OF SEXUAL OUTLET 

Every one of the possible sources of sexual outlet is to be found in both 
single and married histories, except as the activities are limited by definition 
(as in the case of pre-marital intercourse and extra-marital intercourse). 
The primary forces determining which particular outlets are utilized are 
the mores of the social level in which the individual is raised (Chapter 10). 
Nevertheless, marital status is a prime factor in regard to certain of the 
outlets. 

Masturbation, although it is found in practically all male histories, is 
very much restricted by the conventions of particular social groups. In all 
groups, however, it is much more frequent in single histories. It provides 
about three-quarters (71%) of the sexual outlet for about 85 per cent of the 
single males below 15 years of age (Table 61, Figures 53-58). The figure 
is lower for lower educational levels (52.3% for boys who never go beyond 
eighth grade) and higher (79.6%) for boys who will ultimately go to college 
(Chapter 10). Between 16 and 20, the incidence begins to go down for all 
levels, although up until marriage masturbation remains the chief source 
of sexual outlet for unmarried males who belong to that portion of the 
population that goes to college. The unmarried males who are actually in 
college draw nearly two-thirds of their total sexual outlet from masturba- 
tion, while pre-marital intercourse accounts for little more than a tenth 
of the outlet of the group (Table 64). On the other hand, for boys who never 
get beyond the eighth grade in school, masturbation provides a little more 







FREQUENCY PER WEEK PERCENTS FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


275 


NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 



-15 -20 -25 - 50 -35 -40 -45 - 50 - 55 -60 

AGE GROUPS 
30 


25 

20 

15 

10 

5 

0 

0.5 







NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 

PE nee NT OF TOTAL OUTLET 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

s. 

SINGLE 


— 

> 

V'. 


F16. 

63 












POST- fjJ/i 


••••**** 






/ 

/ 

/ 

MARRIEC 

> 









' 

/ 

/ 

TA 

LRECTtON 







Tj 

RAW DA 
1 U 5 COR 


-15 -20 -25 -30 -35 -40 -45 

AGE GROUPS 


-50 -55 


-60 


0.4 


0.5 


0.2 


0.1 







NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 

MEDIAN FREAUENCfES 

ACTIVE POPULATION 







FIS 

W DATA 

64 


SINGLE^ 



s.^ 






POST'Nl/ 

VRITAL 


'•s. 






MARRIEI 

D 






- — — ■ — 

— 


-15 -20 


-25 


-30 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 

AGE GROUPS 


-60 


Figures 62-64. Relation of age and marital status to nocturnal emissions 
Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 






276 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Nocturnal Emissions, Marital Status, and Age 


Total Sample Population 


AGE 

group 

CASES 

mean frequency 

% OF TOTAL outlet 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

AdoL-15 

3012 



0.22 



7.80 



16-20 

2868 

272 

46 

0.32 

0.13 

0.17 

11.29 

2.85 

4.27 

21-25 

1535 

751 

119 

0.33 

0.14 

0.16 

12.40 

3.68 

4.44 

26-30 

550 

737 

182 

0.28 

0 14 

0.17 

10.67 

4.29 

5.79 

31-35 

195 

569 

158 

0.19 

0.12 

0.10 

8 11 

4.54 

4.89 

36-40 

97 

390 

128 

0.12 

0.11 

0.08 

5.93 

4.40 

4.90 

41-45 

56 

272 

96 

0.08 

0.09 ^ 

0.08 

4.73 

4.50 

5.53 

46-50 

39 

175 

63 

0.07 

0.07 

0.08 

3.51 

3.91 

5.96 

51-55 


109 

42 


0.07 

0.05 


4.42 

4.13 

56-60 


67 



0.03 



2.73 



Active Cases in Sample Population 


AGE 

incidence % 

MEAN FREQUENCY 

?o or 

TOTAL OUTLET 

GROUP 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Adol.-15 

16-20 

52.2 

80.3 

52.9 

69.6 

0.43 

0.40 

0.25 

0.25 

26.49 

21.72 

5.57 

11.59 

21-25 

80.8 

59.0 

62.2 

0.40 

0.24 

0.26 

23.47 

6.49 

10.33 

26-30 

78.7 

62.6 

63.7 

0.35 

0.22 

0.26 

22.98 

6.49 

12.55 

31-35 

70.8 

60.6 

58.2 

0.27 

0.20 

0.16 

21.68 

7.18 

12.35 

36-40 

59.8 

53.3 

53.9 

0.20 

0.20 

0.16 

19.12 

6.80 

17.09 

41-45 

48.2 

54,4 

38.5 

0.17 

0.16 

0.21 

19.48 

7.49 

21.58 

46-50 

28.2 

48.0 

31.7 

0.23 

0.15 

0.24 

19.59 

9.40 

21.25 

51-55 

56-60 


44.0 

28.4 

26.2 


0.15 

0.10 

0.19 


7.90 

13.79 

10.27 


Corrected for U. S. Population 



TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

AGE 

MEAN 

% OF TOTAL 



mean 

^ OF 

TOTAL 

GROUP 

FREQUENCY 

OUTTLET 

INCIDENCE 7o ] 

FREQUENCY 

OUTLET 


Sin- 

Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 


g'e 

ried 

gle 

ried 

gle 

ried 

gle 

ried 

gle 

ried 

Adol.-15 

0.14 


4.55 


39.1 


0.36 


19.97 


16-20 

0.23 

0.12 

7.41 

2.52 

70.2 

53.3 

0.33 ' 

0.23 

15.53 

5.17 

21-25 

0.24 

0.12 

8.28 

3.09 

70.9 

58.4 

0.34 

0.21 

17.36 

5.45 

26-30 

0.22 

0.13 

7.92 

3.65 

69.9 

50.8 

0.32 

0.23 

17.86 

6.00 

31-35 

0.17 

0.11 

7.34 

3.73 

64.3 

51.5 

0.27 

0.21 

21.50 

6.56 

36-40 

0.10 

0.08 

5.17 

3.24 

56.4 

42.1 

0.18 

0.19 

16.17 

5.99 

41-45 


0.07 


3.55 


41.1 


0.18 


6.08 

46-50 


0.06 


2.97 


34.9 


0.15 


9.79 

51-55 


0.06 


3.50 


30.4 


0.18 


10.06 


Table 62 . Nocturnal emissions in relation to marital status and age 


Data for the U. S. population are based on the sample population which is corrected 
for the distribution of educational levels shown in the U. S. Census for 1940. For sigmas 
of means, median frequencies, etc., see the tables in Chapter 7. 



MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


277 


than a quarter (29.2%) of the sexual outlet in their late teens. As soon as 
marriage occurs, the incidence and frequency of masturbation drop con- 
siderably in all social levels. Among college-bred males over two-thirds 
(69%) of the married men have some self masturbation in their histories. 
The frequencies in married histories are, however, definitely low, and there 
are few individuals who have rates that exceed once or twice per week. 
At lower educational levels only about a third of the married males (29% 
to 42% in various groups) ever masturbate, and the frequencies are much 
lower than in upper levels, averaging not a quarter as high as in the cor- 
responding group of single males. 

Nocturnal emissions vary in frequency in different social levels, the figures 
being higher for upper level males and lower for more poorly educated 
males. They reach their highest incidence in single males between 21 and 30 
years of age. About 85 per cent of the single males have at least some experi- 
ence with nocturnal dreams that lead to climax (Table 133, Figures 138- 
139). Hardly more than half of the married males (58.4%) have nocturnal 
emissions in any particular age group. In younger single males (of the total 
population), the emissions occur twice as often as among married males of 
the same age (Table 62, Figures 59-64). In older groups, the incidences 
and frequencies of nocturnal emissions in married males are about two- 
thirds of what they are in single males. This, in conjunction with the higher 
rates of total sexual outlet in the married groups, means that nocturnal 
dreams provide 5 to 15 per cent of the outlet of the single males, but only 
2 to 6 per cent of the outlet of the married males. The lower percentages are 
in lower educational levels, the higher percentages in upper educational 
levels (Chapter 10). 

Pre-marital petting as a source of outlet is, by definition, restricted to 
the single male. The incentives for petting to the point of climax are chiefly 
those of avoiding pre-marital intercourse (at the social levels where such 
intercourse is taboo), and initiating sexual relations by way of an activity 
which is simpler than actual coitus (Chapter 16). There are some married 
males who engage in such petting to the point of climax with their wives, 
and there are some upper level males who engage in extra-marital petting 
with women other than their wives. At lower social levels, pre-marital 
petting to the point of cHmax is relatively rare, and there are few records 
of the occurrence of such behavior in marriage among the poorly educated 
groups. 

Heterosexual intercourse is the chief pre-marital outlet of the lower 
social level, although it is a lesser source of outlet for the college-bred 
portion of the population. Between 16 and 20, it occurs in about 85 per 
cent of the pre-marital histories of the males who never go beyond eighth 
grade in school, but it occurs in only 42 per cent of the males who go to 
college (Table 85, Figure 101). Between 20 and 30, pre-marital intercourse 



PERCENT OF POPULATION PERCENTS FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


278 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


TOTAL INTERCOURSE 



ACE CROUPS 


100 


MARRIED 



-IS -20 -25 


-35 -40 

AGE GROUPS 


-60 


100 

60 

60 

40 

20 


— 

••• 



kRITAt 





MAR 












“ni 







/ 

SINGLE 


— 



A 

0 ^^ 


1 






/ 

/ 





1 


DATA 

CORRECTK 

5N 






TOTAL INTERCOURSE 

1 INCfiDENCE 

1 FIS, 67 1 


-15 -20 -25 


-50 


-55 -40 

AGE GROUPS 


-45 


-50 


-55 


-60 


Figures 65-67. Relation of age and marital status to total intercourse 
Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 





PREQUENCY PER WEEK PERCENTS FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


279 


TOTAL INTERCOURSE 


MARRIED 


TOTAL INTERCOURSE 

MEAN FflEaUENCtES 

ACTIVE POPULATION 


RAW DATA 
US CORRECTION 


5.0 \ POST-MARITAL 


-15 -ZO -25 -30 


-55 -40 -45 -SO -55 -60 

AGE GROUPS 


MARRIED 










PDST'MARITAL ’ 




SINGLE 















RAW DATA ' 

■■■■ 0 S CORRECTION 

TOTAL INTERCOURSE 

PeRCCNT OF TOTAL OUTLET 

ACTIVE POPULATION 


■15 -20 


-35 -40 *^45 -50 

AGE GROUPS 


3.0 h MARRIED - 


2.0 POST- MARITAL - 

****••••., 

1.0 





TOTAL INTERCOURSE 

MEDIAN FREQUENCIES 

ACTIVE, POPUtATION 





FIG. 70 

L 












**^ ***"" 








— 






-15 -20 -25 -30 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 -6 

AGE GROUPS 

Figures 68-70. Relation of age and marital status to total intercourse 
Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 





280 


SEXUAL BEILA.VIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Total Intercourse, Marital Status, and Age 


Total Sample Population 


AGE 


cases 


MEAT 

■vl FREQUENCY 

% OF 

total outlet 

GROUP 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Adol.-15 

3925 



0.34 



14.51 ! 



16-20 

3739 

279 

48 

0.67 

4.27 

3.06 

27.91 1 

91.31 

82.28 

21-25 

2121 

766 

150 

0.75 

3.46 

3.08 

33.38 1 

89.96 

80.12 

26-30 

607 

792 

192 

1.00 

2.90 

2.28 

34.37 1 

88.83 

80.11 

31-35 

223 

623 

171 

1.02 

2.48 

1.58 

36.11 

88.76 

81.63 

36-40 

110 

442 

146 

0.92 

2.22 

1.37 

33.83 

89.72 

82.45 

41-45 

61 

306 

107 

0.75 

1.89 

1.22 

33.75 

89.00 

76.78 

46-50 

36 

197 

72 

0.61 

1.76 

1.05 

30.58 

89.57 

75.64 

51-55 


123 

43 


1.49 

0.96 


88.43 

84.38 

56-60 


73 

33 


1.09 

0.62 


92.09 



Active Cases in Sample Population 


age 

INCIDENCE 

% 

MEAN FREQUENCY 

% OF 

TOT.AL OUTLET 

group 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

I Post- 
mar- 
1 ital 

Adol.-15 

16-20 

21.7 

53.6 

100.0 

95.8 

1.56 

1.25 

4.27 

3.19 

36.18 

35,41 

91.64 ! 

81.64 

21-25 

60.7 

99.9 

94.7 

1.24 

3.46 

3.26 

38.75 

89.54 

79.48 

26-30 

66.1 

99.7 

95.3 

1.51 

2.91 

2.39 

49.29 

89.17 

78.60 

31-35 

67.7 

99.7 

91.2 

1.50 

2.49 

1.73 

: 58.46 

89.45 

80.92 

36-40 

70.0 

99.1 

91.8 

1.32 

2.24 

1.49 

57.68 

90.87 

80.37 

41-45 

65.6 

99.7 

86.0 

1.14 

1.89 

1.42 

66.56 

91.56 

85.09 

46-50 

66.7 

98.0 

81.9 

0.91 

1.79 

1.28 

66.33 

90.33 

83.25 

51-55 

56-60 


97.6 

97.3 

88.4 

78.8 


1.53 

1.12 

1.09 

0.79 


90.50 i 
91.31 ; 

84.18 

88.00 


Corrected for U. S. Population 



TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

AGE 

MEAN 

% OF TOTAL 


MEAN 

% OF 

TOTAL 

GROUP 

FREQUENCY 

OUTLET 


FREQUENCY 

OUTLET 


Sin- Mar- 

Sin- Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 

Sin- Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 


gle ried 

gle ried 

gle 

ried 

gle ned 

gle 

ried 

Adol.-15 

0.79 1 

26.17 

39.9 


1.98 

43.38 


16-20 

1.35 4.51 

42.82 92.22 

73.7 

100.0 

1.84 4.51 

50.97 

92.53 

21-25 

1.37 3.73 

50.33 91.66 

75.0 

100.0 

1.82 3.73 

55.64 

91.97 

26-30 

1.34 3.17 

45.44 91.05 

75.4 

99.8 

1.77 3.18 

57.11 

90.99 

31-35 

1.11 2.72 

45.12 91.76 

72.2 

99.9 

1.53 2.72 

62.84 

91.66 

36-40 

1.00 2.28 

43.72 93.29 

80.8 

99.0 

1.23 2.32 

59.66 

93.31 

41-45 

2.04 

93.78 


99.9 

2.04 


94.43 

46-50 

1.87 

94.25 


98.0 

1.91 


93.09 

51-55 

1.48 

94.14 


96.4 

1.54 


94.45 


Table 63. Total intercourse in relation to marital status and age 


Data for the U. S. population are based on the sample population which is corrected 
for the distribution of educational levels shown in the U. S. Census for 1940. For sigmas 
of means, median frequencies, etc., see the tables in Chapter 7. 



MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


281 


accounts for about two-thirds (57.6% to 68.2%) of the outlet of the males 
who have not gone beyond eighth grade, although in that same period it 
accounts for only a fifth (19.4% to 21.4%) of the outlet of the college males. 

In marriage, practically 100 per cent of the males of all social levels 
engage in heterosexual intercourse, except as advancing age limits the 
activity of some individuals after 45 (Table 56). Among married males of 
lower educational levels, more than 95 per cent of the total outlet (at 31 
to 55 years of age) is derived from intercourse with the spouse or with 
other females (Table 97, Figures 131-133). Among college-bred males of 
the same ages, little more than 80 per cent of the total outlet comes from 
that source. The average for the total married population is about 87 per 
cent between 31 and 55 years of age. The frequencies of marital coitus 
are two or three times as high as in the intercourse of the single male 
(Table 56, Figures 65-70). 

For the total married population, of all social levels, about 85 per cent 
of the sexual outlet comes from the intercourse with the wife (Table 56, 
Figures 44-49). It will surprise most people that this figure is not higher. 
Masturbation, nocturnal emissions, heterosexual intercourse with per- 
sons other than the wife, homosexual contacts, and animal intercourse 
provide the remaining 15 per cent of the activity. The proportion derived 
from marital intercourse varies from 81.4 per cent between 16 and 20 
years, to 88.3 per cent at 45 years of age, after which it again drops. In 
view of the rather high frequency of extra-marital intercourse among 
lower social levels, it is interesting to find that the highest percentage of 
outlet derived from the intercourse with the wife (95.5%) is found among 
those persons who have not gone beyond the twelfth grade in school, and 
the lowest percentage (61.9%) among older males who have gone to college 
(Table 97). Whether or not it is true, as some students contend, that mar- 
riage as an institution has resulted from the demand for regular inter- 
course, it is apparent that marriage does provide a more convenient source 
of sexual activity than is to be had in any other state. 

A part of the heterosexual intercourse of the married males is with 
females other than their wives. This provides 5 to 10 per cent of the total 
sexual outlet of the married population taken as a whole (Table 64, Figures 
71-76). In the youngest age groups, 37 per cent of the males are having 
extra-marital intercourse, but this figure cuts down to 30 per cent by 50 
years of age. 

Among single males, in the population as a whole, prostitutes provide 
8.6 per cent of the heterosexual intercourse between 16 and 20 years of 
age. In successive five-year periods, the percentage rises to 13.3, to 22.0, to 
28.9 per cent, and (by age 40) to 38.7 per cent. There is some rise in signifi- 
cance of prostitutes among older males of all social levels, although the 
group that goes to college never derives as much as 1 per cent of its total 



FREQUENCY PER WEEK PERCENTS FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


283 


NON-MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS 


POST' MARITAL 


3*5 

5.0 

2.5 

2.0 

1.5 

|,o|-MARRI?D 


HON-NARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS 

I MEAN FREaUENCIES 

1 r ACTIVE POPULATION T 


RAW DATA 

mam U S CORRECTION 




“15 “20 “25 “50 -35 -40 “45 -50 * 55 -60 

ACE CROUPS 

50 I 

NON'MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS 

PERCENT OF TOTAL OUTLET 

POST- MARITAL I ACTIVE POPULATION , 


POST- MARITAL 


*****..^ 



^ 


'SINGLE ■ 

.MARRI6I 

> 




tl’i: RAW DATA 
MM U 5 CORRECTION 


-15 -20 -25 -50 -35 -40 -45 - 50 - 55 - 60 

AGE GROUPS 


NON* MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS 

MEDIAN FDEQUENCtES 

1 ACTIVE POPULATION i 










POST- MARITAL 




\ 




\ 



SINGLE 




MARRIED ' — — — 

^ 

— 


: RAW DATA 

[-FIG. 7Gi — 


-15 -20 -25 -30 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 

AGE GROUPS 

Figures 74-76. Relation of age and marital status to non-marital intercourse with 

companions 

Solid lines renresent the TJ. vS. Corrections. 





284 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Kon-Marital Intercourse, Marital Status, and Age 
Total Sample Population 


AGE 

cases 

mean frequency 

% OF 

total outlet 

GROUP 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Post- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Post- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Post- 


mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 


ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

Adol.-15 

3012 j 



0.40 



14.12 



16-20 

2868 1 

288 

46 

0.73 

0.41 

3.19 

25.43 

9.08 

79.91 

21-25 

1535 

813 

119 

0.77 

0.30 

2.79 

29.46 

7.44 

76.87 

26-30 

550 1 

850 

182 

0.73 

0.16 

2.13 

28.23 

4.71 ' 

73.65 

31-35 

195 

666 

158 

0.61 

0.15 

1.37 

26.44 

5.35 : 

70 29 

36-40 

97 

475 

128 

0.42 

0.20 

1.17 

20.80 

7.69 i 

68.64 

41-45 

56 

324 

96 

0.40 

0.13 

0.85 

23.10 

5.24 ! 

57.82 

46-50 

39 

210 

63 

0.28 

0.19 

0.76 

14.81 

8.61 f 

60.39 

51-55 


130 

42 


0.17 

0.88 


9.88 1 

74.60 

56-60 


77 



0.14 



12.05 1 



Active Cases in Sample Population 


incidence % 


group 

Pre- 

mar- 

ital 

Extra- 

mar- 

ital 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Adol.-15 

16-20 

24.3 

52.4 

35.4 

93.5 

21-25 

58.5 

24.4 

95.8 

26-30 

59.5 

26.0 

89.6 

31-35 

57.4 

28.4 

81.6 

36-40 

53.6 

27.8 

80.5 

41-45 

51.8 

23,5 

68.8 

46-50 

51.3 

25.2 

66,7 

51-55 


22.3 

71.4 

56-60 


22.1 1 




% OF total outlet 




ra- 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

n 

ir- 

il 

mar- 

ital 

36.12 

35.98 

■ 

66 

79.34 

37.19 

Bg 

68 

68.15 

40.26 


19 

68.52 

44.00 


30 ! 

68.27 

35.21 ! 

l"ral 

44 

67.25 

46.28 

! 21. 

88 

62.43 

40.88 

26.40 

63.71 


30.84 

69.50 


40.94 



Corrected for U. S. Population 


total population 


active population 


age 

group 

mean 

frequency 

% OF TOTAL 
OUTLET 

incidence % 

MEAN 

frequency 

1 % OF TOTAL 
OUTLET 


Pre- 

Extra- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Pre- 

Extra- 


mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 


ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

Adol.-15 

0.80 


25.56 


40.4 


1.98 


42.18 


16-20 

1.32 

0.45 

39.16 

9.64 

70.5 

36.8 

1.87 

1.23 

47.10 

18.36 

21-25 

1.25 

0.40 

40.94 

9.22 

68.3 

31.3 

1.83 

1.28 

47.90 

16.36 

26-30 

0.99 

0.24 

33.76 

6.57 

67.3 

32.3 

1.46 

0.72 

‘ 44.06 

12.32 

31-35 

0.72 

0.15 

30.96 

5.16 

62.2 

30.9 

1.15 

0.50 

44.96 

13.04 

36-40 

0.48 

0.15 

26.00 

5.82 

55.2 

27.2 

0.86 

0.54 

38.18 

17.42 

41-45 


0.10 i 


4.50 


23.1 


0.41 


19.96 

46-50 


0.11 


5.88 


30.1 


0.43 


21.33 

51-55 


1 

1 


6.84 








Table 64. Intercourse with companions in relation to marital status and age 


Data for the U. S. population are based on the sample population which is corrected 
for the distribution of educational levels shown in the U. S. Census for 1940. For sigmas 
of means, median frequencies, etc., see the tables in Chapter 7. 


MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


285 


outlet from this source, while single males who never go beyond high 
school ultimately get more than 10 per cent of their outlet from prosti- 
tutes. The older single males who have never gone beyond grade school 
may draw nearly a quarter (23.4%) of their outlet from females whom they 
pay for intercourse (Table 96, Figures 77-82). 

Among married males (as a whole group), prostitutes never provide 
more than one or two per cent of the total sexual outlet. This is true of all 
social levels and all age groups. Married males are much like single males 
in finding, as they advance in age, an increasing portion of their hetero- 
sexual, non-marital outlet with prostitutes. Between 16 and 20, prostitutes 
provide 10.8 per cent of the extra-marital intercourse and in successive 
five-year periods the percentages rise to 11.1, to 16.5, to 17.6 per cent (at 
35 years of age) and to 22.3 per cent by 55 years of age. Among those 
married males who go to prostitutes at all (the ‘‘active population”), an 
increasing percentage of the total sexual outlet comes from this source; 
the figure is 3.6 per cent of the total sexual outlet from prostitutes at 20 
years of age, with a steady rise in the figures in older groups, until 18.4 per 
cent of the total outlet of the 55-year old, prostitute-frequenting, married 
male is derived from the commercial sources. 

The single males at all ages have more frequent paid contacts than the 
males who have wives. The reported frequencies of intercourse with prosti- 
tutes for the single males between 16 and 25 (total population) are three or 
four times as high as among married males of the same age. Between 46 
and 50, they are 15 times as high as the frequencies reported by the married 
males. Marriage is clearly a factor of considerable importance in reducing 
the frequency of intercourse with prostitutes. 

It should, however, be said that we are not entirely confident of the 
accuracy of these data on extra-marital intercourse, especially among older 
males from upper social levels. Where social position is dependent upon 
the maintenance of an appearance of conformity with the sexual conven- 
tions, males who have had extra-marital intercourse are less inclined to 
contribute to the present study. Consequently, it is not unlikely that the 
actual incidence and frequency figures exceed those given here. 

Homosexual contacts, as might be expected, are most frequent among 
unmarried males. The lowest incidence, about 27 per cent, occurs between 
adolescence and 15 years of age (Table 66, Figures 83-88). The figures 
steadily rise in older groups of single males. Between 36 and 40 years of 
age, over a third (38.7% is the corrected figure for the total U. S. popula- 
tion) of the unmarried males are having some homosexual experience, 
and uncorrected figures indicate that about half of the unmarried 50- 
year olds are so involved. 

In marriage, the liighest recorded incidence is 10.6 per cent, between 21 
and 25 years of age. After that, the incidence in the married groups drops 



PERCENT OF POPULATION PERCENTS FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


286 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


INTERCOXmSE WITH PROSTITUTES 



AOE GROUPS 



ACE CROUPS 





O' ^ ^ 1 f 1 ^ I -- I - 1 

-IS -20 -25 -50 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 

ACE CROUPS 


Figures 77-79. Relation of age and marital status to intercourse with prostitutes 
Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections, 





FReQUENCY PER WEEK PERCENTS FREaUENCY PER WEEK 


MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


287 


INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 








INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 

MEDIAN FREQUENCIES 






rirtr: raw d/> 

TA 








rivi. oc 








^*1 

•^****. 





SiHCi 

/ 

/ 

Le 

.. 

^ 

‘5T-MAR 

TAL 







/ 




MAR 


rs 

\ 


'-v.. 

— — --SJ 



— 







Q i ^ ^ ^ ^ 1 i ^ » 

-15 -20 -25 -30 -35 -40 -46 -50 -55 -60 


AGE GROUPS 

Figures 80-82. Relation of age and marital status to intercourse with prostitutes 


Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 





288 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Intercourse with Prostitutes, Marital Status, and Age 


Total Sample Population 


AGE 


CASES 


MEA^ 

j frequency 

OF 

total outlet 

GROUP 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Post- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Post- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Post- 


mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 


ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

Adol.-15 

16-20 

3012 

2868 

272 

46 

0.01 

0.07 

0.04 

0 10 

0.39 

2.48 

0.87 

2.37 

21-25 

1535 

751 

119 

0.10 

0.03 

0.12 

3.92 

0.72 

3.25 

26-30 

550 

737 

182 

0.16 

0.03 

0.19 

6.14 

0.82 

6.46 

31-35 

195 

569 

158 

0.22 

0.02 

0.22 

9.67 

0.84 

11.34 

36-40 

97 

390 

128 

0.26 

0.02 

0.24 

13.03 

0.85 

13.81 

41-45 

56 

272 

96 

0.18 

0.02 

0.28 

10.65 

0.78 

18.96 

46-50 

39 

175 

63 

0.29 

0.01 

0.19 

15.77 

0.80 

15.25 

51-55 

56-60 


109 

67 

42 


0.02 

0.02 

0.12 


1.13 

1.75 

9.78 

I 


Active Cases in Sample Population 


AGE 

incidence 

% 

mean frequency 

% OF 

total outlet 

GROUP 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Post- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Post- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Post- 


mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

, mar- 

mar- 

1 mar- 


ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

Ital 

ital 

ital 

Adol.-15 

5.2 



0.21 



7.13 



16-20 

30.8 

14.3 

37.0 

0.23 

0.28 

0.26 

8.05 

2.87 

5.50 

21-25 

29.1 

12.9 

47.1 

0.35 

0.21 

0.25 

15.19 

4.03 

18.36 

26-30 

29.8 

11.8 

55.5 

0.53 

0.23 

0.34 

22.82 

4.95 

22.28 

31-35 

40.5 

11.1 I 

60.8 

0.55 

0.20 

0.37 

28.73 

5.42 

28.63 

36-40 

48.5 

10.5 

57.0 * 

0.54 

0.20 

0.41 

39.49 

7.33 ! 

34.23 

41-45 

39.3 : 

8,5 

60.4 

0.47 

0.18 

0.46 

48.23 

8.00 

46.92 

46-50 

48.7 

5.7 

52.4 

0.60 

0.25 

0.37 

52.74 : 

12.25 

48.83 

51-55 


7.3 

40.5 


0.24 

0.29 


15.19 

54.91 

56-60 


7,5 



0.25 



19.50 



Corrected for U. S. Population 



TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

AGE 

GROUP 

MEAN 

FREQUENCY 

% OF TOTAL 
OUTLET 

INCIDENCE % 

MEAN 

FREQUENCY 

% OF TOTAL 
OUTLET 


Pre- 

Extra- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Pre- 

Extra- 

Pre- 

Extra- 


mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- 

mar- , 

mar- 


ital 

Ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital 

ital i 

1 

ital 

ital ' 

ital 

Adol.-15 

0.02 


0.61 


7.7 


0.25 


8.79 : 


16-20 

0.12 

0.05 

3.66 

1.17 

41.9 

15.9 

0.29 

0.36 

11.24 1 

3.58 

21-25 

0.20 

0.05 

6.29 

1.15 

45.5 

19.5 

0.43 

0.25 

20.27 

4.29 

26-30 

0.28 

0.04 

9.56 

1.30 

46.8 

17.2 

0.61 

0.27 

26.88 

5.89 

31-35 

0.31 

0.03 

12.57 

1.12 

51.6 

14.9 

0.62 

0.18 

32.38 

5.76 

36-40 

0.31 

0.02 

16.42 

1.12 

1 55.5 

12.0 

0.55 

0.22 

41.64 

8.52 

41-^5 


0.02 


1.11 


11.4 


0.19 


9.84 

46-50 


0.02 


1.01 


7.9 


0.24 


13.58 

51-55 


0.02 


1.65 


9.1 


1 0.25 ! 


18.43 


Table 65. Intercourse with prostitutes in relation to marital status and age 


Data for the U. S. population are based on the sample population which is corrected 
for the distribution of educational levels shown in the U. S. Census for 1940. For sigmas 
of means, median frequencies, etc., see the tables in Chapter 7. 



MARITAL STATUS ANt) SEXUAL OUTLET 


289 


to about 2 per cent at 45 years of age, and still lower in still older married 
groups. Social factors, particularly the physical and social organization of 
the family, make it difficult for the married individual to have any sort of 
sexual relation with anyone except his wife. However, the incidence of 
the homosexual is probably higher than the available record on the married 
group shows. Married males who have social position to maintain and who 
fear that their wives may discover their extra-marital activities, are not 
readily persuaded into contributing histories to a research study. While it 
is possible to secure hundred percent samples from younger males, which 
make the incidence figures for homosexual contacts fairly reliable there, 
it is rarely possible to get as good a representation of older married males. 
There are hundreds of younger individuals in the histories who report 
homosexual contacts with these older, socially established, married males, 
and the post-marital histories of males who are widowed or divorced 
include the homosexual in 28.3 per cent of the teen-age group, and still in 
10.8 per cent of the 31~35-year old histories. These data make it appear 
probable that the true incidence of the homosexual in married groups is 
much higher than we are able to record. 

Homosexual relations, both among single and married males, are some- 
times a substitute for less readily available heterosexual contacts. This is 
true at all social levels and at all age groups, especially among isolated, 
morally restrained, or timid males who are afraid to approach females 
for sexual relations. On the other hand, it must be recognized that the 
homosexual is in many instances, among both single and married males, 
deliberately chosen as the preferred source of outlet; and it is simply 
accepted as a different kind of sexual outlet by a fair number of persons, 
whatever their marital status, who embrace both heterosexual and homo- 
sexual experiences in the same age period. Consequently, the high inci- 
dence of the homosexual among single males is not wholly chargeable to 
the unavailability of heterosexual contacts. The increased incidence and 
frequency among older single males are, as previously noted, partly de- 
pendent upon the freer acceptance of a socially taboo activity as the indi- 
vidual becomes more experienced and more certain of himself. The very 
high incidence among the still older males may depend upon the fact that 
those persons who are not exclusively or primarily homosexual are ordi- 
narily married when younger, and those who have no interest in hetero- 
sexual contacts are left in higher proportion in the older, unmarried 
populations. 

Animal contacts in the northeastern quarter of the United States are 
largely confined to rural populations and are primarily activities of pre- 
adolescent and younger adolescent boys. Social taboos quickly lead the 
older individual to cover up his activity, to deny it in giving us a history 
and, in actuality, to stop such contacts at a rather early age. There are 



PERCENT Of POPULATION PERCENTS FREaUENCY PER WEEK 


290 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 

MEAN rREOUENCIES 

TOTAL POPULAT/ON 




-15 -20 


-25 -50 -55 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 

ACE GROUPS 


HOMOSeXUAL OUTLET 

PERCENT OF TOTAL OUTLET 

rOV\L POPULATION 


RAW DATA 
U 5 CORRCCTfON 




^ L..P057'MARITAL1 1 1 

A — i I I \ 

-is -20 -25 -50 -55 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 

AGE GROUPS 

)0 

HOM0S€%UMr OUTLET 

INCIDENCE I 

FiO. 85 


IL*” RAW DATA 
mmm u S CORRECTION 


\ POST- MARITAL 




-15 -20 


Figures 83-85. 


-25 “30 -55 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 

AGE GROUPS 

Relation of age and marital status to homosexual outlet 
Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 





FREQUENCY PER WEEK PERCENTS FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


291 


HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 




AGE GROUPS 







HOMOSEXUAL OUTL 

MEDIAN FREQUENC 

ACTIVE POPULAT 

*—11 RA.W DATA 

ET 

.lES 

ON 







FIG 

88 




5(NCL£^ 

y 

y 

y 

y 

y 

/ 

/ 

/ 

/ 

/ 







y 

y 

,•••••’**** 

POST 

- MARITA 

L 





MARRi 

(EO 



Q I ■ - — ■ ' I I I ^ I I 1 I 

-15 -ZO -25 -30 -35 -40 -45 -50 -55 -60 


AGE GROUPS 

Figures 86-88. Relation of age and marital status to homosexual outlet 


Solid lines represent the U. S. Corrections. 





292 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Homosexual Outlets, Marital Status, and Age 


Total Sample Population 


AGE 

group 

CASES 

mean frequency 

% OF 

total outlet 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

rtal 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Single 

! 

Mar- 1 
ricd 1 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Adol.-lS 

3012 



0.14 



4 89 

j 


16-20 

2868 

272 

46 

0.13 

0.10 ! 

0.18 

4.70 

2,21 

4 44 

21-25 

1535 

751 

119 

0.16 

0.03 

0.28 

5.98 

0.83 

7.82 

26-30 

550 

737 

182 

0.37 

0.02 

0.16 1 

14.31 

0.69 

5.38 

31-35 

195 

569 

158 

0.51 

0.02 

0.10 

21.94 

0.67 

5.13 

36-40 

97 

390 

128 

0.44 

0.02 

0.05 

21.74 

0.67 

2.93 

41-45 ! 

56 

272 

96 i 

0.44 

0.02 

0.07 

25.51 

0.86 

4.40 

46-50 

39 

175 

63 

0.61 



32.64 

1.41 


51-55 


109 

42 





0.81 


56-60 


67 









Active Cases in Sample Population 


AGE 

incidence % 

1 MEAN frequency 

i 

°/o OF 

TOTAL OUTLET 

GROUP 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

Post- 

mar- 

ital 

1 

Single 

Mar- 

ried 

1 Post- 
mar- 
ital 

Adol.-15 

16-20 

24.8 

21.6 

8,5 

28.3 

0.56 

0.62 

1.20 

0.63 

12.13 : 
13.25 : 

9.20 

9.54 

21-25 

14.5 

7.5 

26.9 

1.09 

0.43 

1.06 

25.45 ^ 

7.60 

18.94 

26-30 

25.1 

4.6 1 

17.6 

1.48 

0.48 

0.89 

i 37.00 

11.38 

18.70 

31-35 

30.3 

3.9 

10.8 

1.68 

0.47 

0.93 

46.90 

10.50 

25.79 

36-1-0 

40.2 

2.8 1 

6.2 

1.09 

0.57 

0.80 

42.17 

7.55 

14.56 

41-45 

37.5 

2.2 

5.2 

1.18 

0.76 

1.25 

42.17 

5.50 

22.50 

46-50 

51-55 

35.9 

1.7 

1.8 


1.69 

1.47 

0.68 


54.25 

12.17 

15.50 



Corrected for U. S. Population 



TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

AGE 

GROUP 

MEAN 

FREQUENCY 

% of TOTAL 
OUTLET 

INCIDENCE % 

MEAN 

FREQUENCY 

% OF TOTAL 
OUTLET 


Sin- ' 

Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 

Sin- ! 

Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 

Sin- 

Mar- 


gle 

ried 

gle 

ried 

gle 

ried 

gle 

ried 

gle 

ried 

Adol.-15 

0.22 


7.03 


27.3 1 


0.81 


17.46 


16-20 

0.26 

0.11 

7.84 

2.31 

31.0 

9.3 

0.85 

1.25 

17.73 

8.25 

21-25 

0.35 

0.04 

11.81 

0.86 j 

27.5 I 

10.6 

1.30 

0.37 

30.31 

4.69 

26-30 

0.58 

0.03 

19.68 

0.75 

35.8 

6.9 

1.61 

0.38 

35.20 

6.91 

31-35 

0.45 

0.02 

22.61 

0.72 

33.0 

4.8 

1.69 

0.47 

45.75 

9.11 

36-40 

0.41 

0.01 

22.01 

0.53 

38.7 

2.8 

1.06 

0.42 

40.42 

9.04 

41-45 




0.33 


2.1 


0.22 


2.34 

46-50 




0.40 


1.2 


0.52 


6.07 

51-55 




0.29 


1.6 


0.20 


5.82 


Table 66. Homosexual outlets in relation to marital status and age 


Data for the U. S. population are based on the sample population which is corrected 
for the distribution of educational levels shown in the U. S. Census for 1940. For sigmas 
of means, median frequencies, etc., see the tables in Chapter 7. 



MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


293 


differences in incidences and frequencies in different social levels, the ac- 
tivity being highest among those rural boys who ultimately go to college. 
In this latter group, about 28 per cent has intercourse with animals between 
adolescence and 15 years of age and 17 per cent engages in the activity 
sometime between 16 and 20 years of age (Table 59). In this farm popula- 
tion, the incidence drops so rapidly for older single males that we are not 
warranted in calculating averages on the basis of our present data. Simi- 
larly, the active cases are too few to warrant calculations for married males 
in the northeastern quarter of the United States. Limited data which we 
have from more western and southwestern portions of the country indicate 
that such animal intercourse continues as a part of the sexual activity of 
not a few married adults. 

Many of the pre-marital sources of sexual outlet are, to a degree, sub- 
stitutes for less readily available heterosexual coitus. This is particularly 
true of masturbation, the homosexual, and intercourse with animals of 
other species. Sexual activities after marriage are concentrated on hetero- 
sexual intercourse and chiefly, but by no means wholly, on marital inter- 
course. Nevertheless, it would be a mistake to consider that differences in 
the availability of sources are the sole factors which distinguish the pre- 
marital from the marital record. It is to be recalled that the younger male, 
who is in most cases the unmarried male, is the sexually most active indi- 
vidual; and the intensity and frequency of his response to any situation 
which comes along, particularly if that situation is novel and unexpected, 
lead to his involvement in more kinds of sexual activity, whether he be 
single or married. Similarly, among married males it is the younger ones 
who have the highest frequencies of masturbation, extra-marital inter- 
course, homosexual relations, and animal contacts. Marriage is undoubt- 
edly an overwhelming force in focusing sexual activity; but the similar 
concentration of the activities of the older single male upon his particular 
forms of outlet, particularly intercourse with prostitutes and the homo- 
sexual, indicates that marriage is not the only factor involved. It is also a 
matter of conditioning which leads to a centering of attention on those 
activities which have proved most satisfactory. It is a matter of a loss in 
interest in variety, after an initial experimentation with its charms, of 
concentrating upon the outlets that are most available in our society 
as it is set up, and of avoiding those things which are most likely to cause 
social disturbance. 

Throughout the record, the effects of marital status are modified and 
mitigated by the age of the individual (Chapter 7). In spite of all the social 
and legal distinctions between the rights and privileges of married as 
opposed to unmarried individuals, age is, at many points, a more signifi- 
cant factor than marital status in determining the frequencies and, indi- 
rectly, the sources of sexual outlet. 



294 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


POST-MARITAL OUTLETS 

The sexual lives of previously married males who are no longer living 
with wives, have been a closer secret than the lives of single or married 
adults We now have the post-marital histories of 433 white males. The 
sample is too small to warrant analyses even by age; and it is to be noted 
that there are not enough cases to allow corrections in accordance w'ith 
the age, marital status, and education distribution shown in the U. S. 
Census. Uncorrected figures based on our particular sample are, there- 
fore, the only ones available on these post-marital histories. Nevertheless, 
the data do seem to indicate general trends in regard to each of the sources 
of outlet. 

The post-marital histories are, in general, an interesting combination 
of items which are characteristic of both single and married groups. The 
total sexual outlet of the previously married males between the ages of 16 
and 30 is about 85 to 95 per cent as high as among married males, which 
means that it is between 40 and 50 per cent higher than among single 
males (Table 60, Figures 50-52). At these ages, there is little efiect on the 
frequency of the previously married individual’s sexual activity when he is 
deprived of a legalized source of outlet. With advancing age, after 30, 
however, the post-marital frequencies drop more rapidly than those in 
marriage, to about three-quarters (69% to 76%) of the marital rates; and 
this actually places them below the rates of even the single groups after 
age 30. It is not immediately apparent what is responsible for these differ- 
ences, but the data should be kept in mind. 

Many males who dropped masturbation in marriage, return to it after 
they have become widowed, separated, or divorced. Masturbation is found 
in a smaller percentage (56% at 16-20 years, 33% at age 45) of the post- 
marital cases than was true of the single histories; but the post-marital 
incidence is somewhat higher than it was in marriage (Table 61, Figures 
53-58). The frequency of the post-marital masturbation (total population) 
is about a quarter to one-half of what it was in the single histories, but 
a little higher than it was in the married histories. For those persons who 
engage at all in masturbation, the percentage of the total outlet which 
comes from this source (17% to 36%) in the post-marital period is about 
twice as high as it was in marriage; but this is only a half to two-thirds 
as much as it was in the pre-marital period. As in all other populations, 
advancing age brings some decline in the incidences and frequencies; 
but the portion of the outlet which is derived from masturbation increases 
with the years, among these men who live without wives. 

Nocturnal emissions (Table 62, Figures 59-64) occur in approximately 
the same number of persons in pre-marital, marital, and post-marital 
histories. The incidence is only slightly higher in single histories. The fre- 
quencies are highest among single males, two-thirds as high among married 



MARITAL STATUS AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


295 


males, and somewhere between the two among previously married persons 
(where the frequencies range from about 0.26 per week at 16-30, to 0.19 
at age 55). Nevertheless, while the incidences and actual frequencies go 
down, the percentage of the total outlet which is derived from nocturnal 
dreams (among previously married males who have any at all) rises more 
or less steadily from 10 per cent or 11 per cent between 16 and 25, to 21 
per cent at age 50. Only after that does the significance of the dreams in 
the post-marital picture show any considerable drop. 

Heterosexual intercourse is most important in the marital histories, 
least important in the histories of single males, and midway or higher in 
importance in the post-marital group (Table 63, Figures 65-70). The num- 
ber of previously married males involved in intercourse ranges from about 
96 per cent in the younger ages to 82 per cent by age 50. The drop in fre- 
quency is a bit faster than among married males. With advancing age, 
prostitutes provide an increasing part of the intercourse, companions a 
decreasing part of the post-marital intercourse (Tables 64-65, Figures 
71-82). 

The actual frequencies and the proportion of the total outlet which is 
derived from intercourse similarly lie between those of the married and 
single groups. They lie closer to those of the married males when the post- 
marital group is younger, and are nearly identical with those of married 
males when the group is older. Males who have ever become accustomed 
to the coital activities of marriage, keep coitus as their chief source of out- 
let (80% to 85% of their outlet) even after their marriages are terminated 
by the spouse’s death, or by separation or divorce. Nearly all males (about 
95%), after they have once been initiated into regular coital experience, 
whether as older single males or as previously married persons, repudiate 
the doctrine that intercourse should be restricted to marital relations. 
Nearly all ignore the legal limitation on intercourse outside of marriage. 
Only age finally reduces the coital activities of those individuals, and thus 
demonstrates that biological factors are, in the long run, more effective 
than man-made regulations in determining the patterns of human behavior. 
The picture probably differs for different social levels, but this breakdown 
cannot be made with the present-sized sample. 

Homosexual activity occurs among many of the males who have been 
previously married (Table 66, Figures 83-88). It is in 28 per cent of the 
younger tiistories, and in a smaller number of the older histories (5.2% at 
45 years of age). This group is larger than the group that had homosexual 
relations during marriage. While the incidence among the younger single 
and post-marital histories is about the same, it is eight times higher among 
the older males who have never been married. It is evident in a few of the 
individual histories that marriages sometimes break up because of the 
male partner’s developing preference for relations with other males; in 



296 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


a few cases the male is induced by the breakdown of the heterosexual mar- 
riage to accept his first homosexual experience; but in a larger number of 
cases it is a matter of the individual returning to the sort of outlet that 
he had before he was ever married. The mean frequencies of the homo- 
sexual contacts (in the active population) among these previously married 
males are a bit lower than, or of the same order as among single males; 
and they are twice as high as among those married males who are having 
homosexual relations. The percentage of the previously married male's 
total outlet which is derived from the homosexual (19 per cent, rising to 22 
per cent at older ages) is similarly double that found among married males, 
but not nearly so high as is found among older single males (where the 
figure rises to nearly half of the total sexual outlet). With advancing age, 
the homosexual in the post-marital group definitely drops in incidence, 
stays more or less constant in frequency, and slightly increases in its signifi- 
cance as a part of the total sexual outlet. 

In summary, it is to be noted that the average male w'ho is widowed or 
divorced is not left without sexual outlet, as the mores of our society and 
legal codes would have him. On the contrary, and in spite of customs and 
laws, he continues to have almost as active a sexual life as when he was 
married. He depends to a somewhat greater degree upon masturbation 
and nocturnal dreams, and at younger ages he turns to homosexual 
activity about as often as the single, previously unmarried male, but in 
most cases this widowed or divorced individual depends upon hetero- 
sexual intercourse for most (80%) of his not inconsiderable outlet. His is 
not the picture of the single male, unless it be the oldest group of the single 
males with which the comparison is made. Once married, a male largely 
retains the pattern of the married male, even after marriage ceases to fur- 
nish the physically convenient and legally recognized means for a frequent 
and regular sexual outlet. These data are in striking contrast to those 
available for the widowed or divorced female who, in a great many cases, 
ceases to have any socio-sexual contacts and who, very often, may go for 
long periods of years without sexual arousal or further sexual experience 
of any sort. 



Chapter 9 

AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 

For many centuries, men have wanted to know whether early involve- 
ment in sexual activity, or high frequencies of early activity, would reduce 
one’s capacities in later life. It has been suggested that the duration of one’s 
sexual life is definitely limited, and that ultimate high capacity and long- 
lived performance depend upon the conservation of one’s sexual powers 
in earlier years. The individual’s ability to function sexually has been con- 
ceived as a finite quantity which is fairly limited and ultimately exhaustible. 
One can use up those capacities by frequent activity in his youth, or pre- 
serve his wealth for the fulfillment of the later obligations and privileges 
of marriage. 

Medical practitioners have sometimes ascribed infertility to wastage of 
sperm. Erectal impotence is supposed to be the penalty for excessive sexual 
exercise in youth {e.g., as in Vecki 1901, 1920; Liederman 1926, Efferz in 
Bilderlexikon 1930 (3):118, Robinson 1933, pp. 61, 135, 142, et al.. Rice 
1946). The discovery of the hormones has provided ammunition for these 
ideas, and millions of youths have been told that in order “to be prepared” 
one must conserve one’s virility by avoiding any wastage of vital fluids in 
boyhood (Boy Scout Manual, all editions, 1911-1945; W. S. Hall 1909; 
Dickerson 1930: 109ff; 1933: 15ff; U. S. Publ. Health Serv. 1937). Through 
all of this literature, an amazing assemblage of errors of anatomy, physi- 
ology, and endocrinology has been worked together for the good of the 
conservationist’s theories. Why the ejaculation of prostatic and vesicular 
secretions should involve a greater wastage of gonadal hormones than the 
outpouring of secretions from any of the other glands — ^than the spitting 
out, for instance, of salivary secretions — is something that biologists 
would need to have explained. The authors of various popular manuals, 
however, seem able to explain it “so youth may know,” and conserve their 
glandular secretions. 

The Greek writers Empedocles and Diodes, and others including Plato 
after them, are said (Allbutt 1921, May 1931) to have believed that semen 
came from the brain and spinal marrow and that excessive copulation 
would, in consequence, injure the senses and the spine. Today, it is not 
unusual to find exactly the same superstitions about the origin of semen, 
and the consequently debilitating effects of ejaculation, among adolescent 
boys and among certain of their elders who want to believe such things. 
Many teen-age boys, on the contrary, have held to the equally unproved 

297 



298 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


opinions that the exercise of one's sexual functions, either in masturbation 
or in intercourse, may develop genital size and increase one’s erectal 
capacity, and that abstinence for any long period of time may impair one’s 
capacities for subsequent performance. 

There have been few scientific data available to answer these questions, 
but that has not interfered with their being answered. It not infrequently 
happens that the volume of discussion on a subject bears an inverse relation 
to the amount of exact information which is available. The assurance with 
which generalizations and conclusions are drawn, may reach its maximum 
when the least effort has been made to investigate the data which are 
basic to an understanding of the situation. If, as in the present instance, a 
whole system of moral philosophy is involved, the conclusions become 
foregone and by dint of much repetition assume the status of axioms which 
are accepted by laymen and scientists alike. But this is a question of the 
physical and physiologic outcome of physical and physiologic activities, 
and as such it is a question which can be investigated only by scientific 
procedures. 


DATE OF FIRST 

EJACULATION 

AGE OF ONSET OF ADOLESCENCE 

Before 

10 

10 

11 

1 

12 

; i 

13 j 14 i 15 

i 1 

16 

Same year 

Second year 
Third year 
Fourth year j 

Still later 

% 

100 

0 

0 

0 

0 

% 

96 

2 

5 

0 

% 

92 

5 

2 

1 

0 

% 

87 

8 

3 

1 

1 

O/ 

/o 

83 

11 

3 

1 

2 

0 ' 

/() 1 
78 ! 
14 ! 
5 ' 
2 

1 

i 0/ i 

i /o 

78 

16 

4 

2 

1 

/<) 

81 

11 

5 

3 

0 

Cases 

14 

94 

309 

1059 

1510 

1233 

307 

so 


Table 67. Lapse between onset of adolescence and first ejaculation 


Early in the course of the present study. Dr. Glenn V. Ramsey, while 
securing the histories of younger boys, noted differences in their then cur- 
rent sexual frequencies which seemed to be correlated with the degree of 
maturity of each boy. Following that lead, we have subsequently examined 
the histories of the whole population involved in the present study, and 
find that there is, in actuality, a relationship between the age at onset of 
adolescence, the age of first sexual performance, the frequencies of early 
sexual activity, the frequencies of sexual activity throughout most of the 
life span of the individual, and the sources on which he depends for his 
sexual outlet. While chronologic age is of prime importance in determining 
the mean frequencies of sexual activity for populations in different age 
groups (Chapter 7), the biologic factors which account for variation in 
the age of onset of adolescence seem to be of definite importance in effect- 




AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


299 


ing variation within any single group. The data which substantiate these 
generaUzations should provide one more instance of the difference between 
a priori reasoning and conclusions based on statistically accumulated fact. 

ONSET OF SEXUAL ACTIVITY 

Any consideration of the age of onset of adolescence is made difficult 
by the fact that there is no single criterion by which that age can be recog- 
nized. In Chapter 5, in presenting the statistical data on adolescent develop- 
ments, it was pointed out that there are a good many physical characters 
involved, and that they do not all appear and develop at exactly the same 
time. In the same chapter, it was pointed out that the designation of a 
particular year, in any individual’s history, as the age of onset of adoles- 
cence, must, therefore, depend upon judgments which may sometimes be 
arbitrary and not exactly in accord with all of the details of the fact. 

In the present study, the time of onset of adolescence has been fixed as 
the date of the first ejaculation, unless there has been evidence that ejacula- 
tion would have been possible at an earlier age if the individual had been 
stimulated to the point of orgasm. When the year of first ejaculation 
coincides with the year in which the first pubic hair appears, and with the 
time of onset of rapid growth in height, and/or with certain other develop- 
ments, there is no question that that year may be accepted as the first year 
of adolescence. Eighty-five per cent of all male histories fall into this 
category. On the other hand, if the first ejaculation follows these other 
events by a year or more, and if it is clear that there was no test of the 
individual’s sexual capacity prior to the first ejaculation, and if there seems 
to be no question of the reliability of the memory in regard to the dates of 
the other adolescent developments, then the age of onset of adolescence is 
better established by events other than ejaculation. Where first ejaculation 
occurs as a nocturnal emission, it usually (though not always) does not 
come until a year or more after the appearance of the other adolescent 
developments, and the onset of adolescence should be set a year or more 
before the first ejaculation. 

To define the time of onset of adolescence by any single criterion does 
not satisfy the reahty as well as a judgment based on all of the pertinent 
data. Even hormones and the 17-ketosteroids cannot be accepted as the 
sole criteria for determining this event, or any other event. The history of 
systematic botany and systematic zoology is replete with attempts to dis- 
cover significant and diagnostic characters which might provide clear-cut 
and absolute bases for systems of classification; but the modern taxon- 
omist finds that the use of a single character inevitably provides a classifi- 
catory system which is artificial and, at least at certain points, in direct 
conflict with data from other sources. Every aspect of a situation is part of 
the reality which one must take into account, if one is to xmderstand that 



300 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


reality. In the present instance, the onset of adolescence must be recognized 
whenever there is any development of any physiologic or physical character 
that pertains to adolescence. The ages of onset of adolescence used for sta- 
tistical analyses in the present chapter, and throughout this whole volume, 
are based upon this use of the multiple characters which are concerned. 

PERCENT DEPENDING ON EACH SOURCE 


SOURCE 

OF FIRST WHEN AGE AT ONSET OF ADOLESCENCE IS: 


EJACULATION 

8-11 

12 

13 

14 

15+ later 

Masturbation 

71.6 

64.8 

58.9 

55.0 

52.1 

Nocturnal emissions 

21.6 

28.2 

35.6 

38.9 

37.1 

Petting 

0.0 

0.3 

0.6 

0.3 

2.2 

Intercourse 

0.6 

1.4 

0.9 

0.9 

3.2 

Homosexual 

2.6 

3.2 

1.2 

2.0 

2.2 

Animal 

0.3 

0.3 

0.2 

0.3 

0.0 

Spontaneous 

3.3 

1.8 

2.6 

2.6 

3.2 

Total 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

Cases 

306 

722 

984 

650 

186 


Table 68. Sources of first ejaculation in relation to age at onset of adolescence 


■■ MASTURBATION 

70 I I NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 

I Ml '■! OTHER SOURCES 

6-M 12 15 14 15+ 

AGE AT ONSET OF ADOLESCENCE 

Figure 89. Sources of first ejaculation in relation to age at onset of adolescence 

The use of multiple characters in a taxonomic classification inevitably 
calls for a certain exercise of subjective judgment, and this is the most 
serious objection to such a procedure; but the errors introduced by judg- 
ment are not likely to be as misleading as the artificialities introduced by 
the use of a single set of criteria in a classification. 






AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


301 


In this study, the determination of the age of onset of adolescence has 
been further complicated by the necessity for depending on the subject’s 
memory for a report of what is supposed to have happened. Considering 
the indefinite nature of the event itself, and this difficulty of obtaining 
accurate records by way of memory (Chapter 4), it is surprising that it 
has been possible to demonstrate correlations between this phenomenon 
and any aspects of sexual behavior. 

It will be recalled that the average age of onset of adolescence for the 
white male population has been calculated as 13 years and 7 months 
(Chapter 5). There are very few boys who reach adolescence prior to age 
10, and few even before age 11. Consequently, in most of the tables 
accompanying the present chapter, the males who were adolescent prior 
to age 1 1 have, for purposes of calculation, been included in one group 
with those who began adolescence at 1 1. In a few instances, the small num- 
ber of available cases has made it necessary to put all those adolescent 
before 12 into the 12-year group. Those who were adolescent after age 15 
are grouped with those who became adolescent at 15. 

In order to make significant comparisons, it has been necessary to con- 
fine the analyses to groups that are homogeneous for sex, race, marital 
status, and educational level, as well as for the age of onset of adolescence. 
The tables in the present chapter cover all of those segments of the popu- 
lation which are now represented in the sample by enough cases to warrant 
statistical treatment. There has been no other basis for selecting the groups 
which are included. 

The first difference to be observed between the males who become adoles- 
cent at an earUer age, and the males who become adolescent at an older 
age, is the fact that the younger-adolescent boys begin regular sexual 
activity of some sort, and begin having a regular outlet, more or less 
coincidently with the onset of adolescence. Some of them, as a matter of 
fact, had already experienced regular orgasm in pre-adolescence. On the 
other hand, the older-adolescent males, despite the fact that they have 
taken four or five years more in reaching adolescence, often delay a year 
or two beyond that before they ejaculate for the first time. Sometimes it is 
still longer before they acquire anything like regular rates of outlet. Early- 
adolescent males ejaculate in the same year in which they become adoles- 
cent in 92 to 100 per cent of the cases (Table 67). The older-adolescent 
males ejaculate in their first year of adolescence in only about 80 per cent 
of the cases. Nearly every one (99.5%) of the younger-maturing boys ac- 
quires a regular sexual outlet between the time of adolescence and age 15. 

There is an interesting correlation of the source of first ejaculation and 
the age of onset of ejaculation (Table 68, Figure 89). For the boys who 
become adolescent by 11 years of age, masturbation provides the first 



302 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Total Outlet and Age at Adolescence 


Frequencies Among All Single Males 






INDICES, 






TOTAL POPULATION 

TOTAL 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

age at 

ADOL. 

CASES 



POPULATIONS ; 





1 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq 

By 

Means 

% 

By 

Me- 

dians 

0/ 

/o 

Incid. 

0 / 

/o 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Activity between adoL~15 


8-11 

395 

3.89 0.84 

2.98 

184 

223 

99.5 

3.91 ± 0.85 

3.00 

12 

1013 

3 53 ± 0.11 

2.64 

167 

197 

98.6 

3.58 O.Il 

2.68 

13 

1481 

2 90 =t: 0 07 

2.19 

137 

163 

97.6 

2 97 0.08 

2.27 

14 

1175 

2.30 0.08 

1.51 

109 

116 

91.1 

2.53 =b 0.08 

‘ 1.80 

15+ 

292 

2.11 rt 0.14 

1,34 

100 

i 100 

85.6 

2.46 0.15 

|1.74 


Activity between 16-20 


8-11 

361 

3.68 =±= 0.18 

2.89 

149 

159 

99.7 

3,69 =i= 0.18 

2.90 

12 

920 

3.37 ± 0.11 

2.48 

137 

136 

99.8 

3.38 ± 0.11 

2.49 

13 

1359 

2.86 0.07 

2.25 

116 

124 

99.6 

2.87 0.07 

2.26 

14 

1128 

2.54 0.07 i 

1.89 

103 

104 

99. 2| 

2.56 =i= 0.07 

1 1,92 

15 + 

398 

2.46 =1= 0.12 

1.82 

100 

100 

99. 7i 

2.49 0.12 1 

1 1.85 


Activity between 21-25 


8-11 

241 

3.30 

rt 

0.22 

2 

,46 

143 

149 

100.0 

' 3.30 

db 

0.22 ! 

2, 

.46 

12 

515 

3.03 

d= 

0.14 

2 

.16 

132 

131 

99.8 

i 3.03 

=fc 

0.14 1 

2 

.16 

13 

769 

2.49 


0.08 

1 

.91 

108 

116 

99.7 

2.50 

=b 

0.08 

1 

.92 

14 

626 

2.40 

=fc 

0.11 

1 

.65 

104 

100 

98.9 

2.43 

zk: 

0.11 : 

1 

.69 

15+ 

257 

2.30 

=±s 

0.14 

1 

.66 

100 

101 

98.8 

2.33 


0.14 1 

1 

.69 


Activity between 26-30 


8-11 

69 

3.84 

=±: 

0.49 

2,81 

152 

178 

100.0 

3.84 

dr 

0.49 

2.81 

12 

122 

3.07 

=h 

0.28 

1.95 

136 

123 

100.0, 

3.07 

dr 

0.28 

1.95 

13 

213 

2.72 

=1= 

0.20 

2.00 

120 

127 

99.5 

2.73 

dr 

0.20 

2.01 

14 

200 

2.26 

db 

0.16 

1.58 

100 

100 

99.0 

2.28 

dr 

0.16 

1.60 

15+ 

98 

2.49 

dr 

0.27 

1.80 

110 

114 

98.0 

2.54 

dr 

0.27 

1.84 





Activity i 

between 

1 31-35 






8-12 

82 1 

3.30 

d= 

0.43 

2.08 

160 

144 

100.0 

3.30. 

dr 

0.43 

2.08 

13 

86 I 

2.78 

d= 

0.40 

1.81 

135 

125 

98.8 

2.81 

dr 

0.40 1 

1.82 

14 

97 1 

2.06 

dr 

0.20 

1.45 

100 

100 

100.0 

2.06 

dr 

0.20 i 

1.45 


Table 69. Total sexual outlet, in single males, as related to age at onset of 

adolescence 


Based on total white male population, including all education levels. The relative 
sizes of the frequencies are shown in the boldface columns as indices, with the lowest fre- 
quency in each 5-year period equalling 100 per cent. 



AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


303 


ejaculation in nearly three-quarters of the cases (71.6 per cent); but for 
the boys who become adolescent last, masturbation is the source of the 
first outlet in only about half of the cases (52.1 per cent). Nocturnal dreams, 
on the contrary, are the first source for only 21.6 per cent of the younger- 
adolescent boys, but for 37.1 per cent of the late-adolescent males. The 
boys who mature first more often act deliberately in going after their 
first outlet; the boys who mature last more often depend upon the involun- 
tary reactions which bring nocturnal emissions. 

The sexual activities of these boys who start the earliest are far from in- 
cidental. Between adolescence and 15 years, their rates are higher than the 
rates of any other group of single males, of any age, in any segment of the 
white population. Consequent on their quicker start, there are 16 per cent 
more of the early-adolescent boys than there are of the late-adolescent 
boys, who are active between adolescence and 15. Early-adolescent boys 
have four or five years in which to make a start in that period, while the 
later-adolescent boys have only one year in the period; but the higher inci- 
dence of activity among the early-adolescent boys must depend, in part, 
upon the generally higher level of performance in the group. This is con- 
firmed by the fact that in all subsequent age periods there is still a slight 
but consistent difference in incidence in favor of the boys who became 
adolescent earliest. 


FREQUENCIES OF TOTAL OUTLET 

The younger-adolescent and the older-adolescent males differ most in 
respect to the frequencies with which they engage in sexual activities. 
Tables 69 and 71 give the data for single and married males respec- 
tively. 

Upon examining the record for the single males it will be seen that the 
boys who became adolescent first (by 1 1 years of age) have, on an average, 
about twice as much sexual outlet per week as the older-adolescent boys 
have during their early teens. If the means for the total populations are 
compared, the early-adolescent boys have 1.84 times as high frequencies 
as the slower males. If the medians are compared, the younger-adolescent 
boys have 2.23 times as much outlet. This is a material difference, and it is 
a real difference, since the averages for this age period between adolescence 
and 15 are calculated for the active years only, and not averaged with the 
pre-adolescent years (page 1 10). 

These younger-adolescent boys constitute the most active group of 
single (unmarried) males in the whole population. In Chapter 7 it was con- 
cluded that out of all single males, taken as a group, the sexually most 
active are the 16-20-year old group; and that among the married males 
the highest frequencies are also in the 16~20-year period. While boys 
below 16, taken as a group, do not have frequencies as high as do the boys 


11 



304 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Total Outlet and Age at Adolescence 


Frequencies Among Single Males, by Educational Level 





INDICES, 





TOTAL POPULATION 

TOTAL 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

AGE AT 

adol. 

CASES 


POPULATIONS 





Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

By 

Means 

% 

By 

Me- 

dians 

% 

Incid. 

0 / 

/o 

Mean ■ 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Educ. level 0-8 Activity between adoI.-15 


8-12 

131 

5.10 


0.43 

3, 

.75 

244 1 

329 

97.7 

5.22 

:±: 

0. 

.44 

3 

.88 

13 

180 

3.34 

=b 

0.24 

2, 

.38 

160 

196 

97.8 

3.41 

sir 

0, 

.25 

2 

.45 

14 

270 

2.47 

=fc 

0.21 

1. 

.25 

118 

110 

87.8 1 

2.82 

=i= 

0. 

.23 : 

1 

.61 

15+ 

115 

2.09 

=fc 

0.22 

1. 

.14 

100 

100 

90.4 

2.32 

=b 

0. 

.24 ■ 

1 

.34 



Educ. 

level 0-8 


Activity between 1 6-20 






8-12 

114 

5.54 

d= 

0.49 

3, 

.85 

226 

221 

100.0 

5.54 

=fc 

0. 

49 

3 

85 

13 

165 

3.40 

d= 

0.22 

2, 

.71 

139 

156 

99.4 

3.42 


0. 

22 

1 2 

73 

14 

266 

2.88 

=fc 

0.21 

1, 

.96 

118 

113 

97.7 

2.95 

=i= 

0. 

21 

; 2 

03 

15+ 

154 

2,45 

=h 

0.20 

1, 

.74 

100 

100 

97.4 

2.51 

d= 

0. 

21 

I 

79 



Educ. 

level 0-8 


Activity between 21 

-25 






8-12 

46 

5.34 

=t= 

0.84 

3, 

.25 

197 : 

175 

97.8 1 

5.46 

=lr 

0. 

,85 

3, 

30 

13-14 

188 

3.00 

=fc 

0.27 

2 

.01 

111 

108 

97.3 i 

3.08 


0. 

,28 

2, 

07 

15+ 

84 

2.71 

=1= 

0.30 

1, 

.86 

100 j 

100 

98.8 1 

2.74 


0. 

,30 

1, 

89 


Educ. level 9-12 Activity between adoL-15 


8-12 

170 

4.23 


0.30 

3.11 

163 

182 

98.8 

4.28 

=b 

0.30 

3.16 

13 

197 

3.63 

=±= 

0.28 

2.35 

140 

137 

98.5 

3.69 

=b 

0.28 

2.40 

14 

213 

2.59 

db 

0.19 

1.78 

100 

104 

91.5 

2.83 


0.20 

2.04 

15+ 

42 

2.60 

=b 

0.46 

1.71 

100 

100 

88.1 

2.95 

=fc 

0.49 

1.92 



Educ. 

level 9 

-12 

Activity between 1 6-20 




8-12 

162 

4.23 

=fc: 

0.31 

’ 3.25 

144 

139 

99.4 

4.25 

=b 

0.31 

3.28 

13 

196 

3.73 


0.26 

2.80 

127 

120 

100.0 

3.73 

da 

0.26 

2.80 

14 

207 

2.94 

=i= 

0.19 

2.38 

100 

102 

99.5 

2.95 

=b 

0.19 

2.40 

15+ 

58 

3.21 

=fc 

0.45 

2.34 

109 

100 

100.0 

3.21 

=b 

0.45 

2.34 



Educ. 

level 9 

-12 

Activity between 21 

-25 




8-12 

63 

3.87 

db 

0.53 

2.92 

291 

292 

100.0 

3.87 

db 

0.53 

1 2.92 

13-14 

144 

2.91 

=fc 

0.25 

2.08 

219 

208 

98.6 

2.95 

=b 

0.26 

2.13 

15+ 

25 

1.33 

=b 

0.24 

1.00 

100 

100 

96.0 

1.38 

db 

0.25 

j 1.05 


(Table continued on next page) 



AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


305 


Frequencies Among Single Males, by Educational Level 






INDICES, 






TOTAL POPULATION 

TOTAL 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

AGE AT 

CASES 



POPULATIONS j 




ADOL. 










Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

By 

Means 

% 

By 

Me- 

dians 

% 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Educ. level 13+ Activity between adol.-15 


8-12 

893 1 

3.31 

rk 

0.10 

2.57 

176 

185 

98.9 

3.35 

=fc 

0.10 

2.60 

13 

896 ’ 

2.77 

dtz 

0.09 

2.27 

147 

163 1 

97.7 

2.84 

dz 

0.09 

2.35 

14 

600 

2.18 

=fc 

0.09 

1.56 

116 

112 

92.0 

2.37 

=±= 

0.10 

1.84 

15+ 

114 

1.88 

=t 

0.21 

1.39 

100 

100 

78.1 

2.41 

=±= 

0.24 

2.00 



Educ. 

level 13+ 

Activity between 16 

-20 




8-12 

893 

3.00 

=ir 

0.09 

2.34 

133 

134 

99.8 

3.01 

=b 

0.09 

2.34 

13 

896 

2.56 

db 

0.07 

2.10 

114 

121 

99.8 

2.56 

=b 

0.07 

2.10 

14 

600 

2.26 

=b 

0.08 

1.74 

101 

100 

99.8 

2.26 

dz 

0.08 

1.75 

15+ 

170 

2.25 

=b 

0.14 

1.86 

100 

107 

99.4 

2.26 

dz 

0.14 

1.87 



Educ. 

level 13+ 

Activity between 21 

-25 




8-12 

561 

2.79 

=h 

0.11 

2.06 

129 

132 

100.0 

2.79 

dz 

0.11 

2.06 

13 

566 

2.25 


0.07 

1.80 

104 

115 

99.5 1 

2.26 

dz 

0.07 

1.81 

14 

393 

2.17 


0.10 

1.56 

100 

100 

100.0 

2.17 

dz 

0.10 

1.56 

15+ 

136 

2.23 

=h 

0.17 

1.72 

103 

110 

99.3 

2.25 

dz 

0.17 

1.74 



Educ. 

level 13 + 

Activity between 26-30 




8-12 

176 

3.03 

=h 

0.24 

2.17 

133 

133 

100.0 

3.03 

dz 

0.24 

2.17 

13 

179 

2.52 

d= 

0.19 

1.87 

111 

115 

99.4 

2.53 

dz 

0.19 

1.88 

14 

152 

2.27 

d= 

0.16 

1.63 

100 

100 

100.0 

2.27 

dz 

0.16 

1.63 

15+ 

48 

2.45 

=b 

0.41 

1.65 

108 

101 

100.0 

2.45 

dz 

0.41 

1.65 


Table 70. Total outlet, as related to educational level and age at onset of 

adolescence 


in their late teens, we now find that the activities of the early-adolescent 
individuals of this early teen-age group surpass those of the 16-20-year 
olds. 

The mean frequencies of total outlet for the late teen-age boys, taken as 
a whole group, are 3.2 per week. The mean frequencies for the early- 
adolescent boys during the period between 1 1 and 15 average 3.9 per week. 
If this early-adolescent population is broken down into three educational 
levels (Table 70), the mean frequencies become 5.1 per week among the 




Total Outlet and Age at Adolescence 


Frequencies Among All Married Males 






INDICES, 






TOTAL POPULATION 

TOTAL 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

age at 

CASES 



POPULATIONS 




adol. 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- ' 

dian 

Freq. 

1 

By 

Means 

% 

Bv 

Me- 

dians 

0/ 

. 0 

Incid. 

/o 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Activity between 16-20 


8-11 

23 

7.27 =1= 1.31 

6.00 

220 

240 

100.0 

7.27 =t: 1.31 j 

6.00 

12 ' 

65 ’ 

5.21 =i= 0.64 

3.42 i 

158 

137 1 

100.0 

5.21 =i= 0.64 i 

3.42 

13 

85 

4 87 ± 0.52 

3.57 

147 

143 

100.0 

4.87 0.52 

3.57 

14 

102 

4.01 =»= 0.39 

2.97 

121 

119 

100.0 

4.01 0.39 

2.97 

15-f 

31 

3.30 0.44 

2.50 

100 

100 

100.0 

3.30 =fc: 0.44 

2.50 

Activity between 21-25 

8-11 1 

70 

4.93 =b 0.63 

3.41 1 

151 

133 

100.0 

4.93 =b 0.63 i 

3.41 

12 S 

183 

4.48 ± 0.30 

3.37 

137 

131 

100.0 

4.48 =i= 0.30 i 

3.37 

13 

245 

3.94 =i= 0.26 

3.05 

121 

119 

100.0 

3.94 =fc 0.26 

3.05 

14 

278 

3.55 0.21 

2.67 

109 

104 

100.0 

3.55 0.21 

2.67 

15+ 

99 

3.26 ± 0.25 

2.57 

100 

100 

100.0 

3.26 ± 0.25 ’ 

2.57 


Activity between 26-30 


8-11 

82 

4.32 

d= 

0.52 

3.09 

147 

134 

100.0 

4.32 

=fc 

0.52 

3.09 

12 

168 ; 

3.68 

=b 

0.27 

2.86 

125 

124 

100.0 i 

3.68 

dz 

0.27 

2.86 

13 

258 

3,26 

d= 

0.19 

2.59 

111 

112 

100.0 

3.26 

dc 

0.19 

2.59 

14 

276 

2.93 

=b 

0.15 

2.37 

100 

103 

99.3 

2.95 

=b 

0.15 

2.38 

15+ 

97 

3.42 

rb 

0.37 

2.31 

116 

100 

100.0 i 

3,42 

d= 

0.37 

2.31 


Activity between 31-35 


8-11 

46 

3.02 ± 0.34 

2.38 

112 

114 

100.0 

3.02 =i= 0.34 

2.38 

12 

114 

3.21 =i= 0.30 

2.53 

120 

121 

100.0 

3.21 =i= 0.30 

2.53 

13 

193 

2.97 0,20 

2.29 

111 

109 

100.0 

2.97 =5= 0.20 

2.29 

14 

223 

2.68 0.17 

2.09 

100 

100 

100.0 

2.68 =fc= 0.17 

2.09 

15+ 

85 

3.14 ± 0.33 

2.20 

117 

105 

100.0 1 

3.14 ± 0.33 

2.20 

1 


Activity between 36-40 


12 

143 

2.87 =t 0.25 ! 

2.17 

121 

115 

99.3 

2.89 =1= 0.25 

2.18 

13 

152 

2.76 ± 0.21 

2.21 

116 

117 

100.0 

2.76 =1= 0.21 

2.21 

14 

199 

2.37 ± 0.16 

1.89 

100 1 

100 

100.0 

2.37 =t= 0.16 i 

1.89 

15+ 

70 

2.53 0.26 

1.98 

107 

105 

100.0 

2.53 0.26 

1.98 


Activity between 41-45 


13 

84 

2.45 =>= 0.26 

1.95 

121 

119 

100.0 

2.45 0.26 

1 1.95 

14 j 

123 

2.03 0.20 

1.64 

100 

100 

100.0 

2.03 =b 0.20 

1.64 


Activity between 46-50 


13 

52 

2.22 =i= 0.33 

1.63 

112 

121 

100.0 

2.22 0.33 

1.63 

14 

92 

1.99 0.25 

1.35 

100 

100 

98.9 

2.02 ± 0.25 

1.38 


Table 71. Total sexual outlet in married males, as related to age at onset of 

adolescence 


Based on total married population, including all educational levels. 

306 



AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


307 


boys who never go beyond eighth grade in school, 4.2 per week for the 
boys who go into high school but not beyond, and 3.3 per week for the 
boys who will ultimately go to college. For each educational level, the 
maximum frequencies for the early-adolescent boys are in the earUest age 
period (except for the grade school group, where there is a slight increase 
in rate in the next two age periods; but the samples on these grade school 
boys are too small to be accepted as final). This location of the peak of 
sexual activity in the earlier adolescent years is scientifically most interest- 
ing, and it may have considerable social significance. The data emphasize 
the importance of a breakdown by age of onset of adolescence, in any final 
analysis of the problems of sexual behavior. 

Not only do these earlier-developing boys have four years head start, 
and not only do they have higher rates of activity in those initial years, but 
they continue to have higher rates throughout the subsequent age periods. 
In the fifteen years that lie between ages 16 and 30, the younger-developing 
boys have about half again as much outlet as the later-developing 
boys. There is still a discernible difference in the age group 31 to 35, which 
is 20 to 25 years after the time of onset of adolescence 1 Considering the 
multiplicity of other factors that may modify the frequencies of sexual 
activity, it is surprising to find such a long-time correlation with the age of 
onset of adolescence. In spite of their early start, and in spite of their much 
higher expenditure of energy in sexual activity, these early-maturing males 
remain more active than those who were delayed in their adolescence. 

In the histories of married males, the age of onset of adolescence proves 
to be as significant as in the histories of single males (Table 71). This is 
astounding! It might have been expected that the frequency of sexual 
activity for a married male would depend, to at least some degree, upon 
the wife’s interest and wilUngness to engage in marital intercourse; and 
certainly individual histories provide abundant examples of marital part- 
ners having to adjust their rates in accordance with each other’s wishes. 
Nevertheless, during the 16- to 20-year period, the outlets of the married 
males who were adolescent at an early age are about twice as high as the 
outlets of the males who were not adolescent until a later age — and this is 
exactly the difference that would have been found if they had remained 
unmarried. The effect persists throughout the lives of the married males, 
as far as data are available. While the differential between the groups 
decreases with advancing age, the rates of the younger-maturing group in 
the 46- to 50-year old period are still about 20 per cent higher than the 
rates of the slower-maturing group. Thirty-five years after the onset of 
adolescence, there is still a discernible effect, which persists in spite of 
marriage and in spite of all of the other events that affect sexual fre- 
quencies ! 



308 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


SINOLE MALES 


MARRIED MALES 


A<SE AT 
ADOL 
8*11 

12 

15 

14 

15T 


8-n 

12 

15 

14 

15+ 


8*11 

12 

(5 

14 

15+ 


MEAN MEDIAN MEAN 


M£DIAH 


AGE GROUP AOOL-55 



AGE GROUP 26-30 



8-n 

12 

15 

14 

15+ 


AGE GROUP 31-55 5 .IJ 


8*12 

13 

14 




12 

15 

14 

15+ 


12 

13 

14 
15+ 


AGE GROUP 26-30 



Figure 90. Relation of age at onset of adolescence to frequency of total outlet 


Based on the total population. Relative lengths of bars show mean and median 
frequencies for each adolescent group. Single males m columns 1 and 2; married males 
in columns 3 and 4. Effects shown as continuing up to 30 years after onset of adolescence. 



AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


309 


FACTORS INVOLVED 

While the data in Tables 69 to 71 indicate a definite correlation between 
the ages of adolescence and the frequency of sexual activity, it must not 
be concluded that a simple causal relationship exists. Such misinterpreta- 
tions of correlations are too commonly made, both in popular thinking 
and in technical scientific experiment. In many cases, more basic factors 
are involved, and two sets of correlated phenomena may be simply end 
products of the same forces. In the present instance, several basic factors 
may be operating. It is possible that the fact that an early-adolescent indi- 
vidual becomes sexually mature and erotically responsive at an earlier 
age, is the significant item. This gives him more years to become conditioned 
toward sexual experience before he reaches the teen-ages where social 
restraints become more significant. To put the matter in another way, the 
boy who becomes adolescent at 10 or 1 1 has not had as many years to build 
up inhibitions against sexual activity as the boy who does not mature 
until 15 or later; and it is quite possible (but not specifically demonstrable 
from the available data) that the younger boy plunges into sexual activity 
with less restraint and with more enthusiasm than the boy who starts at a 
later date. Moreover, it is possible that the patterns which are established 
by the earliest sexual activity, meaning patterns of higher frequency for 
younger-maturing boys, and patterns of lower frequency for older-matur- 
ing boys, are the patterns by which the individual’s subsequent hfe is 
ordered. At least part of the long-time effects may depend upon psycho- 
logic learning and conditioning. 

But it is also probable that there are physiologic bases for the differ- 
ences. It is diflScult to know just what these may be, for, unfortunately, 
there are next to no studies of physiologic capacities in relation to the age 
at which individuals become adolescent. There are studies of younger 
children, adolescents, and older adults which show correlations between 
their absolute ages and their physiologic performances (Robinson 1938, 
and the references therein). There is at least one study (Richey 1931) which 
shows that there is some correlation between age at the onset of adoles- 
cence and blood pressures (systohe, diastolic, and pulse), the heights and 
weights that are ultimately attained, and some anatomic developments. 
Most significantly, these characters distinguish the various adolescent 
groups as much as six years before the onset of adolescence, and for at 
least six years after the beginning of adolescence. Further investigation of 
a larger number of physiologic characters operating over a longer period 
of years seems not to have been made. On the psychologic side, Terman 
(1925), in his study of geniuses, found that the individuals with the highest 
IQ’s were more often those who became adolescent first. It can, therefore, 
be suggested that the frequency of sexual activity may, to some degree, be 
dependent upon a general metabolic level which the individual maintains 
through much of his life. One who functions at a higher level at one period 



Masturbation Among Single Males, by Educational Level 


AGE AT 

TOTAL POPULATION 

CASES 

INDICES, 

TOTAL 

POPULATIONS 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

ADOL. 

1 

1 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

By 

Means 

0/ i 
/o 

By 

Me- 

dians 

0/ 

/Q 

lincid. 

o/ 

y 0 j 

Mean 

Frequency 

1 

1 Me- 
; dian 
Freq. 


Educ. level 0-8 Activity between adoI.~15 


8-12 

131 

2.65 

dh 

0.25 

1.70 

53 

0 

230 

284 

96.9 

2 

73 

dr 

0 

25 

1.76 

13 

180 

1.71 

dr 

0.16 

1.01 

53 

8 

149 

168 

91.7 

1 

87 

db 

0 

17 

1.19 

14 

270 

1.25 

d= 

0.11 

0.60 

53 

7 

109 

100 

80.4 

1 

55 

rb 

0 

12 

0.94 

15+ 

115 

1.15 

=fc 

0.14 

0.60 

58 

0 

100 

100 

79.1 

1 

45 

rb 

0 

16 

0.90 



Educ. level 0-8 


Activity betw’een 16-20 






8-12 

114 

1.47 

rb 

0.18 

0.77 

28 

3 

179 

197 

91.2 

1 

61 

db 

0 

19 

0.92 

13 

165 

0.96 

dr 

0.09 

0.50 

31 

1 

117 

128 

86.1 

1 

11 

dr 

0 

10 

0.71 

14 

266 

0.82 

dr 

0.07 

0.39 

31, 

9 

100 

100 

82.3 

1 

,00 

rb 

0. 

08 

'0.53 

15-1- 

154 

0.89 

zb 

0.09 

0.47 

40, 

0 

109 

120 

89.0 

!. 

.00 

dr 

0. 

,10 

iO. 58 



Educ. level 9-12 


Activity between 

adol.- 

-15 





8-12 

170 

2.32 

rb 

0.17 

1.64 

58, 

2 

157 

132 

97.1 

2 

39 

db 

0. 

,17 : 

1.72 

13 

197 

2.10 

rb 

0,19 

1.21 

60, 

6 

142 

115 

91.9 

2 

28 

db 

0, 

,20^ 

1.38 

14 

213 

1.62 

rfc 

0.12 

1.06 

66, 

0 

109 

101 

85.0 

1 

91 

rb 

0. 

,13 

i.38 

15+ 

42 

1.48 

dr 

0.26 

1.05 

59, 

3 

100 

100 

83.3 

1 



77 

dr 

0, 

.28 

1.40 


Educ. level 9-12 Activity between 16-20 


8-12 

162 

1.37 ±0.11 

0.80 

34.9 

120 • 

154 

92.0 

1.49 ±0.12 1 

0.91 

13 

196 

1.45 0.17 

0.74 

41,1 

127 

142 

90.3 

1.60 =fc 0.19 ; 

0,88 

14 

207 

1.14=fc0.1o! 

0.52 

42.5 

100 

100 

84.5 

1.34=^0.11 i 

|0.77 

15-t- 

58 

1.34=1=0.19 

0.87 

44.4 

117 

ft i 

167 

94.8 

1.42 0.19 i 

10.92 

1 


Educ. level 13+ Activity between adol.-15 


8-12 

893 

2.61 =t0.09 

1.98 

79.7 

175 

194 

90.3 

2.89 ±0.10 

2.24 

13 

896 

2.15 ±0.08 

1.59 

78.8 

144 

159 

83.4 i 

2.58 ±0.08 

2.01 

14 

600 

1.67 ±0.09 

1.02 

78.6 

112 

100 

73.2 

2.29 ±0.10 

1.82 

15-i- 

114 

1.49 ±0.19 

1,02 

80.9 

100 i 

100 

60.5 

2.46 ±0.25 

1.93 


Educ. level 13+ Activity between 16-20 


8-12 

893 

2.02 ±0.08 

1.43 

67.2 

142 

164 

91.8 

2.20 ±0.08 

1.63 

13 

896 

1.66 ±0.06 

1.08 

65.4 

117 

124 

88.6 

1.88 ±0.06 

1.38 

14 

600 

1.45 ±0.07 

0.92 

64.2 

102 

106 

84.5 

1.72 ±0.07 

1.31 

15-f- 

170 

1 

1.42 ±0.12 

0.87 

63.6 

100 

100 

83.5 

1.70 ±0.13 

1.23 


Educ. level 13+ Activity between 21-25 


8-12 

561 

1.46 ±0.08 

0.78 

53.1 

128 

150 

90.4 

1.61 ±0.08 

0.92 

13 

566 

1.18 ±0.06 

0.68 

52.2 

104 

131 

85.7 

1,38 ±0.07 

0.85 

14 

393 

1.14 dr 0.07 

0.52 

53.2 

100 

100 

82.7 

1.37 ±0.08 

0.80 

15-1- 

136 

1.17 ±0.13 

0.55 j 

i 1 

53.0 

103 

106 

81. 6| 

1.43 ±0.15 

0.81 


Educ. level 13+ Activity between 26-30 


8-12 

176 

1.36±0.17 

0.58 

49.8 

137 

132 

89.8 

1.52±0.18 

0.71 

13 

179 

0.99 ± 0.09 

0.49 

42.7 

100 

111 

80.4 

1.23 ±0.10 

0.78 

14 

152 

1.11 ±0.13 

0.44 

52.9 

112 

100 

82.2 

1.35±0.15| 

0.66 


Table 72. Frequency of masturbation, as related to age at onset of adolescence 

310 



AGE OF AT^OLESC^l^CE A1S[0 SEXUAL OUTLET 


Sfl 


Masturbation and Age at Adolescence 


Accumulative Incidence 
Educational Level 13-h 


»AGE 

adolescent 

BY 11 

adolescent 
AT 12 

adolescent 
AT 13 

adolescent 
AT 14 

ADOLESCENT 

AT 15 + 

Case§ 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

307 

0.3 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

650 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

9 

307 

2.9 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

650 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

10 

307 

21.2 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

650 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

11 

307 

79.8 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

650 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

12 

307 

87.9 

724 

71.4 

986 

0.0 

650 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

13 

307 

91.2 

724 

80.5 

986 

65.4 

650 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

14 

307 

93.2 

724 

85.5 

986 

77.5 

650 

60.2 

187 

0.0 

15 

307 

93.8 

724 

89.0 

986 

82.9 

650 

71.7 

187 

39.6 

16 

307 

94.8 

724 

90.2 

986 

85.8 

650 

76.9 

187 

63.1 

17 

307 

95.1 

724 

91.3 

985 

88.6 

649 

80.3 

187 

73.3 

18 

297 

95.3 

706 

92.5 

957 

89.8 

630 

84.3 

187 

78.1 

19 

282 

96.5 

660 

93.3 

897 

90.3 

593 

86.3 

180 

80.0 

20 

266 

97.0 

609 

93.8 

802 

91.4 

529 

87.7 

1 170 

82.4 

21 

243 

97.9 

510 

95.1 

689 , 

91.9 

472 

88.8 

154 

85.1 

22 

205 

97.6 

407 

95.8 

568 

92.6 

401 

89.8 

126 

88.1 

23 

183 

97.3 

339 

96,5 

474 

93.2 

332 

90.1 

105 

88.6 

24 

152 

97.4 

274 

96.7 

391 

93.9 

288 

88.5 

84 

88.1 

25 

131 

99.2 

238 

97.1 

345 

94.5 

246 i 

89.8 

77 1 

87.0 

26 

112 

99,1 

206 

97.1 

310 

95.8 

224 

92.0 

67 

88.1 

27 

90 

98.9 

174 

97.1 

278 

96.8 

205 

91.7 

54 

90.7 

28 

86 

98,8 

155 : 

97.4 

248 

97.2 

188 

91.0 



29 

74 

98.6 

! 144 

97.2 

223 

96.9 

174 

90.2 



30 

64 

98.4 

125 

97.6 

206 

97.1 

162 

92.6 



31 

60 

98.3 

no 

97.3 

188 

96.8 

156 

92.3 



32 

55 

98.2 

100 

97.0 

: 171 

97.1 

149 

93.3 



33 

53 

98.1 

91 

96.7 

154 

97.4 

135 

! 92.6 



34 



85 

97.6 

142 

97.9 

124 

93.5 



35 



77 

97.4 

131 

97.7 

119 

94.1 



36 



70 

97.1 

119 

97.5 

114 

94.7 



37 



64 

96.9 

105 

98.1 

103 

94.2 



38 



60 

96.7 

100 

98.0 

100 

95.0 



39 



57 

96.5 

89 

97.8 

93 

94.6 



40 



50 

98.0 

83 

97.6 

87 

95.4 




Table 73. Experience in masturbation, as affected by age at onset of adolescence 
Accumulative incidence data based on males of the college level. 



312 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


MASTURBATION PRE-MARITAL HOMOSEXUAL 

INTERCOURSE OUTLET 



EDUCATIONAL LEVEL 13 + 



Figure 91. Relation of age at onset of adolescence to frequencies of masturba- 
tion, pre-marital intercourse with companions, and homosexual outlet 

Based on single males. Relative tenths of bars show mean frequencies for each 
group. Efifects shown as continuing up to 10 years for a grade school group 
(0-8); and up to 20 years for a college group (IS-j-)* 



AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


313 


in his life is likely to function at a higher level through most of his life, 
barring illness and physical accidents that produce permanent incapacities. 
Casual observation would suggest that such an individual is not worn 
down by his quicker and more frequent responses to everyday situations, 
and there would seem to be no more reason for his being exhausted by his 
frequent sexual responses. Moreover, it is possible that the factors account- 
ing for these other evidences of high metabolic level also account for early 
adolescence. Whether this is the correct interpretation is a matter which 
will have to be investigated through extensive research on the physiologic 
quahties of sexually high and low rating individuals. 

SOURCES OF OUTLET 

An examination of the sources of outlet of the younger adolescent males 
indicates that their higher rates of total outlet are not consequent upon an 
increased frequency in each and every kind of sexual activity. On the con- 
trary, nearly all of the increased frequency comes from masturbation, pre- 
marital intercourse, and the homosexual (Tables 72-78). 

Masturbation. While 90 per cent of the younger-maturing boys are 
involved in masturbation during their early teens, only 60 per cent of the 
late-maturing boys are involved in that same period. In successive five- 
year periods the number of earlier-adolescent males who are masturbating 
is 10 per cent to 15 per cent higher than the number of later-adolescent 
males who are so involved. Ultimately, nearly 99 per cent of the younger- 
adolescent boys have some experience in masturbation, while only 93 per 
cent of the later-adolescent boys are ever involved (Table 73, Figure 92). 
The younger-maturing boys have about twice as much masturbation as 
the late-maturing males during the early adolescent years, and 50 to 60 
per cent more masturbation between 16 and 25 years of age, if they remain 
single. The frequencies calculated for the active population (z.e., for that 
portion of the population that is involved in this activity at all) are 
definitely higher in every age period for the males who matured first 
(Table 72). At this college level, these higher frequencies in masturbation 
are the chief source of the higher total outlets of the younger-maturing 
males. 

Pre-marital Intercourse. In their earlier adolescent years, the younger- 
maturing boys are also much involved in pre-marital intercourse. Their 
frequencies are much higher than the frequencies for the late-adolescent 
group in this period (Table 74). In subsequent age periods, the frequencies 
among the boys who became adolescent first remain 50 per cent to 75 per 
cent higher. 

During this early adolescent period, the younger-maturing boys of the 
college level are involved in pre-marital intercourse in 1 1.8 per cent of the 
cases, while the older-maturing boys are involved in 7.0 per cent of the 



Pre-Marital Intercourse and Age at Adolescence 







INDICES, 





TOTAL POPULATION 

TOTAL 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

AGE AT 

CASES 




POPULATIONS 



AUOL. 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

By 

Means 

/o 

By 

Me- 

dians 

% 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 1 

Frequency I 

1 ^ ;Freq. 

I 1 


Educ. level 0-8 Activity between adol.-15 


8-12 

131 

1.86 

ab 

0.33 

0.44 

37.3 

286 

220 

66.4i 2 

80 

=fc 

0 

46 

1 

39 

13 

180 

1.13 

=t: 

0.15 

0.20 

35.3 

174 

100 

60.0 1 1 

88 

=b 

0 

2i 

I 

03 

14 

270 

0.80 

=fc: 

0.12 

0.00 

34.7 

123 

— 

41.5 1 

94 


0 

25 

1 

.25 

15 + 

115 

0.65 


0.15 

0.00 

33.0 

100 

— 

31.3 i 2 

i 

08 

da 

0 

37 

1 

.25 



Educ. level 0-8 

Activity between 

16-20 







8-12 

114 

3.06 

dfc 

0.44 

1,58 

58.8 

306 

396 

86.0: 3, 

.56 

dr 

0^ 

,49 i 

T 


13 

165 

1.64 


0.16 

0.79 

53.3 

162 

198 

89.1 j 1, 

.84 

d= 

0. 

.17 ! 

1 

.09 

14 

266 

1.46 

=h 

0.16 

0.47 

56.9 

145 

118 

82.3; 1. 

.78 

=b 

0. 

.18 i 

0 

.83 

15+ 

154 

1.01 


0.14 

0.40 

45.2 

100 

100 

74.7 1 1, 

.35 

db 

0. 

,17 ^ 

0 

.73 


Educ. level 9-12 Activity between adoL-15 


8-12 

170 

0.91 =b0,20 

0.00 

22.7 

162 



48.2- 

1.88 0.391 

'0.48 

13 

197 

0.96 =5=0.15 

0.01 

27.7 

171 

— 

50,3 

1.91 0.27 . 

0.89 

14 

213 

0.56 =1=0.13 

0.00 

22,9 

100 

— 

35.2 

1.60 ±0.35' 

'0.80 


Educ. level 9-12 Activity between 16-20 


8-12 : 

162 ; 

1.58 ±0.24 

0.29 

40.3 

155 

161 

71. 0| 

12.23 =fc0.32 

0.78 

13 

196 

1.54=5=0.18 

0.57 

43.8 

151 

317 

76.5! 

! 2.01 =fc 0.22 

[1.16 

14 

207 I 

1.02=5=0.15 

0.38 

37.9 

100 

211 

72.5 1 

i 1.40 =fc 0.19 

1 

0.77 


Educ. level 13+ Activity between adoI.-15 


8-12 i 

893 

0.11 0.02 : 

0,00 i 

3.2 

1100 

— 

ll.S|0.90±0.16 

0.29 

13 

896 

0.07 =fc 0,02 

0.00 

2.5 

700 

— 

1 7.8|0.87 :i=0.18 

0.34 

14 

600 

0.06 =1=0.02 ! 

0,00 

2.6 

600 

— 

7.7 0.71 ±0.17 

0.29 

154- 

114 

0.01 =±=0.00 

0.00 

0.4 

100 

— 

7.0 0.11 ±0.04 

0.08 


Educ. level 13+ Activity between 16-20 


8-12 

1 893 

0.30 

=b 

0.03 

0.00 

9.9 

177 1 

— 

[43.8 

0.68 

afc 

0.07 

0.24 

13 

896 

0.22 

d= 

0.03 i 

0.00 

8.7 

129 

— 

35.6 

0.62 

d= 

0.06 

0.20 

14 

600 

0.21 

=fc 

0.03 1 

0.00 

9.3 

124 

— 

35.5 

0.59 

da 

0.08 

0.17 

15+ 

1 170 

0.17 

=±= 

0.05 

0.00 

7.7 

100 ! 

— 

32.9 

0.52 

da 

0.13 

0.21 



Educ. level 13 + 

Activity between 

. 21-25 




8-12 

561 

0.54 

dr 

0.06 1 

0.03 

19.6 

154 

300 

55.3 

0.97 

at 

0.10 

0.36 

13 

566 

0,40 

=i= 

0.05 

0,01 

17.4 

114 

100 

51.4 

0.77 

ab 

0.09 

0.30 

14 

393 

0.35 

da 

0.05 

0.01 

16,4 

100 

100 

50.9 

0.69 

at 

0.09 

0.28 

15+ 

136 

"0.38 

db 

0.09 

0.01 

17.1 

109 

100 

50.7 

0.74 

da 

0.16 

0.34 



Educ. level 13+ 

Activity between 26-30 




8-12 i 

176 

0.78 

d= 

0.11 i 

0.13 1 

28.6 

144 

186 

64.2 

1.22 

da 

0.15 

0.48 

13 

179 

0.80 

da 

0.15 

0.07 

34.4 

148 

100 

59.2 

1.35 

da 

0.24 

0.51 

14 

152 

0.54 

da 

0.08 

0.07 

25.8 

100 

100 

58.6 

0.92 

db 

0.13 

0.44 


Table 74. Frequency of pre-marital intercourse, as related to age at onset of 

adolescence 


Data cover the pre-marital intercourse which is had with companions. 

314 






AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


315 


cases. The active incidence figures beyond 15 years of age are more or less 
the same for each of the adolescent groups; but the accumulative incidence 
figures (showing the number of males from each adolescent group who 
are ever involved, in the course of their lives) show some striking differ- 
ences between the adolescent groups (Table 75, Figure 93). Ultimately 95 
per cent of the early-adolescent boys of this college group obtain experi- 
ence in heterosexual intercourse, either through pre-marital or marital 
relations, by age 30; but hardly more than 80 per cent of the late-adoles- 
cent males have arrived at such experience in intercourse by that age. For 
the males who become adolescent at intermediate ages, i.e., at 12, 13, or 
14 years of age, the accumulative incidence curves are intermediate (Table 
75, Figure 93). Ultimately the curves for the groups which are adolescent 
at 12 to 14 reach the same level (that is, 95%) which is obtained by the 
earlier-adolescent boys, but the group which does not become adolescent 
until 15 or later shows no evidence that it will get much beyond the 80 
per cent mark. It is amazing that there should be nearly 20 per cent of 
these late-adolescent males who have not had some sort of heterosexual 
coitus by age 35. Most of the late-adolescent males who still lack coital 
experience in their thirties, have depended upon masturbation and noc- 
turnal dreams rather than upon any other socio-sexual source for their 
outlet. An unusual proportion of these late-adolescent males is introvert 
and socially timid, and a considerable number is not yet married at 35 
years of age (Table 76, Figure 95). 

Homosexual Outlet. During the early adolescent years, twice as many 
of the early-maturing boys of this college level are involved in homosexual 
activities (Table 78). During subsequent age periods, the differences in 
incidence are not so great. Ultimately about 45 per cent of the early- 
adolescent boys of this college level have some homosexual experience, 
while less than 25 per cent of the late-adolescent males are ever involved. 
On the other hand, the frequencies with which homosexual contacts occur 
(Table 77) remain at about twice the height for these single males who 
first became adolescent, at least through the period between adolescence 
and 25. As a factor in the development of the homosexual, age of onset of 
adolescence (which probably means the metabohe drive of the individual) 
may prove to be more significant than the much discussed Oedipus relation 
of Freudian philosophy. 

Other Outlets. Apart from masturbation, heterosexual coitus, and the 
homosexual, the other possible sources of sexual outlet are utilized to 
about an equal degree by the unmarried males from different adolescent 
groups. A single instance of such conformance is shown in Table 79, 
which covers the data for nocturnal emissions. Similarly, there are no 
fundamental differences between the incidences and frequencies in regard 
to heterosexual petting, and in regard to that part of the heterosexual 



316 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


First Intercourse and Age at Adolescence 


Accumulative Incidence Data 
Educational Level 13-f 


AGE 

adolescent 
BY 11 

adolescent 
AT 12 

adolescent 
AT 13 

j adolescent 
j AT 14 

1 

ADOL FSCENT 
AT 15-1- 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

' ^*0 ^^ilh 
j Exper. 

8 

300 

0.0 

707 

0.0 

979 

0.0 

647 

0.0 

183 

1 0.0 

9 

300 

0.3 

707 

0.0 

979 

0.0 

647 

0.0 

183 

' 0,0 

10 

300 

0.3 

707 

0.0 

979 

0.0 

647 

0.0 

183 

0.0 

11 

300 

1.7 

707 

0.0 

979 

0.0 

647 

i 0.0 

183 

; 0.0 

12 

300 

3.3 

707 

2.4 

979 

0.0 

647 

i 0.0 

183 

0.0 

13 

300 

8.7 

707 

4.2 

979 

2.9 

647 

0.0 

183 

0.0 

14 

300 

13.0 

707 

7.5 

979 

5.0 

647 

5.0 

183 

0 0 

15 

300 

15,0 

707 

11.6 

979 

8.6 

647 

7.6 

183 

4.4 

16 

300 

23.0 

707 

19.0 

979 

13.6 

646 

12,8 

183 

9.3 

17 

300 

29.0 

707 

27.4 

978 

22.2 

645 

19.5 

183 

14.8 

18 

290 

37.2 

690 

35.8 

948 

28.7 

626 

28.8 

183 

21.9 

19 

275 

44.7 

646 

43.2 

886 

36.0 

590 

35.8 

176 

30.7 

20 

259 

53.7 

595 

50.1 

791 

43.9 

526 

42.4 

167 

37.1 

21 

236 

57.2 

499 

54.5 

676 

50.0 

470 

47.9 

151 

43.7 

22 

198 

65.1 

397 

61.5 

555 

58.4 

398 

53.8 

123 

52.0 

23 

176 

68.2 

329 

64.4 

462 

66.7 

327 

57.2 

102 

52.9 

24 

146 

74.7 

264 

72.7 

379 

71.2 

281 

62.6 

81 

58.0 

25 

126 

79.4 

229 

79.0 

334 

76.0 

239 1 

71.5 

73 

64.4 

26 

107 

87.9 

198 

83.8 

299 

79.6 

216 

78.2 

63 

66.7 

27 

86 

90.7 

169 

85.2 

270 

81.9 

198 

81.8 

50 

70.0 

28 

82 

91.5 

150 

85.3 

240 

85.4 

182 

86.3 

44 

70.5 

29 

70 

95.7 

139 

85.6 

216 

86.1 

169 

88.8 

39 

79.5 

30 

60 

95.0 

121 

87.6 

199 

89.4 

158 

91.1 

34 

82.4 

31 

56 

94.6 

107 

91.6 

181 

90.6 

153 ; 

92.2 



32 

51 

94.1 

97 

92.8 

166 

91.0 

146 ‘ 

92.5 



33 



89 

92.1 

148 

91.2 

131 1 

92.4 



34 



83 

92.8 

137 

92.7 

120 

94.2 



35 



76 

92.1 

126 

96.0 j 

115 

93.9 



36 



70 

94.3 

114 

95.6 

110 

94.5 



37 



64 

93.8 

101 

95.0 

99 

93.9 



38 



60 

93.3 

96 

92.7 

96 

95.8 



39 



57 

93.0 

85 

96.5 

90 

95.6 



40 





79 

96.2 

84 

1 95.2 




Table 75. Age of first intercourse, as affected by age at onset of adolescence 


Accumulative incidence data based on males of college level. 



PERCENT OF TOTAL POPULATION PERCENT OF TOTAL POPULATION PERCENT OF TOTAL POPULATION 


AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


317 



AGE 


Figures 92-94. Relation of age at onset of adolescence to accumulative incidence 
of masturbation, total intercourse, and homosexual outlet 

Based on lifetime histories of males of the college level. Each adolescent group main- 
tains Its relative position for 20 or more years. 






318 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUilAK MALE 


Marriage and Age at Adolescence 






Accumulative 

Educationai 

Incidence Data 

Level 13-f- 



age 

adolescent 

ADOLESCENT 

adolescent 

adolescent 

adolescent 


BY 

11 

AT 

12 

at 

13 

at 

14 

at 

15 + 


Cases 

% with 

Cases 

L% with 

Cases 

% with 

< 

i 

Cases ! 

% with 

i 

Cases 

% with 



Exper. 


1 Exper. 


Exper. 


Exper. 


; Exper. 

I 

8 

307 

0.0 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

651 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

9 

307 

0.0 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

651 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

10 

307 

0.0 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

651 

i 0.0 

187 

0.0 

11 

307 

0.0 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

651 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

12 

307 

0.0 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

651 

1 0.0 

187 

0.0 

13 

307 

0.0 

724 

0.0 i 

986 j 

0.0 

651 

i 0.0 

187 

0 0 

14 

307 

0.0 

724 

0.0 

986 i 

0.0 

651 

i 0.0 

187 

0.0 

15 

307 

0.0 

724 

0.0 

986 i 

0.0 

651 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

16 

307 

0,3 

724 

0.1 

986 

0.0 

651 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

17 

307 

0.3 

724 

0.1 

985 

0.2 

650 

0.0 

187 1 

0.0 

18 

297 

0.7 

706 

0.4 

957 

0.4 

631 

0.0 

187 1 

0.0 

19 

282 

2.5 

660 

1.4 

897 

1.2 

594 

0.7 

180 i 

0.0 

20 

266 

3.4 

609 

3.1 

802 

2.9 

530 

2.8 

170 i 

0.0 

21 

243 

5.3 

510 

6.3 

689 

4.9 

473 

5.3 

154 ! 

0.6 

22 

205 

9.8 

407 

12.0 

568 

11.1 

402 

10.0 

126 1 

6,3 

23 

183 

16.9 

339 

19.8 

474 

16.5 

332 

1 14.8 

I 105 : 

i 8.6 

24 

152 

28.3 

274 

32.5 

391 

23.3 

288 

1 20.1 

1 84 : 

i 15.5 

25 

131 

37.4 

238 

1 45.0 

i 345 

29.9 

246 

! 29.7 

1 77 1 

1 23.4 

26 

112 

48.2 

206 

52.4 

310 

42.3 

224 

! 40.6 

i 67 : 

: 26.9 

27 

90 

55.6 

174 

1 60.3 

278 

51.8 

205 

49.3 

! 54 

37.0 

28 

86 

59.3 

155 

63.2 

248 

60.1 

188 

55.3 



29 

74 

67.6 

144 

67.4 

223 

64.6 

174 

59.2 



30 

64 

68.8 

125 

73.6 

206 

71.4 

162 

67.3 



31 

60 

73.3 

110 

83.6 

188 

73.9 

156 

71.2 



32 

55 

76.4 

100 

84.0 

171 

77.2 

149 

73.2 



33 

53 

77.4 

91 

85.7 

154 

78.6 

135 

75.6 



34 



85 

87.1 

; 142 

81.0 

124 

79.8 



35 



77 

87.0 

131 

83.2 

119 

80.7 



36 



70 

88.6 

119 

84.9 

114 

82.5 



37 



64 

87.5 

105 

83.8 

103 

80.6 



38 



1 60 

86.7 

' 100 

83.0 

100 

83.0 



39 



' 57 

86.0 

89 

83.1 

93 

83.9 



40 



50 

88.0 

83 

! 85.5 

87 

86.2 















Table 76. Age at marriage, as aflfected by age at onset of adolescence 


Accumulative incidence data based on males of college level. 



AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


319 


intercourse which is had with prostitutes. There is some suggestion in the 
data that the farm boys who became adolescent first have more animal 
intercourse, but the number of active cases is too small to allow dependable 
calculations. 

For the married males, the rates of total outlet among the early-adoles- 
cent individuals are definitely higher. There is some indication that most 
of this comes from marital intercourse, but the necessity for a six-way 
breakdown of the population before correlations can be run puts such a 
strain on the relatively smaller samples now available for married males, 
that further analyses cannot be made at this time. 



Figure 95. Relation of age at onset of adolescence to age at marriage 


Based on males of the college level. A social phenomenon is correlated with the 
factors which control onset of adolescence. 


AGING VERSUS EARLY ACTIVITY 

It would be of considerable interest to know whether an early onset of 
sexual activity and a lifetime of higher frequencies among the early- 
adolescent males show any correlation with the age at which individuals 
become sexually unresponsive or impotent, and at which they cease sexual 
activity altogetW. 

Some information on these points may be had from Table 71 where the 
record for married males is shown up to the age of 50. At that time, 100 
per cent of the early-adolescent males are still sexually active, and their 
frequencies are still 20 per cent higher than the frequencies of the later- 
maturing males. Nearly forty years of maximum activity have not yet 
worn them out physically, physiologically, or psychologically. On the other 
hand, some of the males (not many) who were late adolescent and who 



Homosexual Outlets and Age at Adolescence 




Frequencies Among Single Males, by Educational Level 






INDICES, 


age at 

adol. 

CASES 

total population 

total 

popULxnoNS 

active pope la HON 



Mean 

Frequency 

: Me- 
dian 
Freq. 

% of 
Total 
Outlet 

By 

Means 

(> 

Incid. Mean 

„ dian 

o Frequency 


Educ. level 0-8 Activity between adoL-15 


8-12 

131 

0.44 

=b0.08 

0.00 

8.8 

367 

46 

.6 

0, 

.94 

:i= 

0. 

,15 

0.40 

13 

180 

0.27 

±0.06 

0.00 

8.6 

225 

25 

.0 

1, 

.09 


0. 

,20 

0.50 

14 

270 

0.22 

=±=0.06 

0.00 

9.7 

183 

18 

.9 

1 , 

.19 

cb 

0. 

.28 

0.38 

15+ 

115 

0.12 

=i=0.04 

0.00 

i 

6.0 

100 

13 

.9, 

0 

85 

=b 

0, 

.24 

0.56 



Educ. level 

. 0-8 

Activity between 

16- 

■20 







8-12 1 

114 

0.52 

=b0.12 

0.00 

10.0 

400 

45, 

.6 

1 

14 

=L 

0. 

22 

0.40 

13 

165 

0.31 

=i=0.08 

; 0.00 

10.1 

238 

28, 

.5 

1 

,09 

=b 

0. 

25 

0.39 

14 

266 

0.15 

=i=0.04 

0.00 

6.0 

115 I 

oo 

.6, 

0 

,68 

zb 

0. 

13 , 

0.30 

15+ 

154 

0.13 

=^0.03 

0.00 

5.9 

100 

22, 

n , 

0 

,58 

=b 

0. 

10: 

,0.43 



Educ. level 9-12 

Activity between adol - 

i: 

) 





8-12 

170 

0.54 

= l 0.11 

0.00 

13.4 

599 

47 

1 

1 

'TT 

=b 

0. 

21 

I0.45 

13 

197 

0.26 

=i=0.06 

0.00 

7.5 

288 1 

31, 

0 

0 

85 

zfc 

0. 

19 

O.IO 

14 

213 

0.17 

=±=0.03 

0.00 

6.9 

1.30 i 

26 

8 

0 

64 

zb 

0 

U) 

;0.27 

15+ 

42 

0.09 

=fc0.04 

0.00 

3.8 

100 

21 

4j 

0, 

,44 


0 

12 

;o.5o 



Educ. Icv'el 

i 9-12 

Activity between 

, 16- 

20 







8-12 

162 

0.72 

±0.14 

0.03 

18.3 

480 

54, 

9 

1. 

,31' 


0. 

24 

0.50 

13 

196 

0.31 

±0.06 

0.00 

8.7 

207 

38, 

8 ; 

0. 

,79 

=L 

0. 

13 

,0.17 

14 

207 

0.31 

±0.06 

0.00 

11.5 

207 

37, 

T j 

0. 

83 

=b 

0. 

13 ; 

10.34 

15+ 

58 

0.15 

±0.05 

0.00 

4.8 

100 

.34, 

,"i 

0. 

,43 

=b 

0. 

‘U 

I0.I4 

i 


Educ. level 13-E Activity betw'cen adol.- 15 


8-12 

893 

0.11 ±0.02 

0.00 

3.2 

220 

27.9 

0.38 ± 0.05 

0.08 

13 

896 

0.09 ±0.01 

0.00 

3.3 

180 

20 9 ! 

0.43 ±0.05 

10.09 

14 

600 

0.07 ±0.02 

0.00 

3.3 

140 

14. 0: 

0.50 ±0.12 

!0.09 

15+ 

114 

0.05 ±0.02 

0.00 

2.8 

100 

14.0 

0.36 =0.14 

jo. 23 


Educ. level 13+ Activity between 16-20 


8-12 

893 

0.08 

rb 

0.01 

0.00 

2.5 

200 

16.6 

0.46 

=b 

0.07 

;o.o8 

13 

896 

0.06 

=b 

O.Ol 

0.00 

2.4 

150 

15.5 

0.39 


0 06 

,0.08 

14 

600 

0.07 

=b 

0.02 

0.00 

3,0 

175 

15.3 

0.44 

b: 

0.12 

|0.09 

15+ 

170 

0.04 

=b 

0.02 

0.00 

1.7 

100 

15.9 

0.24 

=b 

0.09 

0.08 



Educ. level 13 + 

Activity between 

21-25 




8-12 

561 

0.11 

=b 

0.02 

0.00 

4.0 

183 

9.3 

1.18 

=b 

0.19 

0.48 

13 

566 

0.08 

d= 

0.02 

0.00 

3.4 

133 

9,0 

0.84 

=b 

0.20 

0.26 

14 

393 

0.09 

=b 

0.03 

0.00 

4.4 

150 

9.7 

0.97 

=b 

0.31 

0.23 

15+ 

136 

0.06 

=b 

0.03 

0.00 

2.5 

100 

8.8 

0.63 

=b 

0.28 

0.30 



Educ. level 13+ 

Activity between 

26-30 




8-12 

1 176 

0.27 

=b 

0.06 

0.00 

9.8 

159 

17.6 

1.52 

=b 

0.27 

1.17 

13 

179 

0.21 

=b 

0.06 

0.00 

9.0 

123 

17.3 

1.21 

=b 

0.26 

0.63 

14 

152 

0.17 

=b 

0,05 

0,00 

7.9 

100 

16.4 

1.01 

=b 

0.26 

0.37 


Table 77. Frequency of homosexual outlet as related to age at onset of 

adolescence 


320 



AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


321 


Homosexual Outlets and Age at Adolescence 


Accumulative Incidence Data 
Educational Level 13+ 


AGE 

adolescent 

BY 11 

adolescent 
AT 12 

adolescent 
AT 13 

adolescent 
AT 14 

adolescent 
AT 15 + 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

307 

0.3 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

651 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

9 

307 

0.7 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

651 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

10 

307 

4.2 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

651 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

11 

307 

16.3 

724 

0.0 

986 

0.0 

651 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

12 

307 

20.5 

724 

15.2 

986 

0.0 i 

651 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

13 

307 

23.8 

724 

19.9 

986 

11.4 

651 

0.0 

187 

0.0 

14 

307 

29.3 

724 

23.8 

986 

18.1 

651 

11.2 

187 

0.0 

15 

307 

30.6 

724 

27.1 

986 

20.5 

651 

14.3 

187 

9.6 

16 

307 

31.9 

724 

28.6 

986 

22.2 

651 

16.9 

187 

12.3 

17 

307 

32.2 

724 

29.6 

985 

23.4 

650 

18.2 

187 

13.9 

18 

297 

34.0 

706 

30.6 

957 

24.7 

631 

20.1 

187 

15.5 

19 

282 

35.5 

660 

32.7 

897 

25.4 

594 

20.7 

180 

16.7 

20 

266 

37.6 

609 

33,5 

802 

25.6 

530 

21.9 

170 

18.2 

21 

243 

37,4 

510 

35.1 

689 

27.0 

473 

22.2 

154 

19.5 

22 

205 

39.0 

407 

36.4 

568 

28.5 

402 

22.9 

126 

21.4 

23 

183 

42.6 

339 

37.5 

474 

29.5 

332 

24.4 

105 

22.9 

24 

152 

43.4 

274 

38.7 

391 

29.9 

288 

25.3 

84 

22.6 

25 

131 

45.8 

236 

39.4 

345 

29.9 

246 

27.2 

77 

22.1 

26 

112 

41.1 

206 

40.8 

310 

30.0 

224 

28.6 

67 

20.9 

27 

90 

42.2 

174 

40.8 

278 

31.7 

205 

29.3 

54 

22.2 

28 

86 

41.9 

155 

38,7 

248 

33.5 

188 

29.3 



29 

74 

44.6 

144 

38.9 

223 

32.7 

174 

28.7 



30 

64 

43.8 

125 

38,4 

206 

33.5 

162 

I 29.0 



31 

60 

46.7 

no 

37.3 

188 

34.0 

156 

29.5 



32 

55 

43.6 1 

100 

37.0 

171 

32.2 

149 

28.9 



33 

53 

43.4 

91 

38.5 

154 

33.8 

135 

29.6 



34 



85 

40 0 

142 

33.8 

124 

30.6 



35 



77 

39.0 

131 

32,1 

119 

30.3 



36 



70 

40.0 

119 

31.9 

114 

28.9 



37 



64 

42.2 

105 

30.5 

103 

28.2 



38 



60 

43.3 

100 

30.0 

100 

28.0 



39 



57 

42.1 

89 

31.5 

93 

28.0 



40 



50 

42.0 

83 

28.9 

87 

27.6 




Table 78. Homosexual outlet as alBfected by age at onset of adolescence 
Accumulative incidence data based on males of the college level. 



322 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Nocturnal Emissions and Age at Adolescence 




Frequencies Among Single Males, by Educaeional Level 






INDICES, 




total population 

TOTAL 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

AGE AT 

cases 




POPUL \TIONS 


ADOL. 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

”/o of 
Total 
Outlet 

dians 

/o j o 

J o 

i u, 

Incid. Mean ^ 

, Frequency ^ 

' . 1 ^ ticq. 



Educ. level 0-8 Activity between adoI.-15 


8-12 

131 

0.04 ±0.01 

0.00 

0.9 

100 

— 

30.5,0.14 ±0.04 0.07 

13 

180 

0.07 ±0.02 

0.00 

2.3 

175 

— 

37.2.0.20 ± 0.04 0,08 

14 

270 

0.05 ±0.01 

0.00 

2.0 

125 

— 

20.4:0.23 ± 0.06 0.07 

15-i- 

115 

0.06 ±0.02 

0.00 

3.0 

150 

— 

14.8 0.40 ±0.13 0.26 



Educ. level 0-8 

Activity between 

16-20 

8-12 

114 

0.15 ±0.03 

0.03 

2,9 

115 

300 

57.0 0.26 ±0.05 0.10 

13 

165 

0.17 ±0.03 

0.05 

5.5 

131 

500 

66.7 0.25 ±0.04 0.10 

14 

266 

0.13 ±0.02 

0.01 

5.1 

100 

100 

51.5 0.26 =i: 0.04 0.08 

15+ ' 

154 

0.20 ±0.04 

0.03 

8.9 

154 

300 

56.5 0.35 =i= 0.06 0.13 



Educ. level 9-12 

Activity between adol.-I5 

8-12 

170 

0.23 ±0.06 

0.00 

5.7 

288 

— 

46.5'0.49 ±0.n 0.12 

13 

197 

0.15 ±0.03 

0.00 

4.2 

188 



44.7 O..33 ± 0,05 ,0.16 

14 

213 

0.10 ±0.03 

0.00 

4.2 

125 

— 

33.8 0.30 ±0.07 0.09 

15+ 

42 

0.08 ±0.03 

0.00 

3.3 

100 

— 

23.8,0.35 ±0.08 0.33 


Educ. level 9-12 Activity between 16-20 


8-12 

162 

0.25 ±0.04 

0.07 

6.5 

156 

140 

74.7 

0.34 ±0.05 

0.11 

13 

196 

0.22 ±0.03 

0.07 

6.3 

137 

140 

73.0i0.30±0.04 

0.16 

14 

207 

0.22 ±0,03 

0.05 

8.1 

137 

100 

68.6 

0.31 ±0.05 

0.10 

15+ 

58 

0.16 ±0.03 

0.05 

5.1 

100 

100 

62.1 

0.25 ±0.04 

0.18 



Educ. level 13+ 

Activity between adol. 

-15 


8-12 

893 

0.37 ±0.03 

0.10 

11.4 

154 

163 

73.1 

0.51 ±0.04 

0.25 

13 

896 

0.35 ±0.02 

0.13 

13.0 

146 

125 

74.2 

0.48 ±0.03 

0.28 

14 

600 

0.28 ±0.02 

0.08 

13.0 

117 

100 

61.2 

0.45 ±0.03 

0.29 

15+ 

114 

0.24 ±0,06 

0,00 

13.2 

100 

— 

40.4 

0.60 ±0.14 

0.34 



Educ. level 13+ 

Activity between 16-20 


8-12 

893 

0.41 ±0.02 

0.22 

13.6 

105 

100 

88.9 

0.46 ±0.02 

0.27 

13 

896 

0.43 ±0.02 

0.27 

17.0 

110 

123 

93.4 

0.46 ±0.02 

0.30 

14 

600 

0.39 ±0.02 

0.24 

17.5 

100 

109 

90.7 

0.44 ±0.03 

0.28 

15+ 

170 

0.47 ±0.06 

0.27 

21.2 

122 

123 

91.8 

0.52 ±0.06 

0.30 



Educ. level 13+ 

Activity between 21-25 


8-12 

561 

0.37 ±0.03 

0,19 

13.5 

100 

100 

84.7 

0.44 ±0,03 

0.27 

13 

566 

0.40 ±0.03 

0.23 

17,7 

108 

121 

89.0 

0.45 ±0.03 

0.28 

14 

393 

0.37 ±0.03 

0.23 

17.2 

100 

121 

91.1 

0.40 ±0.03 

0.27 

15+ 

136 

0.46 ±0.06 

0.26 

21.0 

124 

137 

86.8 

0.53 ±0.07 

0.31 


Table 79. Lack of correlation between nocturnal emissions and age at onset of 

adolescence 





AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


323 


have had five years less of sexual activity, are beginning to drop completely 
out of the picture; and the rates of this group are definitely lower in these 
older age periods. 

It is unfortunate that the number of histories now on hand from still 
older males is too small to allow further calculations on these points. 
It has, however, been possible to calculate correlations between the age of 
onset of adolescence and the age of onset of impotence for a small group 
of 69 older males. For these cases, the coefficient of correlation proves to 
be 0.30. If the results can be trusted on a sample of this size, the low coejffi- 
cient indicates that there is in actuality no significant correlation. In other 
words, the fact that an individual has started sexual activity in early life 
and has had frequent activity throughout a long period of years is not 
necessarily responsible for the onset of impotence in his old age. Impotence 
is as likely to occur at the same age among those males who did not start 
activity until late and whose rates of sexual activity were always low. The 
ready assumption which is made in some of the medical literature that 
impotence is the product of sexual excess, is not justified by such data as 
are now available. Impotence is clearly the product of a great diversity of 
physical, physiologic, and psychologic factors, and in each individual case 
a multiplicity of factors is likely to be involved. 

It will be recalled (Chapter 7) that impotence is in actuality a relatively 
rare phenomenon. The clinicians, especially the urologists and endocrin- 
ologists, see so many individuals who are badly upset by impotence that 
they may find it difficult to believe that the incidence of the phenomenon is 
as low as we find it in the population at large; but again it should be 
pointed out that a clinic is no place from which to get incidence data. 

Impotence in a male under 55 years of age is almost always the product 
of psychologic conflict, except in those exceedingly few cases where there 
has been mechanical injury of the genitalia or of the portions of the central 
nervous system which control erection, or in those similarly few cases 
where venereal or other disease has interfered with nervous functions. 
There is even some evidence that much of the impotence which is seen in 
old age is psychologic in its origin. In a larger number of cases than has 
ordinarily been reahzed, there are psychologic problems involving sex 
which may not develop until the later years of an individuaFs marriage, 
either in connection with his marital intercourse, or in connection with 
other sexual activities which the male begins in his more advanced years. 
Psychologically, impotence is also predicated among older persons because 
they so often expect it; and the psychologic fatigue which follows long 
years of sexual experience is a pronhnent factor in the development of 
incapacity in old age. It will be recalled (Chapter 7) that only 27 per cent of 
the male population becomes impotent by 70 years of age, and that much 
older histories would be needed to secure any large number of cases for 



PERCENT PERCENT PERCENT 


324 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 






Figure 96. Comparisons of individual variation among early-adolescent and 

late-adolescent males 

Based on single males. Solid lines show frequency distributions for males who became 
adolescent by 12. Broken lines and shaded areas show frequency distributions for males 
who did not become adolescent before 15. 



AGE OF ADOLESCENCE AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


325 


a study of the relation of impotence to the age of onset of adolescence. 
Certain it is that among persons who become impotent by 70, there are 
histories of males who became adolescent at each and every age ; but there 
are also histories of males who are still active after sixty or more years of 
sexual activities which were maintained at a maximum rate from the time 
they first turned adolescent at 10 or 11. 

CONCLUSIONS 

In fine, the data add up as follows : 

1. The males who are first adolescent begin their sexual activity almost 
immediately and maintain higher frequencies in sexual activity for a 
matter of at least 35 or 40 years. 

2. The factors which contribute to this early adolescence apparently 
continue to operate for at least these 35 or 40 years. 

3. Exercise of the sexual capacities does not seem to impair those capac- 
ities, at least as they are exercised by most of the persons who belong in 
the highest-rating segment of the population. While it is theoretically 
conceivable that very high rates of activity might contribute to physical 
impairment, or indirectly to diseased conditions, or to other difficulties 
in certain cases, the actual record includes exceedingly few high-rating 
males whose activities have had such an outcome. 

4. Those individuals who become adolescent late, however, more 
often delay the start of their sexual activities and have the minimum fre- 
quencies of activity, both in their early years and throughout the remainder 
of their lives. If any of these individuals have deliberately chosen low 
frequencies in order to conserve their energies for later use, they appear 
never to have found the sufficient justification for such a use at any later 
time. It is probable that most of these low rating individuals never were 
capable of higher rates and never could have increased their rates to match 
those of the more active segments of the population. 

5. In general, the boys who were first mature are the ones who most 
often turn to masturbation and, interestingly enough, to pre-marital socio- 
sexual contacts as well. They engage in both heterosexual and homosexual 
relations more frequently than the boys who are last in maturing. 

There is some reason for thinking that these early-adolescent males are 
more often the more alert, energetic, vivacious, spontaneous, physically 
active, socially extrovert, and/or aggressive individuals in the population. 
Actually, 53 per cent of the early-adolescent boys are so described on their 
histories, while only 33 per cent of the late-adolescent boys received such 
personality ratings. Conversely, 54 per cent of the males who were last- 
adolescent were described as slow, quiet, mild in manner, without force, 



326 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


reserved, timid, taciturn, introvert, and/or socially inept, while only 31 
per cent of the early-adolescent boys fell under such headings. There are, 
of course, some individuals who do not fall clearly into either of these 
classifications. Prior to analyzing these data for the present chapter, we 
had no indication that we would find this sort of correlation and, conse- 
quently, all of the personality notations on the original histories were 
made without regard for the ages at which adolescence had occurred. 

There is, of course, much individual variation on all of these matters, 
and there is no invariable correlation betw'een personalities and rates of 
sexual activity. There are some very energetic and socially extrovert indi- 
viduals who rate low in their sexual frequencies, and there are quiet and 
even timid individuals w^ho have considerable socio-sexual activity. 
Behavior is always the product of a multiplicity of factors, no one of which 
can be identified as the exclusive or predominant agent in more than some 
small portion of the cases which one studies. 

There is evidence that the late-maturing males have more limited sexual 
capacities which would be badly strained if, through any circumstance, 
they tried to raise their rales to the levels maintained by the sexually more 
capable persons. If further studies show that some physiologic quality, 
such as metabolic rate, works together with or through the hormones to 
determine the time of onset of adolescence, it may become a matter of 
clinical importance to exercise some control over that event. If this were 
done, would the subsequent sexual performance then be affected? Parents 
and clinicians may properly be concerned with such questions. 



Chapter 10 

SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 

The sexual behavior of the human animal is the outcome of its morpho- 
logic and physiologic organization, of the conditioning which its experi- 
ence has brought it, and of all the forces which exist in its living and non- 
hving environment. In terms of academic disciplines, there are biologic, 
psychologic, and sociologic factors involved; but all of these operate 
simultaneously, and the end product is a single, unified phenomenon which 
is not merely biologic, psychologic, or sociologic in nature. Nevertheless, 
the importance of each group of factors can never be ignored. 

Without its physical body and its physiologic capacities, there would be 
no animal to act. The individual’s sexual behavior is, to a degree, pre- 
destined by its morphologic structure, its metabolic capacities, its hor- 
mones, and all of the other characters which it has inherited or which have 
been built into it by the physical environment in which it has developed. 
Two of the most important of these distinctively biologic forces, age and 
the age at onset of adolescence, have been examined in the earlier chapters 
of the present volume. 

But through all of the previous chapters, constant consideration has 
been given to the significance of the psychologic factors which affect sexual 
behavior, and it should be apparent by now that the experience of the 
individual, the satisfactory or unsatisfactory nature of that experience, the 
conformance or non-conformance of that experience with the individual’s 
personality, attitudes, and rational thinking, and a great variety of other 
factors make the psychologic bases of behavior even more important than 
the biologic heritage and acquirements. 

It is evident, however, that psychologic processes depend, to a consider- 
able degree, upon the way in which external forces impinge upon the 
animal. For a creature with as highly organized a central nervous system 
as is found in the human animal, the most important external force is the 
social environment in which it lives. In the human species, the environ- 
ment consists of one’s family, his close friends, his neighbors, his business 
associates, and his mere acquaintances. It also includes the thousands of 
other persons whom he has never seen but whose attitudes, habits, ex- 
pressed opinions, and overt activities constitute the culture in which he 
moves and fives. These are the social forces which contribute to the indi- 
vidual’s behavior. There is, of course, no part of the individual himself 
which is social in nature, in quite the way that morphologic, physiologic, 

327 



328 


SEXUAL BERWIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


or psychologic capacities may be identified and localized in an organism. 
Occasionally social forces provide physical restraints on individuals, or 
facilitate their physical activities: but more often they operate only as they 
affect the individual psychologically. 


EDUC. 

LEVEL 

ATTAINED 

Occupational Class 

CASES 

IN 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

2 

DAY 

LABOR 

3 

SEMI- 

SKILLED 

LABOR 

4 

SKILLED 

LABOR 

5 

LOWER 

WHITE 

COLLAR 

6 

UPPER 

\\ HITE 

COLLAR 

7 

PRO- 

FES- 

SIONAL 

8 

BU.SI- 

NFSS 

EXECU- 

TIVE 


% 

% 

% 

0/ 


% 

% 


0 

4.6 

1.1 

0.7 

0.2 




26 

1 

1.3 

0.4 

0.0 

0.0 




7 

2 

3.8 

1.1 

0,7 

0.2 




23 

3 

4.1 

3.2 

2.9 

0.2 




39 

4 

7.6 

4.3 

3.6 





61 

5 

7.6 

6.1 

1.4 

0,2 




67 

6 

9.6 

6.8 

5.8 

1.8 




92 

7 

15.2 

9.3 

12.9 

2.0 




139 

8 

22.9 

26.4 

23.7 

6.8 

1.6 



309 

9 

8.6 

11.1 

3.6 

4.1 

0.4 



121 

10 

8.6 

13.7 

9.4 

6.5 

1.0 



158 

11 

2.8 

6.3 

5.8 

6.5 

1.6 



90 

12 

3.3 

8.6 

22.3 

27.6 

5.0 


6.3 

239 

13 


0.7 i 

3.6 

12.0 

2.7 



76 

14 


0.7 

0.7 

11.0 

5.8 



84 

15 


0.2 

0.7 

6.3 

4.5 

0,3 


55 

16 



2.2 

8.3 

25,3 

0.9 

18.8 1 

181 

17 




3.4 

22.7 

2.0 

0,0 1 

145 

18 




2.0 

21.9 

6.6 

12.5 1 

167 

19 




0.7 

5.2 

6.1 

18.8 i 

73 

20+ 




0.2 

1 2.3 

! 

84.1 

43.6 : 

i 567 

Total % 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 1 

2719 

Cases in oc- 
cup. class 

394 

559 

139 

444 

516 

651 ; 

i 

16 1 

! i 

2719 

Mean educ. 

Median educ. 

6.23 

6.78 

7.59 

7.69 

8.57 

7.95 

11.84 

11.80 

15.68 

16.11 

19.20 

19.41 

17.81 

18.69 



Table 80. Relation between educational level and occupational class of subjects 

in present sample 


Based on those males in the present sample who have finished their educational careers. 

The present chapter and the three chapters which follow are concerned 
with the relation of the individuaFs pattern of sexual behavior to patterns 
which are followed by other persons in the same social group — in the group 
in which the individual is raised, or into which he moves and establishes 
himself in the course of his lifetime. 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


329 


DEFINING SOCIAL LEVELS 

The data now available show that patterns of sexual behavior may be 
strikingly different for the different social levels that exist in the same city 
or town, and sometimes in immediately adjacent sections of a single com- 
munity. The data show that divergencies in the sexual patterns of such 
social groups may be as great as those which anthropologists have found 
between the sexual patterns of different racial groups in remote parts of the 
world. There is no American pattern of sexual behavior, but scores of 
patterns, each of which is confined to a particular segment of our society. 
Within each segment there are attitudes on sex and patterns of overt 
activity which are followed by a high proportion of the individuals in that 
group; and an understanding of the sexual mores of the American people 
as a whole is possible only through an understanding of the sexual patterns 
of all of the constituent groups. 

These social levels are, admittedly, intangible divisions of the popula- 
tion which are difficult to define; but they are recognized by everyone as 
real and significant factors in the life of a community. In the present study, 
the social level of each subject has been measured by three criteria: 1. The 
educational level, in years, which the individual has reached by the time he 
terminates his formal education (Chapter 3). 2. The occupational class to 
which the individual belongs (as such classes have been defined in Chapter 
3). 3. The occupational class of the individual’s parents at the time that 
he lived in the parental home. 

There are, of course, certain correlations among these three criteria. 
The educational level ultimately attained determines, to some degree, the 
occupation which an individual follows. The nature of the correlation is 
shown in Table 80, where it will be observed that certain educational levels 
send people into several of the occupational classes, while other educational 
levels (e.g., the one which includes those who have done graduate work in 
a university) send nearly all of their members into a single occupational 
class. It is understandable, therefore, that analyses of sexual behavior 
made on the basis of ultimate educational level give results which are close 
to those obtained by the use of a system of occupational classes. 

The ultimate educational level attained by an individual shows a limited 
correlation with intelligence quotients (Lorge 1942). The correlations have 
been shown to run about 0.66, which may mean that there is some trend 
for the more intelhgent students to continue in school. It also indicates, 
however, that there are some perfectly intelligent individuals who stop 
school long before they have reached the limits of their capacities ; and that 
there are some less intelhgent individuals who, by dint of work or fortuitous 
circumstance, manage to get further along in school than their capacities 
would predicate. Since there may be some correlation between mental 



330 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


capacity and the nature of the occupation which an individual chooses, 
here is another reason for one’s educational level correlating with his 
occupational class. 

Educational Level as a Criterion. The educational level attained by an 
individual by the time he terminates his schooling has proved to be the 
simplest and the best-defined means for recognizing social levels (see 
Chapter 3 for details of the way in which this criterion has been used). 
Social level is not necessarily controlled by the amount of schooling that 
an individual has had, but the amount of schooling does provide a measure 
of more basic factors which determine one’s social level. 

Each level has its own attitudes toward education and, consequently, a 
high proportion of the persons in any level go to about the same point in 
school. One group allows its children to terminate their schooling at the 
eighth grade, or as soon thereafter as the law allows ; and in that group 
there is a general acceptance of the idea that it is a waste of time to send 
children further along in school when they might be earning wages and 
contributing to the family income. There is no community action which 
formalizes these things and some individuals in the community may dis- 
agree with the general attitude; but by and large the children hear the 
group opinion so often expressed that they come to accept it and look for- 
ward to the time when they will be allowed to quit school. The individ- 
uals in another social level believe that their children should go part way, 
or perhaps fully, through high school. Going to college is the expected and 
more or less inevitable thing for the children of other social groups. 

Persons who depart from the educational trends of their particular level 
do so against the community opinion and must be ready to defend them- 
selves for their independent action. This is as true of the professor’s son 
who decides to go to work at the end of high school as it is of the lower 
level boy who strikes out for a college education. The boy or girl who 
departs from the custom is quickly made aware of the fact that he has done 
something as unusual as wearing the wrong kind of clothing to a social 
event, or using his table silver in a fashion which is recognized as not good 
manners in that group. There are no penalties attached to departures from 
the custom, except those of being made to feel different from the com- 
munity of which one has previously considered himself a part. Such penal- 
ties, however, may control behavior as effectively as though they were 
physical restraints. 

During the past thirty or forty years, there has been a considerable 
departure of younger generations from the educational levels attained by 
their parents (Table 106); but almost always this has been in the direction 
of an increase in the amount of education which the younger persons re- 
ceive. The idea of a boy or girl being satisfied with less education than his 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 331 

parents had is so abhorrent as to be rarely accepted, and most people are 
startled when they find an individual case of such regression. 

Educational level is a convenient criterion for statistical use because it 
provides a well-defined, simple figure which is discrete and does not vary 
in the individual’s lifetime, after he has once finished his schooling. 
Educational level cannot be used for studying the histories of persons who 
are still in school, since there is no certainty how far they will go before 
they finally terminate their education. Educational level is not a satisfactory 
basis for analysis when the individual changes his social level in the course 
of his life. 

Occupational Class as a Criterion. It has been pointed out (Chapter 3) 
that a modification of the Chapin and Lloyd Warner schemes of occupa- 
tional classes (Chapin 1933, HoUingshead 1939, Warner and Lunt 1941, 
1942, Warner and Srole 1945) is the basis for the analyses made in the 
present study. In brief, the following classes are recognized: 

0. Dependents 

1. Underworld 

2. Day labor 

3. Semi-skilled labor 

4. Skilled labor 

5. Lower white collar group 

6. Upper white collar group 

7. Professional group 

8. Business executive group 

9. Extremely wealthy group 

Occupational classes are more poorly defined than educational levels. 
Whether an individual belongs in one occupational group or the next not 
infrequently calls for a judgment in which equally skilled investigators 
might disagree, although experience in the present research indicates that 
the judgments are not often more than one occupational class apart. 
Whether a person is a laborer or a semi-skilled workman, whether he is a 
semi-skilled or a skilled workman, is not always possible to say; but in 
most cases it is possible to make a definite classification. Labor unions 
often define the occupational qualities of their members. Whether a person 
is a mechanic or a white collar worker is rarely in dispute; but whether the 
white collar worker belongs to class 5 (the lower white collar group) or 
class 6 (the upper white collar group), is sometimes more difficult to say. 
This makes occupational class less precise than educational level for 
measuring social status. 

On the other hand, classifications by occupation probably show a closer 
correlation with the intangible realities of social organization, since this 
classification is designed to express the social prestige of the work with 



332 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN NULE 


which the individual is occupied. The use of occupational class provides 
the best opportunity, and the only opportunity we have had, to take into 
account the migrations of an individual from one social level to another 
within his lifetime; and all of the data given in the next chapter on the 
relation of such migration to changes in patterns of sexual behavior have 
been derived from this source. With younger persons who are still at home, 
it will be recalled that their occupational class is derived from that of their 
parents (their “ascribed status” as some anthropologists have put it). 
Younger individuals who are just beginning to establish themselves away 
from their parents’ home are often involved in more menial occupations, 
and sometimes in occupations totally different from those which they will 
ultimately work into (the latter is “the achieved status” in the anthro- 
pological terminology) ; and in this case, occupational class is not a good 
means of measuring social level 

In this and the next chapter, references to occupational class are usually 
made as double entries which include the parental class in which the sub- 
ject originated, and the ultimate class into which the subject independently 
migrated. 

Realities of Social Levels. If there were invariable correlations between 
education, occupation, and the social organization of our society, “social 
levels” would be recognized as realities which could easily be delimited. 
That there is no invariable relation means that such levels are difficult to 
define; but that does not prove that they are not realities. Quite on the 
contrary, each child soon becomes aware of the social classification to 
which he belongs, and learns the boundaries of the group wdthin which he 
is allowed to move. Each adult lives and moves and does his thinking, to 
a considerable degree, in accord with the movements and the thinking of 
other persons who have about the same education and who usually belong 
to the same occupational class. While there are no sharp boundaries to 
social levels, there are obstacles to the crossing of those boundaries. 

Social levels are hierarchies which are not supposed to exist in a demo- 
cratic society, and many people would, therefore, deny their existence. In 
this country we make it a point that there should be no physical barriers 
nor legal codes which forbid people to move with almost any social group. 
But while there are, admittedly, a few persons who do move between 
groups, most persons do not in actuality move freely with those who belong 
to other levels. Each group recognizes its unity, and its distinction from 
every other group. 

In their occupations or professional activities, persons of different social 
levels may have a certain amount of daily contact, but their close friends 
and companions are more hkely to come from their own groups. The white 
collar executive and the office force may work only a few feet away from the 
factory laborers and mechanics, but they do not really work with them; 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


333 


and in their recreations, after hours, the two groups rarely intermingle. 
Persons in the one group do not invite persons from the other group to 
their homes for dinner, or for an evening of conversation, games, or other 
activities. One’s companions in a card game or around a fireplace are a 
better test of one’s social position than are one’s business contacts, or 
even one’s verbalization of his social philosophy. 

Within the white collar groups, for instance, there are several levels of 
social organization. Store clerks and office staffs do not move freely with 
the business executive groups, outside of their business relations. Persons 
in professional groups have few intimates among any but the better busi- 
ness and professional men. Doctors may serve persons on both sides of the 
tracks, but in off hours they visit and find their recreations with other 
doctors, with some business men, or with college professors. The profes- 
sional group is not particularly at home with financially successful business 
men, nor with persons from the Social Register and the top social strata, 
unless the professional persons themselves happen to have inherited such 
financial or social backgrounds. These social stratifications are very real, 
even though they are difficult to define. 

Social levels are not necessarily determined by the economic status of 
an individual. School teachers belong to a white collar class which is 
generally looked up to by working classes although the working classes 
may have considerably higher incomes than school teachers ever will have. 
The fact that the janitor in the school may earn more than the teacher in 
the same building does not admit him to the social activities of the teacher’s 
group. Conversely, the lesser salary of the teacher does not give her the 
entree into the group with which the janitor finds his recreation. For such 
reasons, neither the current income nor the general economic status of an 
individual has been used in the present study as a criterion for establishing 
social levels. 

It is, moreover, difficult to know what an income may be worth in a 
particular instance. An income of a couple of thousand a year would 
provide a very comfortable living for certain families, although it might 
spell poverty for the next family whose esthetic and cultural ideals demand 
much more to satisfy them. Moreover, the dollar has a different purchasing 
power in different cities and towns in different parts of the country, and it 
may vary within a single community, depending upon the standards of 
dress, of entertainment, and of social front which one must maintain in the 
particular social level to which he belongs. There are economic rating 
scales which are designed to take these many items into account; but any 
such scale, in order to be effective, needs to be so detailed that its use in 
anything but an economic survey is prohibitive. The better economic rating 
scales take about as long to administer as the entire interview on which 
the present case history study has been based. 



334 


SEXUAL BEHAMOR I\ THE HUMAN MALE 


The U. S. Department of Labor has used a job classification (U, S. Dept. 
Labor 1939) which assigns each indi\idual in accord with the inherent 
nature of the occupation or profession in which he engages. Specifically, 
the classification is as foliow's: 

1. Professional and managerial occupations 

2. Service occupations 

3. Agricultural, forestry, fisheries, and kindred occupations 
4-5. Skilled occupations 

6-7. Semi-skilled occupations 
8-9. Unskilled occupations 

There is obviously a certain amount of agreement betw’een this arrange- 
ment and the occupational classes used in the present study, f.e., an eco- 
nomic classification does coincide with one like the Chapin and Warner 
classification which is based on the social prestige of one's occupation. 
There are, however, considerable departures betw'een the tw’o systems. For 
instance, the professional group in the job classification includes college 
presidents and professors, accountants, actors, newspaper reporters and 
copy men, all teachers, all social w'orkers, and all trained nurses. The list 
includes persons wLo have advanced degrees for several years of univer- 
sity post-graduate work, persons w'ho have had no more than twelfth 
grade education aixl, in some cases, those who have had nothing more than 
grade schooling. Socially the group is not a unit. The persons included do 
not come together in their strictly social* activities. Grade school and high 
school teachers do not move in the same social groups as college professors. 
Business managers, who, in many cases, are economically much better off 
than college professors, are not ordinarily included in the social activities 
of the professional groups. Trained nurses in most instances have no more 
than twelve grades of regular schooling. In the same fashion, the several 
manufacturing groups and the agricultural groups in this classification 
include persons who are day laborers and persons w^ho are foremen and 
managers; and these several groups do not mingle socially. Economic and 
job classifications are set up, of course, to serve a totally different purpose 
from the one with which a student of the social organization or of the 
mores is most often concerned. It is unfortunate that so many social 
studies, including army surveys and most other governmental studies, and 
even some of the public opinion polls have used this job classification 
where a social level rating of the sort employed in the present study would 
have served much better. 

The reality of this intangible unit called a social level is further attested 
by the fact that each group has sexual mores which are, to a degree, dis- 
tinct from those of all other levels. Most people realize that each group 
wears clothing of a particular quality and of a particular style, that the 
styles of their clothing differ especially at social events, that there are 
differences in food habits, in table manners, in the forms of their social 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


335 


courtesies, in vocabularies and in pronunciations, and in the sorts of things 
to which they turn for recreation. Among social scientists there has been 
some recognition of these differences, more particularly in European 
countries where the social hierarchies are older and more fixed and even 
legally recognized; but there has been scant recognition of the possibility 
that the sexual patterns of different social levels might differ in any partic- 
ular way. The remarkably distinct patterns of sexual behavior which 
characterize these social levels are the subject of the analyses which follow 
in the present chapter. It is to be noted that the analyses are made for each 
criterion, educational level, and occupational class, separately. The close 
identities of the sexual records thus independently arrived at constitute 
some of the best evidence yet available that social categories are realities 
in our Anglo-American culture. 

INCIDENCES AND FREQUENCIES OF SEXUAL OUTLET 

In the present chapter and the one that follows, comparisons of patterns 
of sexual behavior in different social levels are made for educational levels 
and for occupational classes of the parent and of the subject. Comparisons 
are made for three educational groups: grade school, high school, and 
college. The sample now at hand is not large enough to allow a finer 
classification. Preliminary analyses on a two-year educational breakdown 
indicate that a smoothly graded series lies between each of the three groups 
utilized in this chapter, but the data are insufficient for final publication. 
We do have a college population which is large enough to break down into 
finer educational levels, but it became available at too late a date to be 
included in the present volume. 

The occupational classes utilized in the present analyses are 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 
and 7, as defined above. Class 0, the dependents, should not rate as a 
separate group in such analyses, and classes 1, 8, and 9 are not represented 
by large enough series in the sample to allow the six- way breakdown needed 
here. 

Total Outlet* The frequencies of total sexual outlet vary somewhat with 
the educational level to which an individual belongs (Table 81, Figure 97), 
although they do not differ as much as the frequencies for the several 
sources of outlet. Among single males, at all ages, and whether the calcu- 
lations are made as means or as medians, the highest total outlets are 
found among those boys who go into high school but never beyond. This 
is true while they are still in grade school, while they are in high school, 
and after they have left high school. While they are in grade school they 
may associate with boys who Will stop school at every level. Nevertheless, 
during these school years their outlets average 10 to 20 per cent higher than 
the outlets of the boys who will stop by the eighth grade, and 20 to 30 per 
cent higher than the outlets of the boys who will ultimately go to college. 

12 ' 



336 


SEXUAL BEHAMOR IN TllE HUMAN MALE 





Total Outlet by Educational Lfxtls 

AGE 

GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

CASES 

total population 

ACTIVE POPULATION 




1 

Mean 

Frequency ' 

Median 

Freq. 

1 

i 1 

Incid. ; Mean 

% : Frequency 

Median 

1 Freq. 


Single Males 


Adol.-15 

0-8 

712 

3.02 =fc: 0.14 

1.74 

91.3 

: 3.31 * 0.15 i 

2.08 


9-12 

606 

3.34 =fc 0.15 

2.29 

95.4 

' 3.51 =t 0.15 i 

2.46 


13+ 

2799 

2.83 =i= 0.05 

2.20 

95.8 

i 2.95 ±0.05 

1 t 

2.32 

16-20 

0-8 

720 

3.22=i=0.13 

2.16 1 

97.5 

: 3.30 ± 0.14 

2.24 


9-12 

607 

3.53 ±0.14 

2.66 

99.7 

3.54 ± 0.14 

2.67 


13 + 

2861 

2.70 ±0.07 

2.12 

99.8 

I 2.71 0.07 

2. 12 

21-25 

0-8 

361 

3.15 ±0.20 

1.94 

96.7 

1 3.26 ± 0.21 

2.03 


9-12 

263 

3.00 ±0.20 

2.24 

99.2 

' 3.02 ±0.20, 

2.26 


13 + 

1898 

2.49 ± 0.05 

L87 

99.8 

; 2.49 ± 0.05 ; 

i 1 

1.88 

26-30 

0-8 

159 

3.01 ±0.28 

1.97 

98.7 

i 3.05 ± 0.28 * 

2.00 


9-12 

117 

2.88 ±0.26 

2.05 

99.1 

i 2.91 ± 0.26 

2.07 


13 + 

487 

2.57 ± 0.12 

1.83 

99.8 

: 2.57 ±0.12* 

1.83 


Mamed Males 


16-20 

0-8 

158 

4.67 ±0.41 

3.06 

100.0 

i 4.67 ±0.41 

3.06 


9-12 

87 

5.05 ±0.52 

3.57 

100.0 

5.05 ± 0.52 

3.57 


13+ 

46 

4.13 ±0.54 

3.21 

100.0 

4.13 ±0.54 

3.21 

21-25 

0-8 

324 

4.02 ±0.25 

2.62 

100.0 

4.02 ±0.25 

2.62 


9-12 

164 

4,15 ±0.34 

2.86 

100.0 

4.15 ±0.34 

2.86 


13+ 

440 

3.70 ±0.13 

3.06 

100.0 

3.70±0.13 

3.06 

26-30 

0-8 

292 

3.51 ±0.24 

2.42 

99.3 

3.53 ±0.24 

2.44 


9-12 

135 

3.55 ±0.31 

2.53 

100.0 

3.55 ±0.31 

2.53 


13 + 

532 

3.19±0.11 

2,64 

100.0 

3.19 ±0.11 

2.64 

31-35 

0-8 

186 

2.58 ±0.20 

1.88 

100.0 

2.58 ±0.20 

1.88 


9-12 

82 

3.35 ±0.39 

2.53 

100.0 

3.35 ±0.39 

2.53 


13+ 

301 

2,65 ±0.13 

2.16 

100.0 

2.65 ±0.13 

2.16 

36-40 

0-8 

143 

2.26 ±0.18 

1.71 

100.0 

2.26 ±0.18 

1.71 


9-12 

58 

2.64 ±0.38 

2.04 

100.0 

2.64 ±0.38 

2.04 


13+ 

189 

2.56 ±0.20 

2.02 

99.5 

2.51 ±0.20 

2.02 

41-45 

0-8 

100 

1.87 ±0.20 

1,43 

100.0 

1.87 ±0.20 

1.43 


13 + 

138 

1,98 ±0.14 

1.69 

100.0 

1.98 ±0.14 

1.69 


Table 81. Total sexual outlet, as related to educational level 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


337 


It is obvious that such differences are not the product of something that 
the school contributes or fails to contribute, for the same school is sup- 
porting three very different patterns of sexual behavior at the same time. 
The differences must be dependent upon something which the boy has 
acquired from the community in which he was raised before he went to 
school, in which he lives while he is attending school, and in which he will 
continue to live after he quits school; or else these higher frequencies must 
be dependent upon some physical or physiologic capacity which these 
particular boys have and which is correlated with the progress of their 
schooling. Either social or biological factors, or both, might conceivably 
be operating. 

A finer educational breakdown than the one which is shown in Table 81 
suggests that the sexually most active group is the one that goes into high 
school but not beyond tenth grade. Since the laws in many states set a 
minimum age which must be attained before a boy or girl can stop school, 
it often happens that there is a considerable exodus of students who attain 
the age of sixteen (or whatever other age the particular state requires), 
somewhere about the middle of their high school careers. The boys who 
leave school at that time may represent a group that is not particularly 
studious, whatever its mental ability may be, and a group which is impatient 
of such confinement as the school offers and energetic in its pursuit of 
physical activity and social contacts. These are, however, merely hypoth- 
eses which need further investigation. 

The single males who have the lowest frequencies of total sexual outlet 
are those who belong to the college level. The boys who never go beyond 
eighth grade in school stand intermediate between the high school and the 
college groups, as far as the calculations in Table 81 show. It is to be 
recalled, however, that the breakdown in Chapter 9 indicates that early- 
adolescent males of this lower educational level actually have higher outlets 
than any other group in the population, in practically every age period. 
The over-all averages shown for the grade school males as a group are 
probably pulled down by the large number of undernourished, physically 
poor, and, therefore, late-maturing males who are in this class. It includes 
most of the feeble-minded and mentally lower individuals in the popula- 
tion, and many of these are physically poor and sexually inactive. But the 
physically well-developed and mentally normal individuals among these 
grade school boys are more active than the boys of any other educational 
level. 

The social level picture for total outlet among married males is quite the 
same as for single males. The married males who have the highest total 
outlet are those who went into high school but not beyond. This is true 
for every age group between 16 and 40 years of age, and may be true at 
older ages ; but the data beyond 45 become too scant for significant calcula- 



338 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


tion. It is impressive to find that what is true of populations in their teens 
usually holds true for those same populations at later ages, throughout 
the life span. Only a very few individuals ever depart from their original 
patterns. 

If the record for total outlet is analyzed on the basis of occupational 
classes (Table 107), it will be seen that there is as sharp and as consistent a 
differentiation of groups as there is on the basis of educational level The 
highest rates of total outlet are to be found among the males who belong to 
occupational class 3. This is as true of these males when they are boys 
living at home with their parents as it is of the same persons at older ages, 
when they are independent of their parents. On the other hand, males who 

TOTAL OUTLET 

Qccupkjmki 

CLASS 

2 

l.4x 

■ 3 . 

1.3 X A 

m 

5 _ 

►x t.Ox 

I 6 _ 

Figure 97. Total outlet, by educational level and occupational class 

For single males of the age group 16-20. Relative lengths of bars compare mean fre- 
quencies for the groups. 

belong to class 3 have about the same rates of total outlet, irrespective of 
whether their parents belonged to classes 3, 4, or 5. Since occupational 
class 3 is the one that includes semi-skilled workers, it contains a great 
many persons who do not go beyond grade school, and almost none of 
them go beyond high school (Table 80); and the generalizations based on 
occupational classes agree very well with the generalizations based upon 
educational levels. Since the occupational classes are not as sharply defined 
as educational levels, the frequency series are not quite as consistent as the 
frequencies shown by the educational breakdown. 

The lowest rates of total outlet are to be found in occupational class 4. 
This is the group which includes the skilled mechanics. The group has very 




SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


339 


diverse educational backgrounds (Table 80), and in Chapter 1 1 it will be 
pointed out that it is the most unstable of all occupational classes. Mem- 
bers of this group often aspire to move into higher levels, and they send a 
high proportion of their children to college. 

In general, the white collar groups (classes 5, 6, and 7) are low in their 
rates ; but of these class 7 shows the highest rates. This is the professional 
group. It usually has 17 to 20 years of schooling. The group has not been 
calculated separately in the educational breakdowns made in this volume, 
and it will be interesting to see what such a breakdown ultimately gives. 

Masturbation. Ultimately, between 92 and 97 per cent of all males have 
masturbatory experience (Tables 82, 132, Figures 98, 136). The accumula- 
tive incidence figures are hardly different for the high school and college 
groups^ but the lower figure (92%) belongs to the grade school group. The 
highest active incidence between the ages of adolescence and 15 is to be 
found among the boys who never go beyond high school. In later age 
periods the college males have the highest incidence. 

The highest frequencies of masturbation among single males, in all age 
periods, are in the college level, whether the calculations are made for 
total populations or for the active portions of the populations (that por- 
tion of the population which is actually utilizing this source of outlet). 
Between 16 and 20, for instance, masturbation among the single males of 
college level occurs nearly twice as frequently as it does among the boys 
who never go beyond grade school, and the differential is still higher in the 
twenties. This is the great source of pre-marital sexual outlet for the upper 
educational levels. For that group, masturbation provides nearly 80 per 
cent of the orgasms during the earlier adolescent years, as against little 
more than half the outlet (52%) for the lower educational level. In the late 
teens it still accounts for two-thirds (66%) of the college male’s orgasms, 
while the lower level has relegated such activity to a low place that pro- 
vides less than 30 per cent of the total outlet. In all later age periods the 
relative positions of these groups remain about the same. 

Differences in incidences and frequencies of masturbation at different 
educational levels are even more striking among married males. At the 
grade school level, there are only 20 to 30 per cent who masturbate in their 
early marital years, and the accumulative incidence figure climbs only a bit 
during the later years of marriage. The frequencies are very low. The high 
school group closely matches the grade school group in this regard* On the 
other hand, among the married males who have been to college, 60 to 70 
per cent masturbate in each of the age periods. 

In the grade school group of married males, only 1 to 3 per cent of the 
total sexual outlet is derived from masturbation. The proportion of the 
total sexual outlet derived by college males from this source begins at 8.5 



340 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Masturbation, by Educational Levels 


AGE EDUC. 
GROUP LEVEL 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

ACCUM. 

incid. 

/o 

Mean 

Median 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

j 1 

Incid. j Mean j Median 

Frequency 

Freq. 

% ; Freq. 1 Freq. 

i ' 

1 1 



Single Males 


Adol. 

-15 

0-8 

712 

1.55 

=fc: 

0.078 

0.85 

52.25 

84.7 

1.84 

1.12 

1 84 


9-12 

606 

1.93 

=i= 

0.089 

1.22 

59.08 

89.9 

2.15 

1.44 

* 90 


13+ 

2799 

2.22 

=fc; 

0.048 

1.61 

79.62 

82.4 

2.70 

2.06 

82 

16-20 

0-8 

720 

0.93 

=1= 

0.048' 

0.45 

29.15* 

84.4 

l.IO 

I 0.61 

i 

90 


9-12 

607 

1.30 

=t= 

0.074 

0.69 

37 . 17 ! 

89.0 

1.46 

I 0.84 

94 


134- 

2861 ! 

1.79 

=b 

0.038 

1.18 

66.37| 

88.6 1 

2.02 

, 1 . 50 

91 

21-25 

0-8 

361 

0.63 

=b 

0.057 

0.18 

20 . 15 ! 

62.3 : 

1.01 

0.50 

91 


9-12 

263 i 

0.87 

dr 

0.076 

0.37 

29.67* 

76.4 i 

1.13 

I 0.63 

95 


134- 

1898 

1.31 

dr 

0.039 

0.68 

53.31 

87.0 

1.50 

0.86 

94 

26-30 

0-8 

1 

159 i 

0.59 

dr 

0.087 

0.07 I 

20.68 

60.4 

0.97 1 

0.48 

92 


9-12 

117 

0.78 

dr 

0.095 

0.35 

27.69 

78.6 

0.99 

0.47 

97 


13 + 

487 

1.18 

dr 

0.081 

0.48 

45.88: 

83.2 

1.42 

0.73 

96 


Married Males 


16-20 

0-8 

158 

! 

iO.ll 

dr 

0.031 

0.00 

1 

2.40 

28.5 

0.39 

0.14 

29 


9-12 

87 

10.14 

da 

0.048 

0.00 

2.75 

39.1 

! 0.35 

0.08 

39 


13+ 

46 

j 

|0.35 

=b 

0.14 

0.05 

8.53 

63.0 

i 0.55 

0.19 

63 

21-25 

0-8 

1 324 

io.io 

dr 

0.020 

0.00 

2.43 

28.7 

1 0.34 

0.09 

29 


9-12 

164 

0.15 

dr 

0.037 

0.00 

3.70 

41.5 

0.37 

0.09 

42 


13+ 

440 

0.32 

dr 

0.030 

j 0.06 

8.79 

65.9 

0.49 

0.22 

68 

26-30 

0-8 

292 

0.09 


0.024 

1 0.00 

2.44 

21.2 

0.40 

0.08 

29 


9-12 

135 

0.18 

dr 

0.046 

0.00 

5.05* 

37.0 

0.48 

0.10 

42 


1 13+ 

532 

0.27 

d = 

0.023 

0.06 

8.671 

66.4 

0.41 

0.18 

69 

31-35 

0-8 

186 

0.05 

rfc 

0.019 

0.00 

1.79* 

19.4 

0.24 * 

0.07 

29 


9-12 

82 

0.13 

rfc 

0.045 

0.00 

4.04 

36.6 

0.37 

0. 10 

42 


13+ ! 

301 

0.24 

dr 

0.03 

0.05 

9.28 

64.1 

0.38 

0.13 

69 

36-40 

0-8 

143 

0.04 

d = 

0.013 

0.00 

1.59 

11.2 * 

0.32 

0.20 

29 


9-12 

58 

0.08 

d= 

0.030 

0.00 

3.15 

25.9 i 

0.32 

0.18 

42 


13+ 

189 

0.21 

dr 

0.036 

0.03 

8.26 

59.3 ! 

0.35 

0.09 

69 

41-45 

0-8 

100 

0.03 

dr 

0.013 

0.00 

1.41 

9.0 

0.30 

0.23 

29 


13 + 

138 

0.19 

d = 

0.043 

0.02 

9.71 

55.1 

0.34 

0.09 

69 


Table 82 . Masturbation, as related to educational level 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


341 


per cent during the early years of marriage, and rises to as much as 18 
per cent in the later years. The college group stands out as perfectly dis- 
tinct on this score. 

Among occupational classes, the professional group masturbates most 
frequently (Table 108, Figure 98). This is true whether the persons in that 
class originate from parental class 7, or whether they come- from parental 
classes 3, 4, or 6. Since essentially all professional persons have an educa- 
tional rating of 17+ these data from an occupational class analysis are 
quite in Hne with the data based on educational levels. The distinctions 
between occupational classes are, however, even more extreme than the 
differences between educational levels, as far as masturbation is concerned. 


MASTURBATION 


EDUCATIONAL 

LEVEL 


1.0 X 



OCCUPATIONAL 

CLASS 

■ 


I.Ox 



7 


Figure 98. Masturbation, by educational level and occupational class 


For single males of the age group 1 6-20. Relative lengths of bars compare mean fre- 
quencies for the groups. Note similarity of data based on educational levels and data 
based on occupational classes. 


Between the ages of 16 and 20, for instance, the males of occupational 
class 7 have average frequencies of masturbation which run 2.12, 2.17, 2.21, 
and 1.60 per week, varying with the parental occupational class from which 
they came. The corresponding groups of occupational classes 2 and 3 have 
masturbatory frequencies which run very close to 1 per week — sometimes 
a bit more, sometimes a bit less in the various breakdowns. The educational 
breakdown for. the same age period shows the college level masturbating 
with frequencies which are about 1.9 times the frequencies of the grade 
school males. Differences in the frequencies of the occupational classes are 
more nearly of the order of 2.2 to 2.5. Differences in attitudes on mastur- 
bation, pre-marital intercourse, and prostitution are among the most 



342 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 





Nocturnal Emissions, 

BY Educational Levels 

AGE 

GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

CASES 

TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULAnON | 

ACCUM. 

INCID, 

o/ 


Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq, 

1 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

0/ 

/o 

Mean 

Freq, 

j 1 

I Median 

1 Freq. 


Single Males 


AdoL 

-15 

0-8 

712 

0.05 =1=0.01 

0.00 

1.82 

25.4 

0.21 

0.08 

26 


9-12 

606 

0.15 0.02 

0.00 

4.44 

39.6 

0.37 

0 12 

39 


13+ 

2799 

0. 34 =1= O.OI 

1 

0,10 

12.15 

69.8 

0.48 

0.28 

70 

16-20 

0-8 

720 

0.15 #=0,01 

0.02 

4.83 

56.4 

0.27 

0.09 : 

65 


9-12 

607 

0.22 =1=0.02 j 

0.06 

6.33 

70.7 

0.31 

0.13 ' 

71 


13 + 

2861 

0.42 =1=0.01 

0.25 

15.65 

91.2 

0.46 

0.29 

94 

21-25 

0-8 

361 

0.16 #^0.02 

0.03 

5.02 

59,6 

0.26 

0.09 

73 


9-12 

263 

0.24 =i= 0.02 

0.08 

8.10 

71.1 

0.33 

0.20 

82 


13 + 

1898 

0.38 #= 0,01 

0.22 

15.67: 

87.0 

0.44 

j 

0.28 

96 

26-30 

0-8 

159 

0.18 =#0,03 

0.04 

6.26i 

64.8 

0.27 

0.09 

79 


9-12 

117 

0.21 =# 0.03 , 

0.08 

7.48 

70.1 

0.30 

0.20 

86 


13 + 

487 

0.31 *0.02i 

0.18 

11.93 

85 2 

1 

0.36 

0.25 

97 


Married Males 


16-20 

0-8 

158 

0.14 

db 

0.03 

0.00 

3.08 

48.1 

0.30 

0.08 


9-12 

87 

0.10 

=t= 

0.02 

0.02 

2.04 

54.0 

0.19 

0.10 


13 + 

46 

0.12 

=h 

0,03 

0.03 

2.99 

58.7 

0.21 

0.09 

21-25 

0-8 j 

324 

0.11 


0.02 

0.00 

2.79 

45.1 

0.25 

0.08 


9-12 * 

164 

0.12 

=i= 

0.01 

0.04 

2.85 

63.4 

0.18 

0.09 


13#= 

440 

0.17 


0.02 

0.05 

4.65 

66.4 

0.26 

0.09 

26-30 

0-8 

292 

0.12 

=b 

0.02 

0.00 

3.41 i 

47.3 

0.25 

0.08 


9-12 

135 

0.11 

=1= 

0.02 

0.02 

3.22: 

56.3 

0.20 

0.09 


13 + 

532 

0.15 

=# 

0.01 

1 

0.05 

4.69 

73.5 

0.20 

0.08 

31-35 

0-8 

186 

0.08 

=i= 

0.02 

0.00 

3.03 

39.8 

0.20 

0,07 


9-12 

82 

0.13 


0.03 

0.02 

3.79 

56.1 

0.22 

0,09 


13 + 

301 

0.15 

=j_ 

0.02 

0.05 

5.71 

74.8 

! 

0.20 

; 0.08 

36-40 

0-8 

' 143 j 

0.04 

5# 

0.01 

0,00 

1.85 

29.4 

0.14 

0.07 


1 9-12 

58 

0.12 

#= 

0.03 

0.01 

4.48 

51.7 

0.23 

0.09 


13+ 

189 

0.15 

St 

0.02 

0.06 

6.06 

72.0 

0.21 

0,09 

41-45 

0-8 

100 

0,04 

cb 

0.01 

0.00 

2.25 

31,0 

0.14 

0,07 


13+ 

138 

0.11 


0.01 

0.05 

5.87 

72.5 

0.16 

0.08 


Table 83. Nocturnal emissions, as related to educational level 




SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


343 


marked of all the distinctions between social levels, and this is true whether 
the calculations are made by educational levels or by occupational classes. 

The professional males who originated in parental class 5, becoming 
members of class 7 as a result of their university training, have masturba- 
tory rates which are 25 per cent lower than those of class 7 males who are 
derived from any other source. It is a striking situation for which we have 
no explanation at this time. 

Nocturnal Emissions. Masturbation may appear to be volitional behavior, 
and one may question whether the pattern in masturbation represents the 
individual’s choice, rather than something that has been imposed upon 


EDUCATIONAL 

LEVEL 


0-8 


9-12 


13 + 


Figure 99. Nocturnal emissions, by educational level and occupational class 

For single males of the age group 16-20. Relative lengths of bars compare mean fre- 
quencies for the groups. Note similarity of data based on educational levels and data 
based on occupational classes. 

him by the mores of his group. It is, therefore, particularly interesting to 
find that there are still greater differences between educational levels in 
regard to nocturnal emissions — a type of sexual outlet which one might 
suppose would represent involuntary behavior. 

Nocturnal emissions occur most often in that segment of the population 
that goes to college (Table 83, Figure 99). Among males of the college level 
the emissions begin at earlier ages than among males of lower educational 
levels. About 70 per cent of the boys who will go to college have such 
experience by age 15, whereas only about 25 per cent of the grade school 
group has started by then. Between 16 and 20 years of age, 91 per cent of 
the single males of the college level experience nocturnal emissions, while 




344 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


only 56 per cent of the lower level boys have such experience in the same 
period. The active incidence figures are highest for the college males in 
every other age group. Ultimately, nearly 100 per cent of the better edu- 
cated males have such experience, whereas the accumulative incidence 
figure is only 86 per cent for the high school group, and only 75 per cent 
for the grade school group. 

Between adolescence and age 15, upper level males average nocturnal 
emissions nearly seven times as frequently as the boys of lower educational 
levels. Between 16 and 20 the frequencies among the upper level males 
are nearly three times those for the lower level, if the whole population is 

PETTING TO CLIMAX 

OCCUPATIONAL 
CLASS 

2 

I.Ox 

3 

4 


5 


6 
7 

Figure 100. Petting to climax, by educational level and occupational class 

For single males of the age group 16—20. Relative lengths of bars compare mean fre- 
quencies for the groups. Note similarity of data based on educational levels and data 
based on occupational classes. 

involved in the calculation. For the active populations the frequencies for 
the college group are still twice as high. About the same differences hold 
in the older age periods, at least up to 30 years of age. 

In marriage there are only minor differences between the educational 
levels in frequencies of nocturnal emissions, but the highest incidence 
figures at all ages are to be found among the males who have gone to 
college. Before marriage, college-bred males draw between 12 and 15 per 
cent of their outlet from nocturnal emissions, while the males of lower 
educational levels draw only 5 or 6 per cent of their outlet from that source. 
After marriage the college males draw 3 to 6 per cent of their outlet from 
emissions, but the lower educational levels never draw over 3 per cent from 
that source. 



EDUCATIONAL 

LEVEL 


0-8 


9-12 


L.Sx 

15 + 


SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


345 


While it is clear that higher frequencies of nocturnal emissions are cor- 
related with more extended educational histories, the explanation of this 
correlation is not so apparent. It is evident that nocturnal dreams are not 
the product of the education in itself, for two groups of boys of different 
social levels, working together in the same class in grade school or in high 
school, may have totally different histories of emissions. Is this a measure 
of some difference in the psychologic or physiologic capacities of the two 
groups which correlates in some way with factors which determine their 
educational careers? These are problems which the physiologist and the 
psychologist will want to investigate in more elaborate detail. 

We do know that the frequencies of nocturnal dreams show some corre- 
lation with the level of erotic responsiveness of an individual. The boys of 
lower level are not so often aroused erotically, nor aroused by so many 
items as the boys from the upper educational levels. Nocturnal dreams 
may depend upon an imaginative capacity, in something of the same way 
that daytime eroticism is dependent upon the individual’s capacity to 
project himself into a situation which is not a part of his immediate 
experience. It may be that the paucity of overt socio-sexual experience 
among upper level males accounts both for their daytime eroticism and for 
their nocturnal dreaming. 

The record on frequencies of nocturnal emissions in different occupa- 
tional classes is fully as striking as the record based on an educational 
breakdown, and the two bodies of data lie in exactly the same direction 
(Table 109, Figure 99). The lowest average frequencies of nocturnal 
emissions, averaging not more than 2 or 3 per year, are to be found among 
the males of occupational class 2, which is the group that includes the day 
laborers, and the frequencies are only a bit higher for the semi-skilled 
workmen of occupational class 3. The frequencies for occupational classes 
6 and 7 (the college and graduate school groups), on the contrary, run 
nearer once in 2 weeks at practically every age level and irrespective of the 
nature of the parental occupational class from which these individuals 
come. This means that there are 10 to 12 times as frequent nocturnal 
emissions among males of the upper occupational classes as there are 
among males of the lower classes. 

Heterosexual Petting. Petting is pre-eminently an occupation of the 
high school and college levels. For all social levels, it may begin in high 
school or even before; but from 16 years of age, the males and the females 
who are most often involved are the ones who go into high school or 
ultimately into college (Table 84, Figure 100). About 92 per cent of the 
males of the high school and college levels engage in at least some kind of 
petting prior to marriage, and nearly as many (88%) of the grade school 
group has such experience. These figures are not very far apart, but there 
are greater differences in the limits to which the petting techniques go in 



346 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


these several groups. In general, males of the grade school and high school 
levels are more restricted in their petting behavior than males of the college 
level. 

Unfortunately, the data secured in this study do not allow a statistical 
calculation for each degree of petting experience, but there are precise 
data on the frequencies of petting which extends to the point of orgasm 
(Table 84, Figure 100). In the pre-marital histories of college males, about 
61 per cent reach orgasm by that means. It is only about 32 per cent of 
the high school males who ever have such experience, and only about 16 
per cent of the grade school group. 


Petting to Climax, Single Males, by Educational Levels 


age 

GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

CASES 

TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE population 

accum. 

INCID. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

%0f 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 

Adol 








1 


-1? 

0-8 

712 

0.03 =1=0 01 

0.00 

1.06 

13.3 

0.24 

0.06 

13 


9-12 

606 

0.05 =1=0.01 

0.00 

1.46 

19.5 

0.24 

0.07 1 

19 


13+ 

2799 

0.04 =1=0.01 

0.00 

1.54 

13.8 

0.31 

0.09 1 

14 

16-20 

0-8 

720 

0.05 =1=0.01 

0.00 

1.66 

21.2 

0.25 

0.07 

22 


9-12 

607 

0.08 =1=0.01 

0.00 

2,37 

34.3 

0.24 

0.07 

34 


13+ 

2861 

0.14=1=0.01 

0.00 

5.26 

45.8 

0.31 

0.09 

46 

21-25 

0-8 

361 

0.04=1=0.01 

0,00 

1,23 

15.5 

0.25 

0.07 



9-12 

263 

0.08 =1=0.02 

0.00 

2.77 

28.5 

0.28 

0.08 



13+ 

1898 

0.18 =1=0.01 

0.01 

7.50 

51.9 

0.35 

0.10 

58 

26-30 

0-8 

159 

0.06=1=0.03 

0.00 

1.96 

19.5 

0.28 

0.07 



9-12 

117 

0.05 =1=0.01 

0.00 

1,82 

25.6 

0.20 

0.08 



13+ 

487 

0.13 =1=0.02 

0.00 

5.17 

44.6 

0,30 

0,09 

61 


Table 84. Heterosexual petting to climax, and educational level 


In regard to the frequencies of petting to climax, the differences between 
educational levels are even more extreme. In the later teens, this source 
provides nearly three times as frequent orgasm for the males who go to 
college; and between 21 and 25, there is nearly 5 times as much orgasm 
from this source for the college males as there is for the males who never 
go beyond grade school. The lower level males derive something between 
1 and 2 per cent of their total outlet from petting in their pre-marital 
years. The college males derive between 5 and 8 per cent of their outlet 
from that source. 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


347 


Analyses of the record by occupational classes confirm the statement 
made above that petting is most characteristic of the upper social levels. 
The differences by occupational class (Table 110, Figure 100) are not 
notable in the early adolescent years, but they become greater between 
16 and 20, at which age classes 6 and 7 pet to the point of climax twice as 
often as classes 2 or 3. In the early twenties there is a 3 to 1 difference 
between the two ends of the occupational scale, and the distinctions are 
more or less true irrespective of the occupational classes of the parents. 

Pre-marital Intercourse. Pre-marital intercourse may be had either with 
companions or with prostitutes. In every social level coitus with girls who 
are not prostitutes is more frequent. In younger age groups there is a 10 
to 1 or still higher difference in favor of the non-prostitutes. In older age 
groups, males of the lower educational level who are not yet married turn 
to prostitutes more often than they did when they were younger; but non- 
prostitutes still provide a larger part of the coitus. At the college level, 
contacts with companions exceed the prostitute relations by some factor 
which lies between 20 and 100 in every age group, including the older 
groups. 

Pre-marital intercourse, whatever its source, is more abundant in the 
grade school and high school levels, and less common at the college level 
(Tables 85-87, Figures 101-102). Even in the period between adolescence 
and 1 5 the active incidence includes nearly half (48% and 43%) of the lower 
educational groups, but only 10 per cent of the boys who will ultimately 
go to college. In the later teens, 85 per cent of the grade school group and 
75 per cent of the high school group is having pre-marital intercourse, 
while the figure for the college group is still only 42 per cent. In later 
years the differentials are not so great but, compared with the grade school 
group, it is still only about two-thirds as many of the college males who 
have such intercourse. 

The accumulative incidence figures for pre-marital intercourse show 
much the same differences. About 98 per cent of the grade school level has 
experience before marriage^ while only 84 per cent of the high school level 
and 67 per cent of the college level is involved (Table 136, Figure 145). 

The frequency figures show still greater differences between educational 
levels. In the age period between 16 and 20^ the grade school group has 7 
times as much pre-marital coitus as the college group. There is not much 
drop in the differential even in the older age groups. The mother who is 
afraid to send her boy away to college for fear that he will be morally 
corrupted there, is evidently unaware of the histories of the boys who stay 
at home. Moreover, nearly half of the males who have intercourse while in 
college had their first experience while they were still at home, before they 
started to college (Table 136, Figure 145). Varying with the age period, the 



348 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 





Total Non-marital Intercourse, by Educational Levels 

AGE 

EDUC. 

CASES 

total population 

ACTIVE population 

ACCUM. 

incid. 

% 

GROUP 

LEVEL 


Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

% of 
Total 
Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 


Single Males: Pre-marital Intercourse 


Adol. 

-15 

0-8 

630 

1.08 =t=0.10 

0.00 

37.94 

48.1 

2.24 1 

1.21 

48 


9-12 

511 

0.81 =i=0.10 

0.00 

25.45 

43.2 

1.88 1 

0.84 

43 


13 + 

2421 

0.08 =±=0.01 

0.00 

3.73 

9.8 

0.83 

0.29 

10 

16-20 

0-8 

635 

1.74 =t=0.11 

0.73 

58.92 

85.4 

2.04 

0.99 

86 


9-12 

515 

1.43 =i=0.12 

0.44 

42.17 

75.5 

1.89 

0.89 1 

76 


13+ 

2475 

0.25 + 0.02 

0.00 

11.26 

41.8 

0.60 

0.17 

44 

21-25 

0-8 

312 

2,00 =±=0.19 

0.82 

68.23 

86.2 

2.32 ^ 

1.06 

90 


9-12 

217 

1,25 =i=0.19 

0.38 

42.72 

74.2 

1.69 i 

0.77 

84 


13+ 

1593 

0.44 =±=0.03 

0.02 

19.41 

53.9 

0.81 

0.30 

64 

26-30 

0-8 

137 

1.82 =±=0.24 

0.88 

57.60 

87.6 

2.07 

1.16 

94 


9-12 

95 

1.18 =±=0.22 

0.41 

38.72 

71.6 

1.64 

0.86 

85 


13+ 

373 

0.64 =±=0.08 

0.05 

21.40 

56.3 

1.14 

0.48 

68 


Married Males: Extra-marital Intercourse 


16-20 

0-8 

139 

0.52 

=fc 

0.11 

0.00 

11.37 

44.6 

1.16 

0.44 


9-12 

87 

0.54 


0.16 

0.00 

10.91 

37.9 

1.44 

0.23 


13+ 

46 

0.12 

db 

0.07 

0.00 

2.91 

19.6 

0.61 

0.10 

21-25 

0-8 

284 

0.53 

sfc : 

0,13 

0.00 

12.89 

34.5 

1.53 

0.36 


9-12 

144 

0.48 

rfc 

0.11 

0.00 

11.21 

43.1 

1.11 

0.30 


13+ 

323 

0.05 

=±= 

0.02 

0.00 

1.57 

14.2 

0.38 

0.11 

26-30 

0-8 

244 

0.26 

=fc 

0,05 

0.00 

7.66 

35.7 

0.72 

0.25 


9-12 

113 

0.36 

=±= 

0.11 

0.00 

9.75 

46.9 

0.77 

0.19 


13+ 

380 

0.07 

sir 

0.02 

0.00 

2. 43 1 

19.5 

0.37 

0.08 

31-35 

0-8 

186 

0.18 

=±= 

0.04 

0.00 

6.97 

31.7 

0.56 

0.26 


9-12 

82 

0.19 

=fc 

0.06 

0.00 

5.61 

36.6 

0.51 

0.21 


13 + 

301 

0.16 

d = 

0.04 

0.00 

5.92 

24.6 

0.63 

0.25 

36-40 

0-8 

143 

0.18 

dr 

0.05 

0.00 

8.17 

26.6 

0.68 

0.35 


9-12 

58 

0.09 

dr 

0.03 

0.00 

3.45 

32.8 

0.27 

0.19 


13+ 

189 

0.26 

dr 

0.11 

0.00 

10.38 

29.6 

0.89 

0.21 

41-45 

0-8 

100 

0.12 

dr 

0.03 

' 0.00 

! 6.30 

21.0 

0.57 

0.44 


13 + 

138 

0.12 

dr 

0.03 

0.00 

6.39 

23.9 

0.52 

0.21 

46-50 

0-8 

70 

0.11 

rd 

0.04 

0.00 

6.05 

18.6 

0.59 

1 : 

0.42 1 


13+ 

81 

0.25 

dr 

0.08 

0.00 

14.09 

27.2 

0.93 

0.58 


Table 85. Total pre-marital and extra-marital intercourse, as related to 
educational level 

Including the non-marital intercourse with companions and with prostitutes. 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


349 


college group derives 4 to 21 per cent of its pre-marital outlet from inter- 
course; the high school group derives 26 to 54 per cent of its outlet from 
that source; but the grade school group depends on coitus for 40 to 70 per 
cent of its total pre-marital outlet. 

The number of college-bred males who have some pre-marital inter- 
course is high enough to surprise many persons, but the frequencies with 
which they have it are very much lower than anywhere else in the popula- 
tion. Between a third and a half of the males at college level have inter- 
course only once or twice, or half a dozen times, or a matter of two or 
three times a year for a few years before they marry. It is about 15 per cent 

TOTAL PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

OCCUPATIONAL 

CLASS 

I 

7.0x 7.7x 

3 


9-12 

LOx 

13 + 


Figure 101. Total pre-marital intercourse, by educational level and occupational 

class 

For single males of the age group 16-20. Relative lengths of bars compare mean fre- 
quencies for the groups. Note similarity of data based on educational levels and data 
based on occupational classes, 

of the college males who have pre-marital intercourse with weekly regu- 
larity for any period of years before marriage. A good many college males 
never have pre-marital intercourse with more than the one girl whom they 
subsequently marry, and very few of them have pre-marital intercourse 
with more than half a dozen girls or so. College males are very slow in 
arriving at their first pre-marital intercourse (Figure 146), and a comparison 
of the accumulative incidence curves (Table 136, Figure 146) indicates that, 
on an average, they do not have their first experience until five or six years 
after the lower level males start. 

The pre-marital coital pictures for the grade school and high school 
groups are much alike. They both differ from the college group in starting 


4 

5 

6 

I.Ox 

7 




350 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 





Non-marital Intercourse with Companions, 

BY Educational Levels 

AGE 

GROUP 

EPUC. 

LEVEL 

CASES 

TOTAL population 

ACTIVE population 





Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 

INCID. 

% 

i 


Single Males: Pre-marital Intercourse 


Adol. 










-15 

0-8 

712 

1.04 0.09 

0.00 

35.00 

48.5 

2.15 

1.20 



9-12 

606 

0.81 ±0.09 

0.00 

24.93 

43.4 

1.88 

0.76 



13+ 

2799 

0.08 ±0.01 

0.00 

2.74 

9.3 

0.82 

0.27 


16-^20 

0-8 

720 

1.62±0.11 

0.54 

50.62 

81.2 

1.99 

0.95 



9-12 

607 

1.38 ±0.11 

0.39 

39.48 

73.1 

1.89 

0.86 



13+ 

2861 

0.25 ±0.02 

0.00 

9.13 

38.8 

0.63 

0.22 


21-25 

0-8 

361 

1.65 0.16 

0.43 

52.84 

78.1 

2.11 

0.78 



9-12 

263 

1.11 ±0.16 

0.27 

38.02 

70.7 

1.57 

0.62 



13+ 

1898 

0.45 ±0.03 

0.03 

18.45 

54.6 

0.83 

0.31 


26-30 

0-8 

159 

1.21 ±0.19 

0.37 

42.71 

76.7 

1.58 

0.67 



9-12 

117 

0.84 ±0.16 

0.16 

29.75 

66.7 

1.25 

0.37 



13 + 

487 

0.64 ±0.07 

0.06 

24.97 

57.9 

1.11 

0.44 


Married Males : Extra-marital Intercourse 

16-20 

0-8 

146 

0.47 ±0.10 

0.00 

10.91 

41.8 

1.14 

0.47 

42 


9-12 

94 

0.46 ±0.13 

0.00 

9.43 

35.1 

1.30 

0.34 

35 


13+ 

48 

0.11 ±0.06 

0.00 

2.86 

16.7 

0.68 

0.30 

16 

21-25 

0-8 

309 

0.54 ±0.14 

0.00 

11.62 

30.4 

1.78 

0.38 



9-12 

147 

0.41 ±0.11 

0.00 

9.35 

36.1 

1.14 

0.28 

37 


13+ 

357 

0.05 ±0.01 

0.00 

1.86 

14.3 

0.37 

0.10 

16 

26-30 

0-8 

278 

0.24 ±0.05 

0.00 

6.38 

28.8 

0.82 

0.32 



9-12 

124 

0.28 ±0,10 

0.00 

7.61 

38.7 

0.71 

0.15 

39 


13 + 

448 

0.08 ±0.02 

0.00 

2.72 

20.8 

0,38 

0.09 

23 

31-35 

0-^8 

211 

0.13 ±0.03 

0.00 

5.51 

28.4 

0.47 

0.17 



9-12 

85 

0.18 ±0.05 

0.00 

4.62 

35.3 

0.50 

0.18 

43 


13+ 

370 

0.15 ±0.03 

0.00 

5.51 

26.8 

0,56 

0.22 

31 

36^0 

0-8 

165 

0.14 ±0.04 

0.00 

7.24 

23.6 

0.60 

0.27 



9-12 

62 

0.11 ±0.05 

0.00 

1.76 

32.3 

0.35 

0.12 

44 


13+ 

248 

0.26 ±0.09 

0.00 

9.85 

29.4 

0.87 

0.25 

37 

41-45 

0-8 

114 

0.10 ±0.03 

0.00 

4.84 

20.2 

0.47 

0.26 



13 + 

175 

0.16 ±0.04 

0.00 

6.07 

24.6 

0.64 

0.12 

40 


Table 86. Pre-marital and extra-marital intercourse with companions, as related 

to educational level 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


351 


their intercourse at a much earlier age — in many cases in pre-adolescence, 
and in a large number of cases coincidentally with the onset of adolescence. 
Within two or three years after the onset of adolescence nearly all of those 
who will ever be involved have started heterosexual relations. Ultimately, 
10 to 15 per cent more of the grade school group is involved than of the 
high school group. 

As analyzed by occupational classes, pre-marital intercourse is much 
more frequently had by males of class 3, which is the group of semi-skilled 
workmen (Table 111, Figure 101). Between adolescence and 15 years of age 
there may be 15 times as much intercourse among males of class 3 as there 
is among the boys who will ultimately go to college and whose occupa- 
tional ratings will ultimately be in class 6 or 7. If the parental occupational 
class is 5 (the lower white collar group), there is 122 times as much pre- 
marital intercourse among the boys who regress to class 3 as there is among 
those boys who will ultimately go into the professional group. Between 16 
and 20, the differences between the extreme groups are somewhat less, but 
the boys who will end up in occupational class 3 are still having intercourse 
4 to 9 times as often as the boys who will move into occupational classes 
6 and 7. Even during the twenties, when intercourse becomes more com- 
mon at the upper levels, there is still 4 times as much of it among the males 
of occupational class 3. 

The males of occupational class 2 have high frequencies of pre-marital 
intercourse at all age levels, but they do not rate as high as the males of 
class 3. Just as was pointed out for the lower educational levels, this lower 
rate of the lowest class is certainly due to the higher incidence of feeble- 
mindedness, to the low physical state, and to the low social prestige of 
many of the individuals in the group. It is quite possible that this lower 
occupational class includes some groups who have very much higher rates 
than the average for the whole class. They are probably the lower level 
boys who became adolescent first (Chapter 9). Since class 2 as a group is 
quite unrestrained sexually, any male in the group who does have any 
amount of sexual drive would be likely to have relatively high frequencies 
of pre-marital intercourse. 

Intercourse with Prostitutes. Among those males who are not married by 
age 25, pre-marital intercourse with prostitutes has been had by 74 per cent 
of the grade school level, and by 54 per cent of the high school group, but 
by not more than about 28 per cent of those who belong in the college 
level (Table 87, Figure 102). These striking differences between educational 
levels were as true in a past generation as they are in the present day 
(Chapter 11). The active incidence figures in each of the five-year periods 
indicate that lower level males start relations with prostitutes at a much 
earlier age, and that three to four times as many of them are having inter- 
course with prostitutes in each age period. 



352 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Intercourse with Prostitutes, by Educational Levels 


AGE 

GROUP 


EDUC. 

CASES 

TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

ACCUMUL. 

INCID. 

7o 

LEVEI. 


Mean 

Frequency 

%0f 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 


Single Males: Pre-marital Intercourse 


AdoL-15 

0-8 

712 

0.029 

= 1 = 

0.007 

0.97 ' 

8.7 

0.33 

0.10 

8 


9-12 

606 

0.014 

=fc 

0.004 

0.44 

7.9 

0.18 

0.07 

8 


13 + 

2799 

0.003 

rt 

0.001 

0.11 

2.3 

0.13 

0.07 

2 

16-20 

0-8 

720 

0.20 

zh 

0.019 

6.21 

48.2 

0.41 

0.14 

51 


9-12 

607 

0.096 

dz 

0.011 

2.75 

41.4 

0.23 

0.08 

44 


13+ 

2861 

0.022 


0.002 

0.80 

19.3 

0.11 

0.06 

20 

21-25 

0-8 

361 

0.39 

=fc 

0.045 

12.55 

60.7 

0.64 

0.33 

74 


9-12 

263 

0.14 

db 

0.02 

4.66 

43.7 : 

0.31 

0.10 

54 


13 + 

1898 

0.03 


0.007 

1.27 

17.2 

0.18 

0.07 

28 

26-30 

0-8 

159 

0.41 


0.08 

14.34 

72.3 

0.56 

0.37 

80 


9-12 

117 

0.18 

db 

0.04 

6.46 

42.7 

0.43 

0.17 

61 


13+ 

487 

0.08 

=b 

0.035 

3.16 : 

16.4 

0.49 

0.08 

35 


Married Males: Extra-marital Intercourse 


16-20 

0-8 

158 

0.029 

db 

0.012 

0.61 

16.5 

0.18 

0.07 

17 


9-12 

87 

0.074 

=b 

0.048 

1.48 

16.1 

0.46 

0.07 

16 


13 + 

46 

0.002 

db 

0.001 

0.05 

4.3 

0.05 

0.08 

4 

21-25 

0-8 

324 

0.032 

=b 

0.009 

0.80 

15.1 

0.21 

0.08 

18 


9-12 

164 

0.061 

d = 

0.023 

1.49 

25.0 

0.25 

0.06 

25 


' 13 + 

440 

0.008 

d = 

0.003 

0.23 

5.2 

0.16 

0.07 

6 

26-30 

0-8 

292 

0.040 

db 

0.011 

1.16 

17.1 

0.24 

0.08 

20 


9-12 

135 

0.053 

=b 

0.021 

1.49 

20.0 

0.26 

0.07 

25 


13 + 

532 

0.006 

rfc 

0.002 

0.20 

6.0 

0.10 

0.07 

7 

31-35 

0-8 

186 

0.037 

=b 

0.012 

1.46 

15.6 

0.24 

0.09 

20 


9-12 

82 

0.033 

db 

0.017 

0.99 

17.1 

0.19 

0.08 

25 


13 + 

301 

0.011 

=b 

0.004 

0.41 

6.6 

0.16 

0.08 

8 

36-40 

0-8 

143 

0.021 

=fc 

0.007 

0.93 

9.8 

0.21 

0.13 

21 


9-12 

58 

0.044 

db 

0.024 

1.69 

19.0 

0.23 

0.09 



13 + 

189 

0.013 

=b 

0.005 

0.53 

8.5 

0.16 

0.08 

12 

41-45 

0-8 

100 

0.028 

d = 

0.01 

1.46 

11.0 

0.25 

0.23 

21 


9-12 

34 

0.015 

=b 

0.009 

0.70 

14.7 

0.10 

0.08 



13 + 

138 

0.006 

= i = 

0.003 

0.32 

5.1 

0.12 

0.08 

12 


Table 87. Intercourse with prostitutes, as related to educational level 
Median frequencies for the total populations are, for the most part, 0.00. 


SOQAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 353 

The percentage of the total sexual outlet which is derived by unmarried 
males from intercourse with prostitutes steadily rises in all educational 
levels with advancing age. Between 16 and 40 the percentage for males in 
the grade school level rises from about 6 to 23 per cent. For the high school 
level the figures at the same ages rise from less than 3 per cent to about 1 1 
per cent; and for the college males they start at a fraction of 1 per cent and 
rise no higher than 3 per cent in the later age periods. At 16 years of age, 
the grade school males derive seven times as much of their outlet from 
prostitutes as the college males do; and high school males get three or 
four times as much of their outlet from prostitutes as college males get from 

PRE- MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 


EDUCATIONAL 


OCCUPATIONAL 

LEVEL 


CLASS 

2 

9.1x 

0-8 

9H2 


_ 3 

4 



5 

I.Ox 


13 + 


6 



1.0 K 

7 


Figure 102. Pre-marital intercourse with prostitutes, by educational level and 

occupational class 

For single males of the age group 16-20. Relative lengths of bars compare mean fre- 
quencies for the groups. Note similarity of data based on educational levels and data 
based on occupational classes. 

that source. Among those who are still unmarried between 31 and 35, the 
lower level individuals have 36 times as much contact with prostitutes as 
the college males do. 

Except for these lower level males of older ages, the actual frequencies 
of contacts with prostitutes are relatively low. In spite of some opinion 
that the college male depends primarily on paid contacts for his pre-marital 
socio-sexual experience, this is the least significant part of all his sexual 
activities (except for the incidental outlet that he derives from intercourse 
with animals). The mean frequency of prostitute contacts for the entire 
male population of all ages and of all educational and occupational groups 



354 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


is 0.093 per week, or approximately 5 times per year. For the lower level 
groups it may average as high as 0.50 per week (25 times per year) between 
31 and 35 years of age. For the unmarried college males taken as a group, 
it never averages higher than 0.08 per week (4 times per year) in any age 
period. 

Extra-marital intercourse with prostitutes is a still less important item, 
at all social levels. In any age period, it never constitutes more than 1.5 
per cent of the outlet of the grade school level, 1.7 per cent of the outlet of 
the high school level, and 0.5 per cent of the outlet of the married males of 
college level. 


TOTAL EXTRA- MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


EDUCATIONAL 

LEVEL 

OCCUPATIONAL 

CLASS 

!6.7x 


2 

10.6)c “ 

» > ^ P 

0-8 


. ■ ‘ 

9-12 ' 



I.Ox 

13 + 

6 :;''V 



7 1 



Figure 103. Extra-marital intercourse, by educational level and occupational 

class 

For married males of the age group 21-25- Relative lengths of bars compare mean fre- 
quencies for the groups. Note similarity of data based on educational levels and data 
based on occupational classes. 

A breakdown of the population by occupational classes shows that most 
of the high frequencies of intercourse with prostitutes occur in occupa- 
tional classes 2 and 3, which are the day labor and semi-skilled workmen 
groups. The skilled workmen of class 4 show quite as high frequencies in 
those few instances where we have sufficient material to make calculations. 
Frequencies even in the lower occupational classes are not more than once 
in 6 weeks in any particular age group; but the frequencies are rarely more 
than once or twice in a year in occupational classes 5^ 6, and 7. These rates, 
which represent averages for total populations, are, of course, much lower 
than the rates for the active members of those populations; but if the 
analyses are made on the active populations, the differences are still 2 to 1 
in most cases, and in some Cases nearly 8 to 1, with the higher frequencies 



SOCIAL LFVLL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


355 


occurring in occupational classes 2 and 3. This breakdown by occupational 
classes is a strict parallel to the breakdown by educational levels. 

Marital Intercourse. At all social levels, practically one hundred per 
cent of the married males have intercourse with their wives (Table 88, Fig- 
ure 104). There are a few exceptions among the aged, among persons who 
are married for only brief periods of time, among spouses between whom 
there are insurmountable incompatibilities on questions of sex, in an 
occasional case where one or both partners are completely homosexual, or 
in a very few cases of persons who are religiously much restrained. There 
are exceedingly few such cases of abstinence, and the number is too small 
to show any trend by social levels. 

There are social differences, however, in regard to the percentage of the 
total sexual outlet which is derived from marital intercourse. In the age 
period between 16 and 20, among males of the grade school level, only 
about 80 per cent of the total sexual outlet comes from marital intercourse, 
while extra-marital intercourse accounts for another 11 per cent of the 
total outlet (Tables 86, 97, Figure 103). However, the portion of the outlet 
coming from marital intercourse in this grade school group rises to approxi- 
mately 90 per cent in the late forties and early fifties. Among males of the 
high school group, marital intercourse in the early years accounts for 82 
per cent, but rises to 91 per cent of the total outlet by the late forties. For 
the college level, marital intercourse starts out as a higher portion of the 
total outlet — nearly 85 per cent ; but it drops steadily through the successive 
years until by the middle fifties it accounts for only 62 per cent of the out- 
let of these males (Table 97, Figure 133). In comparison with males of the 
college level, males of the grade school level, in their middle fifties, derive 
26 per cent more of their total outlet from intercourse with their wives. 

In the course of his marriage, the outlet of the married male of the college 
level has increasingly included masturbation and nocturnal dreams and, 
strikingly enough, extra-marital intercourse. On the other hand, the 
lower level males never have much masturbation in their marital histories, 
and the amount becomes less in the later years. During their teens and 
early twenties, lower level males find a considerable outlet in extra-marital 
intercourse, but with the advancing years they become increasingly faithful 
to their wives. In short, lower level males take 35 or 40 years to arrive at 
the marital ideals which the upper level begins with; or, to put it with 
equal accuracy, upper level males take 35 to 40 years to arrive at the sexual 
freedom which the lower level accepts in its teens. Some persons may 
interpret the data to mean that the lower level starts out by trying promis- 
cuity and, as a result of that trial, finally decides that strict monogamy is a 
better policy; but it would be equally correct to say that the upper level 
starts out by trying monogamy and ultimately decides that variety is worth 
having. Of course, neither interpretation is quite correct, for the factors 



356 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

CASES 

Marital Intercourse, 

BY Educational Levels 

TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE population 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

16-20 

0-8 

158 

3.74 ± 0.35 

2.51 

79.92 

100.0 

3.74 =t 0.35 

2.51 


9-12 

87 

4 . 10 = 1 = 0.44 

2.79 

82.19 

100.0 

4 . 10 = t 0.44 

2,79 


13 + 

46 

3 . 47 = 1 = 0.51 

2.58 

85.40 

100.0 

3.47 = i = 0.51 

2.58 

21-25 

0-8 

324 

3.28 = 1 = 0.20 

2.22 

81.03 

99.4 

3.30 = s = 0.20 

2.23 


9-12 

164 

3.35 = 1 = 0.29 

2.53 

81.56 

100.0 

3.35 = i = 0.29 

2.53 


13 + 

440 

3 . 07 = 1 = 0.13 

2.50 

83.93 

99.5 

3.08 = b 0.13 

2.50 

26-30 

0-8 

292 

3 . 00 = 1 = 0.21 

2.11 

86.15 

99.3 

3 . 02 = 1 = 0.21 

2.12 


9-12 

135 

2.88 = 1 = 0.25 

2.08 

81.67 

100.0 

2.88 =b 0.25 

2.08 


13 + 

532 

2.61 = i = 0.10 

2.07 

82.76 

99.2 

2.63 = 1 = 0.10 

2.08 

31-35 

0-8 

186 

2.26 =±= 0.18 

1.72 

88.07 

100.0 

2.26 = 1 = 0.18 

1.72 


9-12 

82 

2.83 = 1 = 0.34 

2.11 

85.18 

100.0 

2,83 ± 0.34 

2.11 


13 + 

301 

2 . 05 = 1 = 0.11 

1.73 

78.34 

1 

99.3 

2.07 ± 0.11 

1.74 

36-40 

0-8 

143 

1 . 95 = 1 = 0.16 

1.56 

88.09 

99.3 

1.97 ± 0.16 

1.57 


9-12 

58 

2.29 = 1 = 0.38 

1.70 

88.18 

100.0 

2.29 ± 0.38 

1.70 


13 + 

189 

1 . 89 = 1 = 0.14 

1.56 

74.41 

98.9 

1,91 ± 0.15 

1.58 

41-45 

0-8 

100 

1 . 72 = 1 = 0,19 

1.16 

89.97 

99.0 

1.74 ± 0.19 

1.19 


13 + 

138 

1.48 = 1 = 0.10 

1.25 

76.38 

99.3 

1 . 50 ± 0.10 

1.26 


Table 88. Marital intercourse as related to educational level 


MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

EDUCATIONAL OCCUPATIONAL 


LEVEL 

CLASS 

7 

0-6 

3 

9-12 

l.lx 

5 


1.0, 

13 + 

0 

7 


Figure 104. Marital intercourse, by educational level and occupational class 

For married males of the age ^oup 21-25. Relative lengths of bars compare mean fre- 
quencies for the groups. Note similarity of data based on educational levels and data 
based on occupational classes. 




358 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

GROUT 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

CASES 

Total Intercourse, Married Males, by Educational 
Levels, Including Marital Intercourse and All 
ExtrA'MArital Intercourse 

total population 

active population 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

% of 
Total 
Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

16-20 

0-8 

140 

4.18=b0.38 

2.73 

90.97 

100.0 

4.18 ±0.38 

2.73 


9-12 

92 

4.73 =i= 0.50 

3.06 

93.10 

100 0 

4.73 ±0.50 

3.06 


13+ 

47 

3.63 =i=0.53 

2.63 

88.31 

100.0 

3.63 ±0.53 

2.63 

21-25 

0-8 

297 

3.75 =i=0.25 

2.41 

93.17 

100.0 

3.75 ±0.25 

2.41 


9-12 

143 

3.90 0.37 

2.63 

92.44 

100.0 

3.90 ±0.37 

2.63 


13+ 

326 

3.01 ±0.14 

2.38 

85.27 

99.7 

3.02 ± 0.14 

2.39 

26-30 

0-8 

265 

3.23 ±0.23 

2.27 

93.94 

99.6 

3 24 ±0.23 

2.28 


9-12 

117 

3,32±0.33 

2.32 

90.80 

100.0 

3.32 ± 0.33 

2.32 


13+ 

410 

2.56 ±0.11 

2.04 

84.42 

99.8 

2.57±0.11 

2.04 

31-35 

0-8 

201 

2.67 ±0.20 

1.87 

95.04 

100.0 

2.67 ±0.20 

1.87 


9-12 

83 

2.96 ±0.36 

2.10 

90.79 

100.0 

2.96 ±0.36 

2.10 


13+ 

338 

2.24±0.11 

1.87 

84.26 

99.4 

2.25 ±0.11 

1.87 

36-40 

0-8 

154 

2.32±0.18 

1.74 

96.26 

99.4 

2.34±0.18 

1.75 


9-12 

: 60 

2.28 ±0.37 

1.71 

91.63 

98.3 

2.32 ±0.37 

1.73 


13+ 

; 228 

2.14±0.15 

1.72 

84.79 

99.1 

2.16±0.15 

1.73 

41-45 

0-8 

109 

2.09 ±0.22 

1.46 

96.27 

100.0 

2.09 ±0.22 

1.46 


9-12 

33 

2.07 ±0.43 

1.70 

92.93 

100.0 

2.07 ±0.43 

1.70 


13 + 

164 

1.71 ±0.11 

1.39 

82.77 

99,4 

1.72 ±0.11 

1.40 

46-50 

0-8 

74 

1.99 ± 0.29 

1.18 

96.01 

97,3 

2.04 ±0.30 

1.25 


9-12 

' 24 

1.66 ±0.44 

0.95 

94.28 

100.0 

1.66 ± 0.44 

0.95 


13+ 

99 

1.61 ±0.15 

1.15 

82.59 

98.0 

1.64±0.15 

1.20 

51-55 

0-8 

53 

1.50 ± 0.30 

0.85 

95.26 

96.2 

1.56 ±0.31 

0.88 


13+ 

58 

1.36±0.18 

0.98 

79.27 

98.3 

1.38 ±0.18 

1.00 


Table 89. Total intercourse among married males, in relation to educational level 

The data cover the total outlet derived from marital intercourse plus the extra-marital 
intercourse which is had with both companions and prostitutes. 


SOCIAL LEVEL Am SEXUAL OUTLET 


359 


90 will show that the sexual outlet which is provided by homosexual rela- 
tions amounts to three or four times the outlet which is provided by prosti- 
tutes. 

Among the males who ultimately go to college, homosexual relations 
are less frequent, but they are still a material part of the total sexual pic- 
ture. Between adolescence and 15 years of age, 21 per cent of the single 
males of the college level is actively involved, at least in incidental experi- 
ence to the point of orgasm. The active incidence figure drops to 17 per 
cent by age 30. The number of college-bred males who ultimately have 
experience is 40 per cent, if they are not married by age 30. 

Frequencies for the college males are much lower than for any of the 
other educational levels. They average only about once in ten weeks for the 
population as a whole, and less than once in two weeks for the active 
population. For those males who are not yet married by 30, the mean 
frequencies rise to as much as 1.3 per week for the active portion of the 
population. Only about 3 per cent of the outlet of the college males is de- 
rived from the homosexual between adolescence and age 25, but in the 
next age period they derive nearly 9 per cent of their outlet from such con- 
tacts. 

After marriage only 2 or 3 per cent of the college males engage in homo- 
sexual relations, according to the histories that are now available. There 
is no doubt, however, that this is one of the points on which there has been 
considerable cover-up, and it is certain that a good many married males 
who are having homosexual relations have deliberately avoided contrib- 
uting their histories to this study. The 3 per cent incidence figure and the 
low frequencies shown here are, consequently, absolute minima, and they 
should be increased by some unknown quantity if they are to represent 
the reality. 

The data on the incidence, frequency, and total significance of homo- 
sexual relations among grade school males are intermediate between the 
data for the high school and the college males. In any single age period, 
about one-fourth of all the males of grade school level have some homo- 
sexual relations. This is true for all the years between adolescence and 30. 
Ultimately, about 45 per cent of the grade school group is involved. Fre- 
quencies of homosexual contacts are about once in four weeks for the 
group taken as a whole, and nearly once a week for those who are actively 
involved between the ages of 16 and 20. In marriage, the grade school 
group continues its homosexual relations in 10 per cent of the cases, but 
the incidence figures drop to about 3 per cent by age 45. The frequencies 
of homosexual contacts for homosexually active married males of the 
grade school level begin at about 1.4 per week and drop to a few times per 
year, or once in a year or two, in the older groups. 



360 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Homosexual Outlet, by Educational Levels 


AGE 

GROUP 


EDUC. 

CASES 

TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

ACCUMUL. 

LEVEL 









Mean 

Frequency 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Tncid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 

INCID. 

% 


Single Males 


Adol.-15 

0-8 

712 

0.24 

=1= 

0.03 

8 03 

23.7 

1.01 

0.42 

25 


9-12 

606 

0 29 


0.04 

8.73 

32 5 1 

0.88 

0.30 

33 


13+ 

2799 1 

0.09 

=t 

0.01 

3.14 

21.5 

0.41 

0.09 

22 

16-20 

0-8 

720 

0.22 

=b 

0.03 

6.85 

26.1 

0.84 

0.34 

32 


9-12 

607 

0.38 

d= 

0.05 i 

10.81 i 

40.9 

0.93 

0.31 

48 


13+ 

2861 

0.07 

db 

0.01 

2.43 1 

16.0 

0.41 

0.08 

27 

21-25 

0-8 

361 

0.25 

=h 

0.05 

8.06 

22.4 

1.12 

0.41 

38 


9-12 ! 

263 

0.48 

=b 

0.08 

16.31 

37.6 

1.26 

0.68 

53 


13+ 

1898 

0.09 

=fc 

0.01 

3.72 

9.5 

0.96 

0.30 

33 

26-30 

0-8 

159 

0.40 

d= 

0.14 

14.04 

27.7 

1.44 

0.48 

45 


9-12 

117 

0.73 

zh 

0.18 

25.95 

46.2 

1.58 

0.73 

55 


13+ 

487 

0.23 

=t 

0.04 

8.82 

17.2 

i 1.31 

0.66 

40 


Married Males 


16-20 

0-8 

158 

0.14 0.06 

3,08 

10.1 

1.43 

0.35 

10 


9-12 

87 

0.11 =fc 0.08 

2.11 

9.2 

1.14 

0.39 

12 


13 + 

46 


0.16 

2.2 



3 

21-25 

0-8 

324 

0.05 ± 0.02 

1.33 

9.3 

0.58 

0.09 



9-12 

164 

0.04 0.02 

1.05 

13.4 

0.32 

0.10 



13 + 

440 

0.02 =b 0.01 

0.53 

2.7 

0.72 

0.58 

3 

26-30 

0-8 

292 

0.02 0.01 

0.46 

4.8 

0.34 

0.09 



9-12 

135 

0.03 =1= 0.01 

0.96 

8.1 

0.41 

0.30 



13+ 

532 

0.03 =b 0.01 

0.96 

2.6 

1.16 

1.25 

4 

31-35 

0-8 

186 


0.14 

4.3 

0.08 

0.06 



9-12 

82 

0.05 ± 0.02 

1.38 

6.1 

0.75 

0.70 



13 + 

301 

0.02 0.01 

0.75 

3.0 

0.66 

0.10 

4 

36-40 

0-8 

143 


0.30 

2.8 

0.24 

0.08 



9-12 

58 

0.02 0.01 

0.73 

3.4 

0.55 

0.75 



13+ 

189 


0.89 

2.8 


0.10 


41-45 

0-8 

100 


0.08 

3.0 

0.05 

0.07 



13+ 

138 


1.64 

2.2 

1.47 

0.10 



Table 90. Homosexual outlet, as related to educational level 


Median frequencies for the total populations are uniformly 0.00. 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


361 


A breakdown of the homosexual data for the several occupational 
classes does not show marked or consistent differences between occupa- 
tional classes 2, 3, and 5 (Table 114, Figure 105). On most items of sexual 
activity class 5 is closer to classes 6 and 7, but in regard to the incidences 
and frequencies of the homosexual, it is closer to the semi-skilled and 
skilled labor groups. The active incidence figures for homosexual contacts 
among the lower occupational classes may be as high as 35 or 40 per cent 
in different groups at particular age periods, but they never go higher than 
14 per cent for the males of class 7, except during the period of earliest 
adolescence for that portion of class 7 which originates from parental 
class 5. 


HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


EDUCATIONAL 

LEVEL 


OCCUPATIONAL 

CLASS 


2 



I.Ox 


7 


IJ.Ox 


Figure 105. Homosexual outlet, by educational level and occupational class 

For single males of the age group 16-20. Relative lengths of bars compare mean fre- 
quencies for the groups. 


The frequencies of homosexual activity among the males of class 6 are 
a bit lower than the frequencies in the lower occupational levels. Class 7 
is the most distinct. Its frequencies are very much below those of every 
other occupational class. In practically every age group, and irrespective 
of the parental occupational class from which these class 7 males may have 
come, the frequencies average only about one-fourth or one-fifth of those 
for the lower occupational classes. If the calculations are made only for 
those males who do become actively involved, the mean frequencies for 
class 7 are still only half as high as the mean frequencies for the active 
males of classes 3 and 5. Males of occupational class 6 are intermediate 
between the males of the lower levels and those of class 7. 

The situation portrayed by frequencies in the homosexual is more or 
less paralleled by the calculations showing the percent of the total sexual 



362 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


outlet which is derived from this source in each of the occupational 
classes. An average of 10 per cent or more of the total sexual outlet may be 
derived from the homosexual by males of classes 2 and 5, while among 
males of class 7 the average of the total outlet which is so derived is never 
more than 2 per cent. The males of class 6 are rather intermediate in this 
regard, or more nearly approach the males of class 5 in deriving upward of 
10 per cent (in one group slightly more than 10 per cent) of their orgasms 
in contacts with other males. 

Animal Intercourse. Intercourse with animals other than the human is 
almost entirely confined to males raised in rural areas. Only an occa- 


AGE 

GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

CASES 

Animal Contacts, Single Males, by Educational Level 

TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

ACCUMUL. 

INCID. 

RURAL 

MALES 

ONLY 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 

Adol.-15 

0-8 

712 

0.026 =1=0.007 

0.86 

5.8 

0.45 

0.14 

10 


9-12 

606 

0.030 =1=0,009 

0.91 

5.9 

0.50 

0.16 

9 


13+ 

2799 

0.020=1=0.005 

0.71 

5.3 

0.37 

0.07 

23 

16-20 

0-8 

720 

0.022 =1=0.007 

0.68 

4.0 

0.54 

0.17 

14 


9-12 

607 

0.038 =1= 0.012 

1.08 

4.6 

0.82 

0.24 

17 


13+ 

2861 

0.010=1=0.004 

0.36 

2.5 

0.39 

0.09 

25 

21-25 

0-8 

361 

0.005 =1= 0.003 

0.15 

1.1 

0.43 

0.33 

14 


9-12 

263 

0.014 =b 0.010 

0.47 

2.7 

0.51 

0.10 

20 


13+ 

1898 

0.002 =1= 0.001 

0.09 

0.7 

0.32 

0.10 

26 


Table 91. Animal contacts, as related to educational level 

The active population is almost wholly rural and the active frequencies are essentially 
those for that portion of the rural population which has animal contacts. Median fre- 
quencies for the total populations are uniformly 0.00. 


sional contact is had by city boys, unless they visit farms in vacation 
periods. Consequently, averages of animal contacts for the total American 
population are so low that they cannot be calculated with an accuracy 
which means anything in terms of the actualities of human behavior. For 
the rural males who are actively involved in such contacts, animal inter- 
course is more significant (Table 91). 

The accumulative incidence figures for animal intercourse go to about 
14 per cent for the farm boys who do not go beyond grade school, to about 
20 per cent for the group which goes into high school but not beyond, and 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


363 


to 26 per cent for the males who will ultimately go to college. The boys of 
college level who are ever involved in animal intercourse number nearly 
twice as many, relatively, as the boys who never go beyond grade school. 

On the other hand, the boys of lower educational levels who are actually 
involved are the ones who have the highest frequencies in animal contacts 
(Table 91). For them the frequencies average close to once in two weeks, 
plus or minus. The frequencies for the boys of the college level who are 
actually having any animal contacts average nearer once in three weeks. 

ATTITUDES ON SEXUAL TECHNIQUES 

In addition to differences in frequencies and sources of sexual outlet, 
social levels differ in their attitudes on other matters of sex. Their sources 
of erotic interest, attitudes toward nudity, and techniques utilized in 
coitus are the items on which we have sufficient data to warrant some 
treatment here. 

Sources of Erotic Arousal. The upper level male is aroused by a con- 
siderable variety of sexual stimuli. He has a minimum of pre-marital or 
extra-marital intercourse (Tables 96, 97). The lower level male, on the 
other hand, is less often aroused by anything except physical contact in 
coitus; he has an abundance of pre-marital intercourse, and a considerable 
amount of extra-marital intercourse in the early years of his marriage. 
How much of this difference is simply the product of psychologic factors 
and how much represents a community pattern which can be properly 
identified as the mores, it is difficult to say. The very fact that upper level 
males fail to get what they want in socio-sexual relations would provide a 
psychologic explanation of their high degree of erotic responsiveness to 
stimuli which fall short of actual coitus. The fact that the lower level male 
comes nearer having as much coitus as he wants (Table 92) would make 
him less susceptible to any stimulus except actual coitus. 

The higher degree of eroticism in the upper level male may also be con- 
sequent on his greater capacity to visualize situations which are not imme- 
diately at hand. In consequence, he is affected by thinking about females, 
and/or by seeing females or the homosexual partner, by burlesque shows, 
obscene stories, love stories in good literature, love stories in moving 
pictures, animals in coitus, and sado-masochistic literature. Upper level 
males are the ones who most often read erotic literature, and the ones who 
most often find erotic stimulation in pictures and other objects. None of 
these are significant sources of stimulation for most lower level males, who 
may look on such a thing as the use of pictures or literature to augment 
masturbatory fantasies as the strangest sort of perversion, 

While these group differences may be primarily psychologic in origin, 
there is clearly an element of tradition involved, Each community more or 
less accepts the idea that there will be or will not be erotic arousal under 



364 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


particular sorts of circumstances. The college male who continuously talks 
about girls does so with a certain consciousness that the other persons in 
his group are also going to be aroused by such conversation, and that they 
accept such arousal as natural and desirable. The homosexual male, and 
the heterosexual male who does not approve of such deliberately induced 


Attitudes on Pre-marital Intercourse 
AT Three Educational Levels 


restraints 

ON 

ESrrERCOURSE 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

all 

ages 

ADOL.-25 

26^5 

46+ 

Cases 

% 

Cases 

% 

Cases 

% 

Cases 

% 

Moral objections 

0-8 

814 

20.8 

317 

18.9 

338 

16.2 

159 

33.9 


9-12 

650 

25.5 

369 

22.8 

232 

28.5 




13 + 

3161 

61.4 

2016 

62.5 

969 

56.6 

176 

76.2 

Fear of public 

0-8 

775 

13.5 

300 

14.4 

322 

11.5 

153 

16.3 

opinion 

9-12 

615 

14.3 

343 

12.0 

224 

17.0 




, 13 + 

2847 

22.8 

1756 

21.5 

918 

24.1 

173 

28,9 

Fear of pregnancy 

0-8 

814 

20.4 

318 

19.2 

336 

20.6 

160 

22.5 


9-12 

645 

17.5 

364 

18.6 

232 

15.5 




13-H , 

3136 

27.6 

1995 

28.0 

964 

27.5 

177 

23,7 

Fear of venereal 

0-8 

811 

28.6 

317 

27.1 

335 

29.0 

159 

30.8 

disease 

9-12 

641 

25.3 

361 

23.8 

231 

26.8 




13 + 

3143 

24,8 

2001 

25.1 

965 

23.9 

177 

25.4 

Lack opportunity 

0-8 

785 

34.6 

1 

312 

36.9 

323 

33.8 

150 

32.0 


9-12 

627 

38.0 

358 

37.5 

225 

39.6 


I 


13 + 

3104 

51.6 

1980 

51.4 

950 

55.5 

174 

33.4 

Lack of interest in 

0-8 

327 

41.9 

155 

35.5 

117 

45.3 

55 

52.7 

having more 

9-12 

279 

44.5 

153 

45.1 

111 

45.9 




13 + 

1831 

18.8 

1041 

18.7 

688 

19.2 

102 

17.6 

Male desires to 

0-8 

595 

43.3 

215 

40.9 

267 

38.5 

113 

59.3 

many a virgin 

9-12 

523 

39.2 

309 

40.8 

176 

32.4 




13 + 

2972 

46.5 

1943 

50.7 

880 

36.2 

149 

41.6 


Table 92. Attitudes on pre-marital intercourse, at three educational levels 


eroticism, considers this public display of elation over females as a group 
activity which is more or less artificially encouraged. The lower level male 
who talks about girls quite as frequently, or even more so, is less often 
aroused by such talk and may be inclined to consider a listener who is so 
aroused as somewhat aberrant. There is an element of custom involved in 
these styles of erotic response. 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


365 


Nudity. In many cultures, the world around, people have been much 
exercised by questions of propriety in the public exposure of portions or the 
whole of the nude body. There are few matters on which customs are more 
specific, and few items of sexual behavior which bring more intense reac- 
tions when the custom is transgressed. These customs vary tremendously 
between cultures and nations, and even between the individual communities 
in particular countries. The inhabitant of the Central American tropics has 
one custom, the Indian who comes down from his mountain home to trade 
in the lowland has totally different customs. There is neither rhyme nor 
reason to the custom — there is nothing but tradition to explain it. The 
mountain Indian of the warmer country of Southern Mexico is thoroughly 
clothed, the mountain Indian of the coldest part of Northern Mexico is 
more completely nude than the natives of the hottest Mexican tropics. But 
there are probably no groups in the world who are free of taboos of some 
sort on this point. The history of the origin of clothing is more often one of 
taboos on nudity than a story of the utility of body coverings. 

The English are more or less justly reputed to be the most completely 
clothed people in the world, and Americans have been slow in breaking 
away from the English tradition. The American visitor to foreign lands is 
often amazed at the exposure which is allowed in some other cultures, and 
he criticizes it on moral grounds. The nudity of the French burlesque is 
ascribed to the “low morality” of Frenchmen as a group; and although an 
approach is made to the same sort of display in American burlesque, the 
institution here does not achieve the same free acceptance of complete 
nudity which the original French has. The German nudist movement is 
assumed by the average American to be immoral in intent, and its counter- 
part in this country survives only after considerable public discussion and 
continual wrangling in court over the obscenity of such activity. Although 
Anglo-American law has tried for six or seven centuries to define indecent 
exposure, there is no legal agreement on the decency or indecency of nude 
art, nor on the rights of art schools, photographers, magazines, and books 
to portray the nude human form. Public sentiment, backed by sporadic 
police action, has dictated the styles of bathing suits, from the gay nineties 
down to the present. It is only within the last decade or two that the 
male’s right to appear in swimming trunks without tops has been estab- 
lished for public swimming beaches and pools. 

More definite limits may be set on nudity than on more overtly sexual 
activities. The kissing which is commonplace in American films is con- 
sidered most immoral in some of the foreign countries to which the films 
are distributed. A completely nude art production may be shown in a 
Latin American moving picture theatre to an audience which takes the 
film complacently, for its artistic value, although it will hiss the next 
picture off the screen because it contains a Hollywood kissing scene. 



366 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


The acceptance of nudity may even vary with the hour and the place of 
the exposure. The costume which is accepted on the swimming beach is 
strictly forbidden in most other places i In the middle of the day, the female 
may safely expose her arms in public, although she is then limited in regard 
to the exposure of her back. At the formal affair in the evening, she may 
expose the whole of her back, but she is then most proper if she covers 
her arms with long gloves. In a Latin American tropic town, inside a public 
building, there may be considerable objection when one rolls his shirt 
sleeves to the elbow, even on the hottest summer day; but out of doors 
both men and women may go stripped to the waist through the streets of 
the town, and all of them may come together for nude bathing in the 
nearby stream. It would require a considerable treatise to portray the reac^ 
tions of the peoples of the world to nudity, and a larger treatise to explain 
the origins of those customs. 

Most amazing of all, customs in regard to nudity may vary between the 
social levels of a single community. In our American culture, there is a 
greater acceptance of nudity at upper social levels, and greater restraint at 
lower social levels. Compared with previous generations, there is a more 
general acceptance of nudity in the upper social level today (Table 95). 
There is an increasing amount of nudity within the family circle in this 
upper level. There is rather free exposure in the home for both sexes, 
including the parents and the children of all ages, at times of dressing and 
at times of bathing. Still more significant, there is an increasing habit 
among upper level persons of sleeping in partial or complete nudity (Table 
95). This is probably more common among males, though there is a con- 
siderable number of upper level females who also sleep nude. Among the 
males of the college level, nearly half (41%) frequently sleep nude, about 
one-third (34%) of the high school males do so, but only one-sixth (16%) 
of the males of the grade school level sleep that way. 

Finally, the upper level considers nudity almost an essential concomitant 
of intercourse. About 90 per cent of the persons at this level regularly 
have coitus nude (Table 95). The upper level finds it diflBcult to comprehend 
that anyone should regularly and as a matter of preference have intercourse 
while clothed. This group uses clothing only under unusual circumstances, 
or when variety and experimentation are the desired objectives in the inter- 
course. On the other hand, nude coitus is regularly had by only 66 per cent 
of those who never go beyond high school, and by 43 per cent of those 
who never go beyond grade school. 

This intercourse with clothing is not a product of the inconveniences of 
the lower level home, nor is it dependent upon the diflBculties of securing 
privacy in a small home, as too many sociologists have gratuitously 
assumed. It is primarily the product of the lower level’s conviction that 
nudity is obscene. It is obscene in the presence of strangers, and it is even 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


367 


obscene in the presence of one’s spouse. Some of the older men and women 
in this group take pride in the fact that they have never seen their own 
spouses nude. 

Many persons at this level strictly avoid nudity while dressing or undress- 
ing. They acquire a considerable knack of removing daytime clothing and 
of putting on night clothing, without ever exposing any part of the body. 
This is less often true of the younger generation which has been exposed to 
the mixture of social levels encountered in the CCC camps, the Y.M.C.A., 
and the Army and the Navy. Exposure of the upper half of the male body 
on swimming beaches started as an upper level custom, but the democracy 
of the public beach has fostered a much wider acceptance of nudity among 
lower social levels today. Compare the three generations of the educational 
level 0-8 in Table 95. Younger males, even of the laboring groups, are often 
seen at work, out of doors, in public view, while stripped to the waist; but 
older males of the same social level still keep their arms covered to the 
wrist, even on the hottest of days and while engaged in the most uncomfort- 
able of jobs. These inroads on the traditions against nudity are reflected 
in the sleeping and coital customs of younger persons of these lower levels, 
but the older members of these groups still observe the traditions. There 
are some cases of lower level males who have been highly promiscuous, who 
have had intercourse with several hundred females, and who emphasize 
the fact that they have never turned down an opportunity to have inter- 
course except “on one occasion when the girl started to remove her clothing 
before coitus. She was too indecent to have intercourse with!” 

Manual Manipulation. At upper social levels there may be considerable 
manual petting between partners, particularly on the part of the male who 
has been persuaded by the general talk among his companions, and by the 
codification of those opinions in the marriage manuals, that the female 
needs extended sensory stimulation if she is to be brought to simul- 
taneous orgasm in coitus. Upper level petting involves the manual stimu- 
lation of all parts of the female body. 

Manual manipulation of the female breast occurs regularly in 96 per cent 
of the histories of the married males of the upper level, and manual manipu- 
lation of the female genitalia is regularly found in about 90 per cent of the 
histories (Table 93). The upper level believes that this petting is necessary 
for successful coital adjustment; but preliminary calculations indicate that 
the frequency of orgasm is higher among lower level females than it is 
among upper level females, even though the lower level coitus involves a 
minimum of specific physical stimulation (Table 93). 

The manual manipulation of the female breast occurs in only 79 per cent 
of the married male histories at lower levels, and the manipulation of the 
female genitalia occurs in only 75 per cent of the cases (Table 93). Even 


13 



Utilization of Petting Techniques 
AT Three Educational Levels 


TECHNIQUE 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

ALL 

AGES 

ADOL.-25 

26-45 

46+ 

Cases 

V 

/o 

Cases 

% 

Cases 

% 

Cases 

o/ 

/o 

Kissing, lip. Mar- 

0-8 

457 

88.4 

73 

95.9 

252 

90.1 

132 

81.1 

ital. Frequent 

9-12 

267 

93.2 

80 

98.7 

149 

94.0 



13 + 

1071 

97.7 

225 

99.6 

689 

97.5 

157 

95.5 

Kissing, deep. 

0-8 

455 

40.5 

73 

54.8 

252 

42.4 

130 

28.5 

Marital. Fre- 

9-12 

267 

56.2 

80 

60.0 

149 

58.4 



quent 

13 + 

1069 

77.3 

225 

86.7 

687 

78.3 

157 

59.2 

Breast, manual. 

0-8 

458 

78.6 

74 

85.1 

252 

81.4 

132 

69.7 

Marital. Fre- 

9-12 

266 

90.6 

80 

92.5 

148 

93.9 



quent 

13 + 

1071 

96.3 

225 

99.0 

689 

96.5 

157 

91.1 

Breast, oral. Mar- 

0-8 

458 

33.2 

74 

32.4 

252 

36,1 

132 

28.0 

ital. Frequent 

9-12 

266 

57.5 

80 

57.5 

148 

62.9 



13 + 

1071 

81.6 

225 

92.5 

689 

81.1 

157 

67.5 

Female genitalia, 

0-8 

235 

79.1 

151 

82.2 

63 

76.2 



manual. Pre-m. 

9-12 

224 

85.3 

173 

87.3 





Frequent 

13+ 

566 

91.0 

439 

91.8 

118 

90.7 



Female genitalia, 

0-8 

457 

74.8 

74 

74.3 

252 

78.2 

131 

68.7 

manual. Mar- 

9-12 

266 

79.7 

80 

77.4 

148 

85.1 



ital. Frequent 

13 + 

1071 

89.6 

225 

94.6 

689 

89.2 

157 

83.4 

Male genitalia, 

0-8 

235 

66.4 

151 

65.6 

63 

69.9 



manual. Pre-m. 

9-12 

224 

71.0 

173 

74.0 





Frequent 

13+ 

566 

75.1 

439 

74.7 

118 

78.0 



Male genitalia, 

0-8 

457 

57.1 

74 

54.1 

252 

58.0 

131 

57.3 

manual. Mar- 

9-12 

266 

60.9 

80 

65.0 

148 

63.5 



ital. Frequent 

13 + 

1069 

75.3 

225 

84.0 

687 

' 74.5 

157 

66.2 

Female genitalia, 

0-8 

235 

8.5 

151 

9.3 

63 

7.9 



oral. Pre-mar- 

9-12 

224 

10.3 

173 

11.6 




i 

ital. Ever 

13 + 

564 

18.4 

438 

15.5 

117 

25.6 



Female genitalia. 

0-8* 

458 

4.1 

74 

5.4 

252 

4.0 

132 

3.8 

oral. Marital. 

9-12 

267 

15.4 

80 

13,7 

149 

16.8 



Ever 

13+ 

1070 

45.3 

225 

35.1 

688 

49.6 

157 

41.4 

Male genitalia, 

0-8 

235 

22.1 

151 

21.8 

63 

25.4 



oral. Pre-mar- 

9-12 

224 

29.9 

173 

27.7 

45 

37.8 



ital. Ever 

13+ 

564 

38.6 

438 

34.5 

117 

51.3 



Male genitalia, 

0-8 

458 

6.6 

74 

9.5 

252 

6.8 

132 

4.6 

oral. Marital. 

9-12 

267 

15.3 

80 

12.5 

149 

18.1 



Ever 

13 + 

1070 

42.7 

225 

38.7 

i 688 

45.5 

157 

36.3 


Table 93. Utilization of pre-coital petting techniques at three educational levels, 

in three generations 


Ages shown represent ages of subjects at time of reporting. 
368 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


369 


when there is such stimulation, it is usually restricted in its extent 
and in its duration. The lower level female agrees to manipulate 
the male genitalia in only 57 per cent of the cases. The record is, therefore, 
one of more extended pre-coital play at the upper levels, and of a mini- 
mum of play at the lower levels. Many persons at the lower level consider 
that intromission is the essential activity and the only justifiable activity 
in a “normal” sexual relation. 

Oral Eroticism. Many persons in the upper levels consider a certain 
amount of oral eroticism as natural, desirable, and a fundamental part of 
love making. Simple lip kissing is so commonly accepted that it has a 
minimum of erotic significance at this level. The college male may expect 
to kiss his date the first time they go out together. Most college students 
understand there will be good night kisses as soon as their dating becomes 
regular. Many a college male will have kissed dozens of girls, although he 
has had intercourse with none of them. On the other hand, the lower level 
male is likely to have had intercourse with hundreds of girls, but he may 
have kissed few of them. What kissing he has done has involved simple 
lip contacts, for he is likely to have a considerable distaste for the deep kiss 
which is fairly common in upper level histories. 

Deep kissing is utilized as a prime source of erotic arousal by many 
persons in the better educated and top social levels. A deep kiss may 
involve considerable tongue contacts, deep lip contacts, and extended 
explorations of the interior of the partner’s mouth. Such behavior is, as 
noted before, a regular concomitant of coital activity among many of the 
vertebrates, and particularly among the mammals (Beach 1947, and origi- 
nal observations which we have). In the human mammal, at the upper 
level, oral eroticism may still be considered a bit sophisticated, but deep 
kissing is in the experience of 87 per cent of the group (Table 93). Its 
sanitary implications seem no obstacle to its acceptance. This group 
accepts mouth contacts in its erotic play, although it objects to the use of 
a common drinking glass. 

On the other hand, the lower level male considers such oral contacts to 
be dirty, filthy, and a source of disease, although he may drink from a 
common cup which hangs in the water pail, and he may utilize common 
utensils in eating and drinking. Obviously, the arguments, at both levels, 
have nothing to do with the real issues. They are rationalizations of mores 
which place taboos upon mouth contacts for reasons which only the student 
of custom can explain. Once again, it is the upper level which first reverted, 
through a considerable sophistication, to behavior which is biologically 
natural and basic. 

Mouth-breast contact does occur at aU social levels, but it is most 
elaborately developed again in the upper social level (Table 93). Almost 



370 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 





Oral Contacts: Accumulative Incidence 






BY 

Percents of Total Population 














HOMO- 






heterosexual 



sexual 







> 

O 

> 






AGE 

CASES 





o ^ 

c/3 

C3 


o 




GROUP 




1 


^ i 

(L 









1 



Si 


o 

< 

!/) 

a 

CL 







U 


s * 

2 


0) 

O 





o 

< 

a 

s 

CL 

s 

'cS 

'u 

oi 

“si 

3 

'C 

C3 

< 

13 

13 

s 
^ ! 

13 

s 

13 

1 

o 

< 

O 

> 

*S5 

c/3 

CCS 

CL 





u 




'G 

‘G 

‘G 

S 

s 






o 

(D 



a 

a 

c3 

a 



< 

W 



& 

& 

S 


s 

S 

2 


All Educational Levels : U. S. Correction 


16-20 

1208 

46.2 

8.2 

22.8 

23.7 

7.7 

22.8 




12.0 

38.3 

21-25 

1502 

49.1 

8 41 

27.2 

26.7 

6.9 

26.5 

16.7 

12,1 

12.6 

14.1 

36.3 

26-30 

653 

55.9 

12.3 

40.0 




22.3; 

15.8 

21.3 

13.5 

36.6 

31-35 

370 

55.2 

15,3 

40.6 




19.2 

15.8 

13.8 

15.4 

32.0 

36-40 

317 

58.9 

16.7 

46.7 


i 

1 


i 


11.5 

29.8 


Educational Level 0-8 


16-20 

176 

44.9 

8.0 

22.2 

21.0 

8.0 

21.0 




15.2 

38.2 

21-25 

123 

41.5 

4.9 

22.8 

21.0 

6.5 

21.0 

8 2 

3.3 

6.6 

10.6 

30.9 

26-30 

109 

45.0 

5.5 

37.6 




9.3 

2.7 

9.3 

4.6 

33.0 

31-35 

74 

40.5 

6.8 

29.7 




10.5 

7.0 

7.0 

10.8 

27.0 

36-40 

88 

44.8 

9.1 

35.2 




10.8 

6.2 

9.2! 

5.7 

24.1 

41-45 

67 

34.3 

1.5 

28.8 





0.0 

0.0 

5.9 

13.2 

46-55 

84 

22.6 

1.2 

16.7 





3.0 

4.5 

6.0 

14.5 

564- 

77 

23.1 

5.2 

18.2 





4.5 

4.5 

6.3 

15.2 


Educational Level 9-12 


16-20 

228 

54.8! 

9.6 

26.8 

28.9 

8.8 

27.5 




12.4 

46.5 

21-25 

142 

57.0 

9.1 

30.1 

31.0 

6.9 

31.0 

14.3 

10.7 

8.9 

18.0 

45.8 

26-30 

104 

64.4 

10.6 

40.4 




21.7 

15.0 

21.7 

21.4 

45.7 

31-35 

62 

66.1 

14.8 

47.5 







21.3 

42.6 

36-40 

49 

77.6 

16.3 

61.2 







20.4 

42.9 


Educational Level 13-f 


16-20 

802 

15.8 

3.2 

8.8 

9. 

,5 

3.0 

8.7 




3, 

.6 

6.6 

21-25 

1237 

34.9 

13.5 

25.7 

22. 

,1 

8.0 

20.9 

44.1 

36.2 

39.9 

6. 

.5 

11.5 

26-30 

440 

57.0 

34.8 

44.8 

29. 

1 

12.6 

28.1 

56.9 

51.5 

50.6 

11, 

.4 

17.1 

31-35 

234 

70.5 

42.7 

55.1 





48.7 

43.5 

36.8 

13. 

,8 

18.9 

36-40 

180 

71.7 

49.4 

58.9 





54.2 

50.3 

47.7 

13, 

.7 

21.3 

41-45 

118 

74.6 

51.7 

55.1 





62.1 

55.3 

46.6 

11. 

.9 

17.8 

46-55 

122 

63.9 

43.4 

50.8 





47.2 

42.5 

36.8 

9. 

.8 

10.7 

56+ 

55 

154.5 

41,8 

40.0 





43.1 

39.2 

35.3 

9, 

.1 

12.7 


Table 94. Oral techniques at three educational levels 

Showing accumulative incidences. Data not calculated as described for accumulative 
incidence curves in Chapter 3, but derived from experience of each subject up to time of 
reporting. Lower incidences in some older age groups may be due to small size of samples 
and to possible cover-up, but most probably to the fact that incidences were actually a 
bit lower in that generation. 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


371 


invariably it is a matter of the male manipulating the female breast with 
his mouth. It is interesting that females rarely attempt to manipulate male 
breasts (Chapter 18). 

The upper level male considers it natural that the female breast should 
interest him, and that he should want to manipulate it, both by hand and 
by mouth. The biologic origin of this interest is, however, open to question, 
because many lower level males do not find the female breast similarly 
interesting and have little inchnation to manipulate it, either by hand or by 
mouth. Many lower level males rate such mouth-breast contacts as per- 
versions, and some of them dismiss the idea with considerable disgust, as 
something that only a baby does when nursing from the mother’s breast. 
Considering these opposite reactions to a single type of situation, it must 
be apparent that a considerable psychic element is involved in the develop- 
ment of individual patterns on this point. The concentration of these 
patterns in whole social levels indicates that the mores, the long-time cus- 
toms of the groups, are the fundamental factors in the picture. 

Mouth-genital contacts of some sort, with the subject as either the 
active or the passive member in the relationship, occur at some time in the 
histories of nearly 60 per cent of all males (Table 94). As noted elsewhere 
(Chapter 18), these are quite common in the sexual activity of many of the 
other mammals, particularly among the other anthropoids (Beach 1947). 
There have been some other human cultures which have accepted such 
contacts as usual behavior, and even as a part of their religious service. 
The suggestion that such techniques in our present-day society are a recent 
development among sophisticated and sexually exhausted individuals is 
curiously contrary to the specific record, for the figures for at least three 
generations do not show significant changes in this respect (Table 93). 

Mouth-genital contacts (of any kind) occur much more often at high 
school and college levels (Table 94), less often in the grade school group. 
In the histories of the college group, about 72 per cent of the males have 
at least experimented with such contacts, and about 65 per cent of the 
males who have gone into high school but not beyond. Among those males 
who have never gone beyond eighth grade in school the accumulative 
incidence figure is only 40 per cent. 

The percentages for males who have made mouth contacts with female 
genitalia prior to marriage are 9, 10, and 18 for grade school, high school, 
and college levels, respectively (Table 93). In marriage, such contacts are 
in 4, 15, and 45 per cent of the histories, for the three groups. Before mar- 
riage, the percentages of males with histories which included mouth stimu- 
lation of the male genitalia during heterosexual relations were 22, 30, and 
39, for the three educational levels. In marriage, such relations have been 
had in 7, 15, and 43 per cent of the cases, for the three levels, respectively. 



372 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HTOIAN MALE 


POSITIONS IN 

MARITAL COITUS 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

Coital Techniques and Nudity 

AT Three Educational Levels 

ALL , 

AGES 

ADOL.-25 

26-45 

46+ 

Cases 

o/ 

/o 

Cases 

% 

Cases 

% 

Cases 

0/ 

/o 

Female above; 

0-8 

457 

17.1 

74 

1 24.3 

251 

20.3 

132 

6.8 

frequent 

9-12 

265 

28.3 

80 

31.2 

148 

20.4 




13+ 

1071 

34.6 

226 

42.9 

688 

34.4 

157 

23.6 

Side; frequent 

0-8 

457 

16.4 

74 

32.4 

251 

16.4 

132 

7.6 


9-12 

263 

22.8 

80 

20.0 

147 

27.2 




13+ 

1066 

26.0 

225 

22.7 

684 

27.2 

157 

25.5 

Rear; frequent 

0-8 

456 

7.6 

74 

12.2 

250 

8.4 

132 

3.8 


9-12 

265 

11.3 

80 

7.5 

148 

14.9 




13+ 

1068 

10.6 

225 

9.8 

686 

11.1 

157 

9.6 

Sitting; frequent 

0-8 

457 

7.7 

74 

17.6 

251 

7.2 

132 

3.0 


9-12 

265 

9.4 

80 

6.2 

148 

11.5 




13+ 

1065 

6.1 

224 

6.7 

684 

6.1 

157 

5.0 

l 

Standing; frequent 

0-8 

455 

7.5 

74 

17.5 

249 

7.2 

132 

2.3 


9-12 

263 

9.1 

80 

7.5 

146 

11.0 




13+ 

1062 j 

3.6 

224 

2.6 

683 

3.6 

155 

* 4.5 

NUDITY 


In sleep ; frequent 

0-8 

724 

16.1 

271 

19.6 

296 

14.5 

157 

13.4 


9-12 

486 

34.1 

256 

34.8 

187 

34.2 1 




13 + 

2407 

41.1 

1412 

34.9 

832 

52.2 j 

163 

37.4 

In coitus. Pre- 

0-8 

436 

31.7 

198 

32.8 

181 

32.6 

57 

24.5 

marital. Fre- 

9-12 

366 

45.1 

217 

52.1 

126 

37.3 



quent 


894 

54.9 

456 

52.2 

407 

58.7 



In coitus. Marital. 

0-8 

383 

42.8 

54 

63.0 

208 

43.3 

121 

33.1 

Frequent 

9-12 

205 

66.3 

55 

81.8 

118 

67.0 




13 + 

924 

89.2 

184 

92.4 

598 

90.8 

142 

78.1 


Table 95. Coital techniques and nudity at three educational levels, in three 

generations 


The most frequently used coital position is the one in which the male is above. It is 
not shown in the table because its use does not significantly vary between educational 
levels. Ages shown represent ages of subjects at time of reporting. Consequently it may 
be expected that the incidence data for the youngest generation, although they are already 
higher than any other on most items, will go still higher before this group reaches the 
age of the oldest generation shown in the table. 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


373 


Most of the mouth-genital contacts are had between spouses. Prostitutes 
provide a portion of such contacts. However, it should be noted that most 
prostitutes are from the lower social levels, and consequently that few of 
them engage freely in oral activities. Even among those who make such 
contacts professionally, few of them would accept the same type of rela- 
tionship with their boy friends. In her private life, even the prostitute does 
not depart from the mores of her social level, although she may do any- 
thing for pay. 

Mouth-genital contacts in homosexual relations occur most commonly 
among the males of the high school level, and not quite so often in the 
males of the college and grade school groups (Table 94). Of the entire male 
population (U. S. Corrections), about 30 per cent has been brought to 
climax at least once in such relations with other males, and 14 per cent 
has brought other males to climax by the same techniques. The total of 
those who have had any type of oral relation in the homosexual is 
something over 30 per cent. 

Positions in Intercourse. Universally, at all social levels in our Anglo- 
American culture, the opinion is held that there is one coital position which 
is biologically natural, and that all others are man-devised variants which 
become perversions when regularly engaged in. However, the one position 
which might be defended as natural because it is usual throughout the 
Class Mammalia, is not the one commonly used in our culture. The usual 
mammalian position involves, of course, rear entrance, with the female 
more or less prone, face down, with her legs flexed under her body, while 
the male is above or to the rear. Among the anthropoids this mammalian 
position is still the most common, but some variety of positions also occurs 
(Bingham 1928, Yerkes and Elder 1936, Beach 1947, Nowlis ms.). 

Most persons will be surprised to learn that positions in intercourse are 
as much a product of human cultures as languages and clothing, and that 
the common English-American position is rare in some other cultures. 
Among the several thousand portrayals of human coitus in the art left by 
ancient civilizations, there is hardly a single portrayal of the English- 
American position. It will be recalled that Malinowski (1929) records the 
nearly universal use of a totally different position among the Trobrianders 
in the Southwestern Pacific; and that he notes that caricatures of the 
English- Ameiican position are performed around the communal camp- 
fires, to the great amusement of the natives who refer to the position as the 
“missionary position.” 

The origin of our present custom is involved in early and later Church 
history, and needs clarification before it can be presented with any author- 
ity; but certain it is that there was a time in the history of the Christian 
Church when the utilization of any other except the present-day position 
was made a matter for confession. What has been taken to be a question 



374 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


of biologic normality proves, once again, to be a matter of cultural develop- 
ment. 

Since this is so, it is not surprising to find that within our American cul- 
ture there is some variation in coital positions among the social levels. 
Throughout the population as a whole, a high proportion of all the inter- 
course is had in a position with the female supine, on her back, with the 
male above and facing the female. Only a part of the intercourse is had 
with the female above the male. This occurs in about 35 per cent of the 
college level histories, in 28 per cent of the high school histories, but in only 
17 per cent of the grade school histories (Table 95). At the upper level 26 
per cent of the males may use a position in which the partners lie on their 
sides, facing each other, W only 23 and 16 per cent of the high school and 
grade school males try such a technique. Rear entrance into the vagina is 
found in 1 1 per cent of college and high school histories, but in less than 
8 per cent of the grade school histories. The lower level experiments more 
often than the upper level only in sitting and standing positions, but no 
group uses these two positions very often. 

It should be emphasized that the most common variant position is the 
one with the female above. It is used, at least occasionally, by more than a 
third (34.6%) of the upper level males. The position was more nearly 
universal in Ancient Greece and Rome {vide the art objects and materials, 
as well as the literature from that period). It is shown in the oldest known 
depiction of human coitus, dating between 3200 and 3000 b.c., from the 
Ur excavations in Mesopotamia (Legrain 1936). The position with the 
female above is similarly the commonest in the ancient art of Peru, India, 
China, Japan, and other civilizations. In spite of its ancient history, many 
persons at lower social levels consider the position a considerable perver- 
sion. It is associated in their rationalizations with the idea that the female 
becomes mascuhne while the male becomes effeminate in assuming such 
a position, and that it destroys the dignity of the male and his authority 
in the family relationship. There may be a feeling that a male who accepts 
this position shows homosexual tendencies. One of the older psychiatrists 
goes so far as to insist that the assumption of such a dominating position 
by the female in coitus may lead to neurotic disturbances and, in many 
cases, to divorce. Even the scientifically trained person is inclined to use 
such rationalizations to defend his custom, 

PATTERNS OF BEHAVIOR 

Within any single social level there are, of course, considerable differ- 
ences between individuals in their choice of sexual outlets, and in the fre- 
quencies with which they engage in each type of activity. The range of 
individual variation in any level is not particularly different from the range 
of variation in each other level. Within each group, each individual pattern 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


375 


is more or less duplicated by the patterns of individuals in every one of the 
other social levels. Nevertheless, the frequencies of each type of variant 
are so different for different social levels that the means and the medians 
and the general shapes of the frequency curves for the several groups are 
perfectly distinct. Translated into everyday thinking, this means that a 
large proportion of all the individuals in any group follows patterns of 
sexual behavior which are typical of the group, and which are followed by 
only a smaller number of the individuals in other groups. 

If the mean or median frequencies for each type of sexual activity, at 
each social level, are brought together in a single chart (Figures 106, 107), 
it becomes possible to see what material differences there are in these 
patterns of behavior. Each horizontal line, followed across the chart, 
epitomizes the story for one social level. It is, as it were, a silhouette, a 
profile representing the essence of the group’s attitudes on matters of sex, 
and the translation of those attitudes into overt sexual activity. 

Even a child would comprehend that the creature represented in each of 
these silhouettes is distinct and unhke the creatures represented in the 
other silhouettes. 

It is, of course, of prime concern to ask why patterns of sexual behavior 
differ as they do in different social levels. It is of scientific importance to 
understand how such patterns originate, how they are passed on to each 
individual, and how they become standards of behavior for such a high 
proportion of all the individuals in each group. It is of equal importance 
to understand the social significances of these patterns of sexual behavior. 
Few of us have been aware that there were such differences in patterns in 
the various subdivisions of our culture. An understanding of the facts may 
contribute something toward easing the tensions that arise because indi- 
viduals and whole segments of the population fail to understand the sexual 
philosophies and the sexual behavior of groups in which they have not 
been raised. 

We do not yet understand, to the full, the origins of these diverse sexual 
philosophies ; but it will be possible to record what the thinking of each 
group is in regard to each type of activity. 

Masturbation. At lower social levels, and particularly among the older 
generations of the lowest levels, masturbation may be looked down upon 
as abnormal, a perversion, and an infantile substitute for socio-sexual con- 
tacts. Although most lower level boys masturbate during their early adoles- 
cence, many of them never have more than a few experiences or, at the most, 
regular masturbation for a short period of months or years, after which 
they rarely again depend on such self-induced outlets. Among many of 
these lower level males, masturbation stops abruptly and immediately 
after the first experiences in heterosexual coitus. The lower level boy who 



PATTERNS OF THREE EDUCATIONAL LEVELS - ADOLESCENCE - 15 



Figure 106. Patterns of sexual behavior at three educational levels, among single males 

^ For 3 age groups. Each horizontal line extending across the page summarizes the pattern for one of the educational levels. Rela- 
tive lengths of bars in each outlet show average mean frequencies for the group. The scales vary for different sources of outlet, but 
there is an approximate indication of the relative importance of each source in the total outlet. 


SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


377 


continues to draw any material portion of his sexual outlet from mastur- 
bation after his middle teens may be much ashamed of it, and he may 
become the object of community jokes and of more serious disapproval if 
his history becomes known. In many instances, these attitudes are bolstered 
by rationalizations to the effect that masturbation does physical harm; but 
the objections are in reality based on the idea that masturbation is either 
abnormal, or else an admission that one is incapable of securing hetero- 
sexual intercourse and, therefore, socially inadequate. Among some primi- 
tive peoples (e.g., Bryk 1933), there is a somewhat similar attitude toward 
masturbation — an attitude which does not involve moral evaluations as 
much as it involves amusement at the social incapacity of the individual 
who has to resort to self stimulation for his sexual outlet. The better edu- 
cated portion of the population which so largely depends upon masturba- 
tion for its pre-marital outlet, and which draws a not insignificant portion 
of its outlet from masturbation after marriage, will be surprised to learn 
what the less educated segments of the population think of one who 
masturbates instead of having intercourse. 

The upper level more or less allows masturbation as not exactly desirable 
nor exactly commendable, but not as immoral as a socio-sexual contact. 
Older generations of the upper level were not so ready to accept mastur- 
bation. As many males were involved in the older generations, but the 
frequencies were definitely lower (Chapter 1 1), and there was considerable 
moral conflict over the rightness or wrongness .of the “habit” (Chapter 14). 
Upper level males have accepted masturbation more freely within the last 
two or three decades, and today a high proportion of the teen-age boys 
of the college group frankly and openly admit this form of pre-marital 
outlet. During their years in college about 70 per cent of these males 
depend upon masturbation as their chief source of outlet. They derive 
about 66 per cent of their orgasms from this source during their college 
years. 

The upper level’s pre-marital experience leads it to include masturbation 
as a source of outlet after marriage. The coital adjustments of this group 
in marriage are frequently poor, particularly because of the low degree of 
erotic responsiveness which exists among many of the college-bred females. 
This offers some excuse for masturbation among the married males of the 
group; but their early acceptance of masturbation in their pre-marital 
histories, and their tardy acceptance of heterosexual coitus, are prime 
determinants in the marital patterns. There are few things in all human 
sexual behavior which will surprise the poorly educated groups more than 
this considerable utilization of masturbation by the college-bred male as 
an outlet after marriage. 

Petting. The social levels are furthest apart in their attitudes on petting 
and on pre-marital intercourse. The two items are related, for petting, 



378 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


SOURCES 

Sources of Orgasm: Single Males 
% OF Total Outlet 


ADOL.-15 

16-20 

21-25 

26-30 

31-35 

36-40 


Educational Level 0-8 


Masturbation 

Nocturnal emissions 

Petting to climax 

Intercourse with companions 
Intercourse with prostitutes 
Homosexual outlet 

Animal contacts 

52.26 

1.82 

1.06 

35.00 

0.97 

8.03 

0.86 

29.15 

4.83 

1.66 

50.62 

6.21 

6.85 

0.68 

20.15 

5.02 

1.23 

52.84 

12.55 

8.06 

0.15 

20.68 
6.26 
1.96 
42.71 
14.34 
14.04 1 
0.01 

24.24 

5,49 

0.68 

23.74 

18.42 

27.43 

28.95 

5.97 

0.05 

23.08 

23.35 

18.60 

Total outlet 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

Number of cases 

712 

720 

361 

159 

61 

47 

Total solitary outlets 

54.08 

33.98 

25.17 

26.94 

29.73 

34.92 

Total heterosexual outlets 

37.03 

58.49 

66.62 

59.01 

42.84 

46.48 

Total homosexual outlet 

8.03 

6.85 

8.06 

14.04 

27.43 

18.60 


Educational Level 9-12 


Masturbation 

Nocturnal emissions 

Petting to climax 

Intercourse with companions 
Intercourse with prostitutes 
Homosexual outlet 

Animal contacts 

59.09 

4.44 

1.46 

24.93 

0.44 

8.73 

0.91 

37.17 

6.33 

2.37 

39.49 

2.75 

10.81 

1.08 

29.67 

8.10 

2.77 

38.02 

4.66 

16.31 

0.47 

27.69 

7.48 

1.82 

29.75 

6.46 

25.95 

0.85 

18.48 i 
8.21 
1.35 
42.81 
10.32 
18.83 


Total outlet 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 


Number of cases 

606 

607 

263 

117 

41 


Total solitary outlets 

63.53 

43.50 

37.77 

35.17 

26.69 


Total heterosexual outlets 

26.83 

44.61 

45.45 

38,03 

54.48 


Total homosexual outlet 

8.73 

10.81 

16.31 

25.95 

18.83 



Educational Level 13+ 


Masturbation 

Nocturnal emissions 

Petting to climax 1 

Intercourse with companions 
Intercourse with prostitutes 
Homosexual outlet 

Animal contacts 

79.61 

12.15 

1.54 

2.74 

O.ll 

3.14 

0.71 

66.37 

15,65 

5.26 

9.13 

0.80 

2.43 

0.36 

53.30 

15.67 

7.50 

18.45 

1.27 

3.72 

0.09 

45.88 

11.93 

5.17 

24.97 

3.16 

8.82 

0.07 

44.28 

10.67 

4.98 

21.52 

0.65 

17.90 


Total outlet 

100.00 

100,00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 


Number of cases 

2799 

2861 

1898 

487 

87 


Total solitary outlets ^ 

91.76 

82.02 

68.97 

57.81 

54.95 


Total heterosexual outlets 

4.39 

15.19 

27.22 

33.30 

27.15 


Total homosexual outlet 

3.14 

2.43 

3.72 

8.82 

17.90 



Tftble 96. Sources of sexual outlet for single males, at three educational levels 


Showing percentages of total outlet derived by each group from each source. 






SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 379 

among males of the college level, is more or less a substitute for actual 
coitus. 

In the upper level code of sexual morality, there is nothing so important 
as the preservation of the virginity of the female and, to a somewhat lesser 
degree, the similar preservation of the virginity of the male until the time 
of marriage. The utilization of pre-marital petting at this level is fortified 
by the emphasis which the marriage manuals place upon the importance 
of pre-coital techniques in married relations ; and the younger generation 
considers that its experience before marriage may contribute something to 
the development of satisfactory marital relations. Compared with coitus, 
petting has the advantage of being accessible under conditions where 
coitus would be impossible; it provides a simpler means of achieving both 
arousal and orgasm, it makes it possible to experience orgasm while 
avoiding the possibility of a pregnancy, and, above all, it preserves one’s 
“virginity.” Whether consciously or unconsciously, petting is chosen by 
the upper level because intercourse destroys virginity and is, therefore, 
unacceptable. It is significant to note what different values are attached, 
at that level, to erotic arousal and orgasm achieved through the union of 
genitalia, and to erotic arousal and orgasm achieved through physical 
contact of other portions of the body, or even through genital contact or 
genital manipulation which does not involve actual copulation. 
There are many males in the upper level who develop a fine art of 
achieving orgasm by petting techniques which avoid intercourse. The 
youth who may have experienced orgasm scores or hundreds of times in 
petting, and who may have utilized every type of petting technique, in- 
cluding mouth-genital contacts, still has the satisfaction of knowing that 
he is still a virgin, as his level defines virginity. There are even cases of 
males who effect genital union; but because they avoid orgasm while in 
such union they persuade themselves that they are still virgins. The illogic 
of the situation emphasizes the fact that the basic issue is one of conforming 
with a code (the avoidance of pre-marital intercourse, the preservation of 
one’s virginity), which is of paramount importance in the mores of this 
social level. 

The lower educational levels see no sense in this. They have nothing 
like this strong taboo against pre-marital intercourse and, on the contrary, 
accept it as natural and inevitable and a desirable thing. Lower level 
taboos are more often turned against an avoidance of intercourse, and 
against any substitution for simple and direct coitus. Petting involves a 
considerable list of techniques which may be acceptable to the college 
group, and to some degree to the high school group, but which are quite 
taboo at lower levels (as discussed above). It is just because petting involves 
these techniques, and because it substitutes for actual intercourse, that it 
is considered a perversion by the lower level. 



PATTERNS OF THREE EDUCATIONAL LEVELS • AGE GROUP 16-20 


380 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 






E 


T3 

<U 

a 


§ 

.2 

o 

j3 

o 


<U (D 


C3 3 

C o 
o ^ 
' o 

c /5 


<L> 


e * 


2 ° 
X! ^ 


tlfl 

fl 

O 

B 

cS 


D 

Sfe 

O T3 
^ i=j 
c2^ 

S 

o ^ 

Kj C /3 , 

<=^M tJ 

m 

J5 2 ° 

SfS S 

■G O 

S 5 ^ 
E S - 

p W)d 
bXi’X P 

g-S 3 


U 

o 

03 

X 

0) 

X 


P 

g 

CO 

O 


03 

PL, 


O 


g 

£ 


p: p X 
c« o 
w G p 

»-i ^ 

p^ p 

w)cG y 
c ^ S 

-a P 
^ oj ti 
c p G 

■2 E S. 

^ &•§ 

y P P 

X ^1 

S > P 
G ^ 

.§l| 

l|‘3 

X 2 C3 

P o o 

r- X 

“Is 

s."-g 

G C X 


M 


P 


' irt C3 

r-i 

X 6 

^ 'P 

^ S 

^ Cl 
■S Ph 
6 j 0 oi 


i § 


SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


381 


In particular cases, older persons, even at upper levels, have objected 
to pre-marital petting; but individual objections do not have the force of 
long-established mores. Pre-marital intercourse is condemned by mores 
which go back hundreds and thousands of years. Such taboos are very 
different from the criticisms which lone individuals have levied against 
petting within the last few decades, and for the most part the younger 
generation has paid little attention to such criticisms. 

There is nothing in the behavior of the upper level which is more respon- 
sible than petting is for the general opinion that college students are sex- 
ually wild. The lower level has many times as much pre-marital intercourse 
as the college male has, and it is not the intercourse of the college student 
which is the source of the lower level’s criticism. It is the fact that petting 
may be engaged in for many hours without arriving at intercourse — it is 
the fact that intercourse itself is not more often accepted as a pre-marital 
outlet by the upper social level. 

Pre-marital Intercourse. With the upper educational level, the question 
of pre-marital intercourse is largely one of morals. Some of the younger 
generation find it modern to insist that they do not avoid pre-marital 
intercourse because it is wrong, but because they consider intercourse top 
precious to have with anyone except the girl that they marry, or because 
they consider that marriages work out better when there has been no pre- 
marital intercourse. To this extent the younger generation is “emanci- 
pated”; but the change in the form of its rationalizations has not affected 
its overt behavior one whit (Chapter 1 1). 

A large portion of the 85 per cent of the population which never goes to 
college accepts pre-marital intercourse as normal and natural. Most of 
this group would insist that there is no question of right or wrong involved. 
Even some lower level clergymen, of the group that has never gone beyond 
grade school or high school, may react as the rest of the community of 
which they are a part, preaching against profanity, smoking, drinking, 
gambling, and extra-marital intercourse, but considering that no moral 
issue is involved in pre-marital intercourse. So nearly universal is pre- 
marital intercourse among grade school groups that in two or three lower 
level communities in which we have worked we have been unable to find 
a solitary male who had not had sexual relations with girls by the time 
he was 16 or 17 years of age. In such a community, the occasional boy 
who has not had intercourse by that age is either physically incapacitated, 
mentally deficient, homosexual, or ear-marked for moving out of his 
community and going to college. 

Lower level males may have a certain respect for virginity, and this may 
lead them to insist (in 41 per cent of the cases) that they would not marry 
a girl who had had previous intercourse (Table 92); but this may be more 
of a profession than a matter on which they will stand when it comes to the 



382 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HOMAN MALE 


Sources of Orgasm: Married Males 
% OF Total Outlet 


SOURCES 


16-20 

21-25 

26-30 

31-35 

36-40 

41-45 

46-50 

Educational Level 0-8 

Masturbation 

2 40 

2.43 

2.44 

1.79 

1.59 

j 

1.41 

1.4 

Nocturnal emissions 

3 08 

2.79 i 

3.41 

3 03 

1.85 

2.25 

2.6 

Intercourse, marital 

79.92 

81.03 

86.15 

88 07 

88.09 

89.96 

89.9 

Interc., extra-m., comp. 

10 91 

11.62 

6.38 

5 51 

7.24 

4.84 

4.7 

Interc., extra-m., prost 

0.61 

0.80 i 

1.16 

1.46 

0.93 

1.46 

1.4 

Homosexual outlet 

3.08 

1.33 

0.46 

0.14 

0.30 

0.08 

0.0 

Total outlet 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.0 

Number of cases 

158 

324 

292 1 

186 

143 

100 

70 

Total solitary outlets 

5.48 

5.22 

5.85 

4.82 i 

3.44 

3.66 

4.0 

Total hetero. outlets 

91.44 

93.45 

93.69 

95.04 

96.26 

96.26 

96.0 

Total homo, outlet 

3.08 

1.33 

0.46 

0.14 

0.30 

0.08 

0.0 


Educational Level 9-12 


Masturbation 

2.75 

3.70 

5.05 

4 04 1 

3.15 

1.68 

2.8 

Nocturnal emissions 

2.04 

2.85 

3.22 

3.79 , 

4 48 

5.29 

2.9 

Intercourse, marital 

82.19 

81.56 

81.67 

85.18 

88.19 

89.18 

91.0 

Interc., extra-m., comp. 

9.43 

9.35 

7.61 

4.62 

1.76 

3.15 

3.1 

Interc., extra-m., prost. 

1.48 

1.49 

1.49 

0 99 

1.69 

0.70 1 

0.2 

Homosexual outlet 

2.11 

1.05 

0.96 

1.38 

0.73 

j 


Total outlet 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.0 

Number of cases 

87 

164 

135 

82 

58 

34 

24 

Total solitary outlets 

4.79 

6.55 

8 27 

7 83 

7.63 

6.97 

5.7 

Total hetero. outlets 

93 10 

92.40 

90.77 

90.79 

91.64 

93.03 

94.3 

Total homo, outlet 

2,11 

1.05 

0.96 

1.38 

0.73 




Educational Level 13+ 


Masturbation 

8.53 

8.79 

8.67 

9.28 

8.26 

9.71 

9.0 

Nocturnal emissions 

2.99 

4.65 

4.69 

5.71 

6.06 

5.87 

5.4 

Intercourse, marital 

85.41 

83.94 

82.76 

78.34 , 

74.41 

76.39 

68.5 

Interc., extra-m., comp. 

2.86 

1.86 

2.72 

5.51 

9.85 

6.07 

13.7 

Interc., extra-m., prost. 

0.05 

0.23 

0.20 

0.41 i 

0.53 

0.32 

0.4 

Homosexual outlet 

0.16 

0.53 

0.96 

0.75 

0.89 

1.64 

3.0 

Total outlet 

100.00 

100,00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.00 

100.0 

Number of cases 

46 

440 

532 

301 

189 

138 

81 

Total solitary outlets 

11.52 

13.44 

1 13.36 

14.99 

14.32 

15.58 

14.4 

Total hetero. outlets 

88.32 

86.03 

1 85.68 

84.26 

84.79 

82.78 

82.6 

Total homo, outlet 

0.16 

0.53 

0.96 

0.75 

0.89 

1.64 

3.0 


Table 97. Sources of sexual outlet for married males, at three educational levels 
Showing percentages of total outlet derived by each group from each source. 





SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


383 


actual choice of a mate. Lower level males are likely to acquire weekly or 
more than weekly frequencies in intercourse soon after they start in early 
adolescence, or at least by the middle teens. They are often highly promis- 
cuous in their choice of pre-marital partners, and there are many who have 
no interest in having intercourse with the same girl more than once. This 
strikingly parallels the promiscuity which is found among those homosexual 
males who are “oncers,” as the vernacular term puts it. Some lower level 
males may have pre-marital intercourse with several hundred or even a 
thousand or more different girls before marriage, and here their behavior 
is most different from the behavior of the college-bred males. 

Extra-marital Intercourse. In lower social levels there is a somewhat 
bitter acceptance of the idea that the male is basically promiscuous and 
that he is going to have extra-marital intercourse, whether or not his wife 
or society objects. There is some continuation of the group attitude on 
pre-marital intercourse into the realm of extra-marital intercourse, at least 
in the early years of marriage. On the other hand, the upper level male who 
has been heterosexually restrained for 10 or 15 years before marriage does 
not freely let down and start extra-marital intercourse as soon as he has 
learned to have coitus with his wife. As a matter of fact, a male who has 
been so restrained often has difficulty in working out a sexual adjustment 
with his wife, and it is doubtful whether very many of the upper level males 
would have any facility in finding extra-marital intercourse, even if they 
were to set out deliberately after it. The lower level’s extra-marital inter- 
course does cause trouble, but we do not yet understand all the factors 
which account for the fact that with advancing age there is a steady decline 
and finally a near disappearance of extra-marital intercourse from lower 
level marital histories (Chapter 7). 

The development of extra-marital intercourse in the histories of the 
older males of the upper level (Chapter 7) is done with a certain delibera- 
tion which in some cases may be acceded to and encouraged by the wife. 

Homosexual Contacts. The considerable differences which exist in the 
incidences and frequencies of the homosexual in the three educational 
levels (Table 90) would seem to indicate basic differences in attitudes to- 
ward such activity; but we are not sure that we yet understand what these 
differences are. 

The fewest objections to the homosexual are found in the very lowest of 
the social levels, in the best educated groups, and in top society. At the 
lowest social levels sex, whether it be heterosexual or homosexual, is more 
or less accepted as inevitable. The children here are the least restrained 
sexually and usually become involved in both heterosexual and homo- 
sexual activities at an early age (Chapter 5). Since this is the group in which 
pre-adolescent behavior most often carries over into adult behavior 



384 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


(Table 29), it is not surprising to find a fair number of the males at this 
level continuing both types of activity through the major portion of their 
lives. It is notable, however, that there are few individuals in this group 
who become exclusively homosexual. There are some who definitely con- 
demn the homosexual, but there are many who accept it simply as one 
more form of sex. Rarely do they interfere with other persons who are 
involved, even though they themselves may not enter into such activities. 

The acceptance of the homosexual in top educational and social levels is 
the product of a wider understanding of realities, some comprehension of 
the factors involved, and more concern over the mental qualities and social 
capacities of an individual than over anything in his sexual history. 

The highest incidences of the homosexual, however, are in the group 
which most often verbalizes its disapproval of such activity. This is in the 
group that goes into high school but never beyond in its educational career. 
These are the males who most often condemn the homosexual, most often 
ridicule and express disgust for such activity, and most often punish other 
males for their homosexuality. And yet, this is the group which has the 
largest amount of overt homosexual activity. Their involvement may be 
due to curiosity, to the fact that one may profit financially by accepting 
homosexual relations, or to the fact that one may derive a sadistic satis- 
faction from beating up the partner after orgasm has been achieved in the 
homosexual activity. In a certain segment of this group the idea is more or 
less accepted that one may uphold the heterosexual mores while “playing 
the queers,” provided one punishes them after orgasm is achieved in the 
homosexual relation. As a group these males may strenuously deny that 
their sexual contacts have anything to do with homosexuality; but the full 
and complete record indicates that many of them have stronger psychic 
reactions to other males than they care to admit. When they no longer find 
themselves being paid for such contacts, many of them begin paying other 
males for the privilege of sexual relations. 

If there are group attitudes in regard to the homosexual, they are not as 
freely discussed at most social levels. It may be that this explains why 
community thinking is not so well crystallized on this subject as it is in 
regard to other forms of sexual activity. 

SOCIAL IMPLICATIONS 

Each social level is convinced that its pattern is the best of all patterns; 
but each level rationalizes its behavior in its own way. 

The upper level rationalizes on the basis of what is right or wrong. For 
this group, all socio-sexual behavior becomes a moral issue. Morality and 
sexual morality become more or less synonymous terms. Many persons at 
this level believe that there are few types of immorality which are more 
enormous than sexual immorality. Proper, straight, upright, honorable. 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


385 


clean, fine, wholesome, manly, and pure refer primarily to abstinence from 
socio-sexual relations. Their opposites refer to participation in non-marital 
sexual relations. Honor, fidelity, and success in marriage are understood 
to involve the complete absorption of the individual’s sexual urge in coitus 
with his wife. There is nothing of which persons at this level are more afraid 
than a charge of immorality, as immorality is defined by the group. There 
is no disgrace that is jnore feared than that which may result from sexual 
scandal. Sex is so clearly a moral issue that many persons in the group 
consider it a religious obligation to impose their code upon all other seg- 
ments of the population. 

Lower social levels, on the contrary, rationalize their patterns of sexual 
behavior on the basis of what is natural or unnatural. Pre-marital inter- 
course is natural, and it is, in consequence, acceptable. Masturbation is 
not natural, nor is petting as a substitute for intercourse, nor even petting 
as a preliminary to intercourse. 

There are some individuals at lower levels who do see moral issues in 
sexual behavior, but by and large even they recognize that nature will 
triumph over morals. They may “know that intercourse is wrong,” but 
“they expect to have it anyway, because it is human and natural to have it.” 
It is not at all unusual to find middle class persons who have had inter- 
course with scores or even hundreds of girls, still insisting that they would 
never marry a girl who was not a virgin (Table 92). If the upper level male 
departs from his code and has intercourse, he is most likely to have it with 
the fiancee. His excuse is that “it is not wrong when love is involved.” 
The middle class or lower level male, on the contrary, may frankly state 
that “I didn’t think anything of her, so we had intercourse. But when I 
find the girl that I really love, I won’t touch her until I marry her.” To 
many persons in the upper level, and to some in the middle class, the moral 
issues are matters of divine revelation and mandate. As a fundamentalist 
professor of philosophy put it, “There are some things that one innately 
understands to be right or wrong, and about which there is no need for 
logical discussion.” 

For both upper and lower levels, these matters do lie deeper than logic. 
There are, in consequence, no rational arguments, no cool discussions, no 
initial presentations of data, no investigations after the fact when diverse 
sexual patterns come into conflict. Like matters of religion, the mores are 
simply accepted and defended. For many persons, the mores are even 
more implicit than religious tenets. The arguments that are produced in 
their defense are the veriest sorts of rationalizations. If they cannot be 
defended in any other way, they are accepted as products of the experience 
of the past which has culminated in the wisdom of the present custom. 

Most of the tragedies that develop out of sexual activities are products 
of this conflict between the attitudes of different social levels. Sexual activi- 



386 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


ties in themselves rarely do physical damage, but disagreements over the 
significance of sexual behavior may result in personality conflicts, a loss of 
social standing, imprisonment, disgrace, and the loss of life itself. 

In Clinical Practice. Wherever professionally trained persons try to 
direct the behavior of lower level individuals, conflicts are likely to arise 
because of these diverse sexual philosophies. Clinicians of all groups, 
including physicians, clinical psychologists, school psychologists, nurses, 
psychiatrists— particularly if they work in public clinics — have a portion 
and sometimes a major portion of their contacts with lower social levels. 
The sexual advice which the upper level clinician gives will mean most when 
it takes into account the background of the community from which the 
client comes. The upper level physician or nurse who expects the lower 
level patient to disrobe for physical examination should understand that 
he outrages the mores of the group in which the patient has been raised. 
The physician who mixes moral advice with his medical prescription should 
realize that the applicability of his advice will vary with the social level 
from which the patient comes. The woman physician in a prison may never 
become reconciled to the fact that every one of the inmates in the institution 
proves to have had coital experience before reaching the institution; but 
she must comprehend that her effectiveness as a physician is impaired 
when she proffers moral advice which has no relation to the realities of the 
world from which the inmate comes. 

Marriage counseling, as set up today, is based upon concepts of mar- 
riage, goals, and ideals which may appear right to the educational level from 
which the marriage counselors come, and from which most of the coun- 
selor’s clients also come, but which mean something else in the communi- 
ties from which a lower level client may come. The sexual techniques which 
marriage councils and marriage manuals recommend are designed to 
foster the sort of intellectual eroticism which the upper level esteems. It 
depends on prolonged pre-coital play, a considerable variety in techniques, 
a maximum of stimulation before coital union, some delay after effecting 
such union, and, finally, orgasm which is simultaneous for the male and 
the female. Most of this, however, would be anathema to a large portion 
of the population, and an outrage to their mores. Many marriage counselors 
would like to impose their own upper level patterns on their clients, with- 
out regard to the complications which may develop when an individual is 
educated into something that puts him at discord with the mores of the 
society in which he was raised and in which he may still be living. 

In industry, some of the conflicts which arise between the better educated 
management and the more poorly educated labor may depend on failures 
to comprehend the diverse sexual patterns which are involved. Trained 
persons are increasingly used for personnel staffs in industrial plants. These 
persons, however, are not always aware of the viewpoints of lower level 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


387 


groups. Personnel managers, social workers, psychologists, physicians who 
try to comprehend and accept the patterns by which these other levels live, 
might aid in establishing a better rapport between labor and management. 

In Social Service. Wherever people of different social levels come into 
contact, conflicts between sexual patterns, and failures to understand the 
patterns of other groups and the philosophies that lie back of them, pro- 
vide considerable impediments to any cooperation between the groups. 
Administrators of institutions need to understand the patterns of the com- 
munities from which their inmate populations come. This is especially 
true in penal institutions, in homes for the feeble-minded, in children’s 
homes, in homes for the aged, in hospitals, and in other institutions whose 
populations come mostly from lower social levels. Heads of boarding 
schools and of colleges are not so often concerned with this problem, 
because their populations come largely from their own social level; but 
teachers in public grade schools and in public high schools are regularly 
confronted with the problem of understanding cultures other than their 
own. The unmarried college graduate who is an eighth grade teacher will 
find it difficult to understand how her eighth grade boy, from the laborer’s 
or mechanic’s home, could be so evil as to have had intercourse with one of 
her fourteen-year old girls. Her reaction, based upon her upper level 
standards, may result in the boy’s expulsion from school, and in public 
disgrace for both the boy and the girl. The teacher does not realize that 
more than a fourth (28%) of all her other eighth grade boys have similarly 
had intercourse (Table 136). The boy who was caught might have been 
handled differently if the teacher had known more about the boy’s back- 
ground. 

Social workers are involved with sexual problems even more often than 
physicians. There are cases of pre-marital pregnancies, of rape, of divorce 
resulting from sexual conflicts between the parents of the children in whom 
the social worker is interested. There may be coitus, and sometimes inces- 
tuous relations, between the children and the adults in the community. 
These last are things that may offend the community as well as the social 
worker. But everywhere the social worker runs into a record of sexual 
contacts among children, pre-marital intercourse, and extra-marital inter- 
course; and although the community accepts these things as inevitable, the 
social worker sees the behavior in terms of her own mores, and may be 
outraged and vindictive in her reactions. She may refuse welfare allow- 
ances to a family in which there is such “delinquency.” In many cases, 
it is the welfare worker who brings the case of sexual activity to the atten- 
tion of the court. Often it is she who initiates the moves to have such 
“neglected” children taken away from their parents and made wards of 
the court, for placement in other families or in children’s homes or in 
juvenile disciplinary institutions. The untrained, less educated individual 



388 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


who enters social work, particularly in smaller communities, sometimes 
has a better understanding of the realities of these lower level groups. Some 
of the graduates of some of the better schools for social workers may also 
have some comprehension of these differences between levels. The least 
comprehending are the well intentioned, upper level women who turn to 
social work as a contribution to civic welfare. Some of the most poorly 
understood groups are in lower level Negro communities, and it takes a 
social worker who is capable of comprehending a great deal more than her 
own social level to work effectively with such a group. It is sometimes sug- 
gested that Negro communities should be handled only by Negro social 
workers; but educated, upper level Negroes may have as little compre- 
hension of a lower level Negro community’s attitudes as upper level white 
persons would have. In fact, the upper level Negro worker may be even 
more intent upon “raising” the pattern of the lower level community, in 
a move designed to bring credit to Negroes as a race. 

In the Army and Navy. Officers in the Army and Navy are faced with 
the problem of dealing with persons of diverse social levels who are brought 
together into a single, closely knit community. Since most of the popula- 
tion has not gone beyond the tenth grade in school, most of the men in 
the armed forces have lower level patterns of sexual behavior. Some of the 
officers come out of the ranks and comprehend these patterns. Profes- 
sionally trained officers who are products of West Point or Annapolis, or 
of some other special school, are more likely to come from better educated 
levels. Some of the incongruities which exist between Army and Navy 
rules and the administration of those rules are products of these differ- 
ences in the backgrounds of officers and enlisted men. American armies of 
occupation have found themselves in cultures that are different from our 
own in their attitudes on matters of sex. The upper level officer who estab- 
lishes the law for the country he is temporarily ruling may try to impose 
“moral standards” which reflect the mores of only a limited portion of our 
American population, upon the whole of a foreign people who have none 
of the sexual patterns of any of our social levels. 

During times of peace, the better educated segments of the population 
are sufficiently isolated to be unaware of the sexual patterns in the mass of 
the population. In times of war, when these upper level groups are suddenly 
thrust into close contact with these other levels, they are startled to dis- 
cover the realities of human behavior. They are inclined to blame all of 
the sexual activity which enlisted men have upon the organization of the 
Army and Navy itself. The specific data which we have indicate that very 
few of the men in the armed forces are as active sexually as they would have 
been at home in times of peace, but the upper level, especially the older 
generatioii of the upper level, is unaware of this. Considerable pressure, in 
consequence, is brought upon military officials to establish and enforce 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


389 


rules, and upon Congress to enact laws which are designed to force all 
of the heterogeneous group which constitutes a draft army into an upper 
level pattern of sexual behavior. The demand is fortified by an emphasis 
upon the dangers of venereal disease; but it is certain that many of the 
persons who discuss disease are more concerned over the morals of the 
men for whom the government has suddenly become responsible. Such an 
issue could be grasped more intelligently if more people understood the 
origins of the sexual patterns of the men in uniform. 

In Everyday Contacts. In general, the upper level feels that ‘dower level 
morality” lacks the ideals and the righteousness of the upper level philos- 
ophy. The lower level, on the other hand, feels that educated and upper 
level society has an artificial and insincere pattern of sexual behavior which 
is all the more obnoxious because the upper level tries to force its pattern 
upon all other levels. Legends about the immorality of the lower level are 
matched by legends about the perversions of the upper level. One is in- 
clined to accept the particular legends that apply to the group to which one 
does not belong. Such legends reach their maximum proportions when 
they concern whole racial or national groups: “The French do this, the 
Chinese do that.” Primitive peoples and pagans are always believed to be 
aberrant in their sexual lives. There are exaggerated legends concerning 
the Negro's sexual behavior, and Negro leaders are much disturbed over 
such popular beliefs (Cobb 1947). Sexual propaganda against the Jews as 
a race was a cornerstone of the Hitlerian attack on that group in Germany. 
Both Nazi and Japanese propaganda included attacks on the sexual 
behavior of Americans at home. There are traditions concerning the sexual 
behavior of the Italian, Spanish, Latin American, and other groups, even 
though there are no objective data to establish any generalizations. There 
are, of course, endless variations in sexual patterns in each of these popu- 
lations, just as there are in our own American population. What data we 
have so far on these other groups indicate that there is at least some 
stratification of social levels in all of them; and this would lead one to pre- 
suppose that each group would, therefore, have a variety of sexual patterns. 

In the La^v. Anglo-American sex laws are a codification of the sexual 
mores of the better educated portion of the population. While they are 
rooted in the English common law, their maintenance and defense lie 
chiefly in the hands of state legislators and judges who, for the most part, 
come from better-educated levels. 

Consequent on this fact, the written codes severely penalize all non- 
marital intercourse, whether it occurs before or after marriage; but they do 
not make masturbation a crime, even though there are a few courts which 
have tried to read such interpretations into the law (Chapter 8). 

However, the enforcement of the law is placed in the hands of police 
officials who come largely from grade school and high school segments of 



390 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


the population. For that reason, the laws against non-marital intercourse 
are rarely and only capriciously enforced, and then most often when upper 
level individuals demand such police action. It is difficult for a lower level 
policeman or detective to feel that much of a crime is being committed 
when he finds a boy and a girl involved in the sort of sexual activity which 
was part of his own adolescent history, and which he knows was in the 
histories of most of the youth in the community in which he was raised. 
If the behavior involves persons against whom the policeman has a grudge 
(probably for some totally non-sexual reason), if the relation involves too 
public an exhibition, if it involves a contact between a much older and a 
younger person (which under the policeman’s code is more or less taboo), 
if it involves a relation between persons belonging to different racial 
groups (which under his code may be exceedingly taboo), then the laws 
against pre-marital intercourse become convenient tools for punishing these 
other activities. But if it is the routine sort of relationship that the officer 
very well knows occurs regularly in the lower level community, then he 
may pay little attention to the enforcement of the laws. The policeman’s 
behavior may appear incongruous or hypocritical to the citizen from the 
other side of the town, but it is based on a comprehension of realities of 
which the other citizen is not often aware. There are policemen who frankly 
state that they consider it one of their functions to keep the judge from 
knowing things that he simply does not understand. 

On the other hand, if it is the case of a boy who is found masturbating 
in a back alley, the policeman is likely to push the case through court and 
see that the boy is sent to an institution for indecent exposure, for moral 
degeneracy, or for perversion. When the boy arrives in the reformatory, 
the small-town sheriff may send a letter urging that the administration of 
the institution pay especial attention to curing the boy of the perversion. 
However, the educated superintendent of the institution is not much im- 
pressed by the problem, and he may explain to the boy that masturbation 
does him no harm, even though the law penalizes him for his public expo- 
sure, The superintendent may let it be known among his officers that mas- 
turbation seems to him to be a more acceptable form of sexual outlet than 
the homosexual activity which involves some of the inmates of the insti- 
tution, and he may even believe that he has actually provided for the 
sexual needs of his wards by making such a ruling. On the other hand, 
the guards in the institution, who are the officials most often in contact 
with the inmates, have lower level backgrounds and lower level attitudes 
toward masturbation. In consequence, they continue to punish inmates 
who are discovered masturbating as severely as they would punish them 
for homosexual activity. 

On sex cases, the decisions of the judge on the bench are often affected 
by the mores of the group from which he originated. Judges often come 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


391 


from better educated groups, and their severe condemnation of sex offend- 
ers is largely a defense of the code of their own social level. Lower level 
individuals simply do not understand the bitter denunciations which many 
a judge heaps upon the lower level boy or girl who has been involved in 
sexual relations. They cannot see why behavior which, to them, seems 
perfectly natural and humanly inevitable should be punishable under the 
law. For them, there is no majesty in laws which are as unrealistic as the 
sex laws. Life is a maze. The sex laws and the upper level persons who 
defend them are simply hazards about which one has to learn to find his 
way. Like the rough spots in a sidewalk, or the traffic on a street, the sex 
laws are things that one learns to negotiate without getting into too much 
trouble; but that is no reason why one should not walk on sidewalks, or 
cross streets, or have sexual relations. 

The influence of the mores is strikingly shown by a study of the decisions 
which are reached by judges with different social backgrounds. There is 
still a portion of the legal profession that has not gone to college and, 
particularly where judges are elected by popular vote, there are some 
instances of judges who have originated in lower social levels and acquired 
their legal training by office apprenticeship or night school courses. The 
significance of the background becomes most apparent when two judges, 
one of upper level and one of lower level, sit in alternation on the same 
bench. The record of the upper level judge may involve convictions and 
maximum sentences in a high proportion of the sex cases, particularly 
those that involve non-marital intercourse or prostitution. The judge with 
the lower level background may convict in only a small fraction of the 
cases. The lower level community recognizes these differences between 
judges, and expresses the hope that when it is brought to trial it will come 
before the second judge, because “he understands.” The experienced 
attorney similarly sees to it that his case is set for trial when the under- 
standing judge is on the bench. Parole officers and social workers who 
investigate cases before they are decided in court may have a good deal to 
do with setting a particular case before a particular judge, in order to get 
a verdict that accords with the philosophy of their (the parole officers’) 
background. 

Judges who are ignorant of the way in which the other three-quarters of 
the population lives, naively believe that the police officials are apprehend- 
ing all of those who are involved in any material infraction of the sex laws. 
If the community has been aroused by a sex case which has involved a 
forceful rape or a death following a sexual relation, the judge may lead 
the other public officials in demanding the arrest of all sex offenders in the 
community. Newspapers goad the police, and there is likely to be a wave of 
arrests and convictions which carry maximum sentences, until the wide 
scope of the problem becomes apparent to even the most unrealistic 



392 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


official. It will be recalled that 85 per cent of the total male population has 
pre-marital intercourse (Table 136), 59 per cent has some experience in 
mouth-genital contacts (Table 94), nearly 70 per cent has relations with 
prostitutes (Table 138), something between 30 and 45 per cent has extra- 
marital intercourse (Tables 85, 111), 37 per cent has some homosexual 
experience (Table 139), 17 per cent of the farm boys have animal inter- 
course (Table 151). All of these, and still other types of sexual behavior 
(Chapter 8), are illicit activities, each performance of which is punishable as 
a crime under the law. The persons involved in these activities, taken as a 
whole, constitute more than 95 per cent of the total male population. Only 
a relatively small proportion of the males who are sent to penal institu- 
tions for sex offenses have been involved in behavior which is materially 
different from the behavior of most of the males in the population. But 
it is the total 95 per cent of the male population for which the judge, or 
board of pubhc safety, or church, or civic group demands apprehension, 
arrest, and conviction, when they call for a clean-up of the sex offenders in 
a community. It is, in fine, a proposal that 5 per cent of the population 
should support the other 95 per cent in penal institutions. The only possible 
defense of the proposal is the fact that the judge, the civic leader, and most 
of the others who make such suggestions, come from that segment of the 
population which is most restrained on nearly all types of sexual behavior, 
and they simply do not understand how the rest of the population actually 
lives. 

The penalties visited upon persons who are convicted of sex offense may 
be peculiarly severe, just because the judge does not comprehend the lower 
level background of the offender. The judge may give a long sentence 
because he beheves that such a stay in prison will reform the ways of the 
particular individual who is being punished; but again he fails to under- 
stand the deep origins of sexual behavior. Data which we have on more 
than 1200 persons who have been convicted of sex offenses indicate that 
there are very few who modify their sexual patterns as a result of their 
contacts with the law, or, indeed, as a result of anything that happens to 
them after they have passed their middle teens. This is not because con- 
victed sex offenders are peculiarly degenerate or different from the mass 
of the population. It is simply because all persons have their sexual pat- 
terns laid down for them by the custom of the communities in which they 
are raised. 

The sex offender is a marked individual in the penal institution to which 
he is sent. He is lectured on the heinous nature of his crime by the prison 
official who receives him, even though in many cases he has not been in- 
volved in sex behavior which is fundamentally different from that of the 
institutional official himself. There is a mystery connected with the nature 
of the specific sexual activity for which a sex offender is convicted, and 
this brings emotional reactions from all persons concerned. 



SOCIAL LEVEL AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


393 


When it comes to a question of releasing sex offenders, parole boards 
are loath to take action. The inmate is judged by the standards of the 
upper level community from which most parole board members come. 
Women on prison boards are especially likely to come from a social level 
where the loss of virginity before marriage is an unforgivable moral 
offense. The girl whose future they are deciding comes from a community 
where three-fourths of the girls have intercourse before marriage. Persons 
who attempt to control the behavior of other persons might more properly 
be concerned with determining the extent of the departure of the individual 
from the behavior of the community of which he is a part. 

Conflicts between social levels are as intense as the conflicts between 
nations, between cultures, between races, and between the most extreme 
of the religious groups. The existence of the conflict between sexual pat- 
terns is, however, not recognized by the parties immediately concerned, 
because neither of them understands the diversity of patterns which exist 
at different social levels. Each thinks that he is in a conflict with a particular 
individual. He is, however, more often in conflict with a whole culture. 

SOCIAL LEVELS AMONG NEGROES 

As already explained, we do not yet have enough histories of Negroes 
to warrant their inclusion in the analyses that have been made in the pres- 
ent volume. Any fair comparison of Negroes and whites will have to be 
made for groups that are homogeneous in regard to age, education, social 
level, religious background, and still other factors. It is impossible to 
generalize concerning the behavior of a whole race. Analyses of any com- 
plex population, to be scientific, must be confined to particular segments 
of that population. Preliminary findings show that there are as many 
patterns of behavior among Negroes of different social levels as there are 
among whites. It is already clear that Negro and white patterns for com- 
parable social levels are close if not identical. Since erroneous conclusions 
to the contrary have been drawn by certain persons who have seen some 
of our data prior to publication, it is important to emphasize here that 
final generalizations will be warranted only after a sufficient body of his- 
tories has been obtained at each and every social level among Negro 
groups. 



Chapter 11 

STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


Before going further in discussing the stability of patterns of sexual 
behavior, it should be emphasized again (as in Chapter 10) that there is, 
inevitably, a considerable variation among the individuals in any social 
group. This variation involves the frequencies of total sexual outlet, the 
choice of activities in which each individual may engage, and his frequen- 
cies in each type of activity. There is similar variation in attitudes on all 
other matters of sex. 

The frequency curves in Chapters 14 to 21) show how far individ- 
uals in any particular educational level or occupational class may depart 
from the averages which are the bases for most of the discussions in the 
present chapters on social levels. These same curves, however, show that 
80 to 85 per cent of each population is likely to lie within an area close to 
the calculated means or medians. This is true for each of the outlets in- 
volved; but if an individual is rather far removed from the average in 
regard to any one outlet, he is still likely to fit the generalizations made for 
his group for most of the other outlets. He is much less likely to depart 
from the pattern of his social group in regard to each and all of the indi- 
vidual outlets. 

How often individuals conform in every regard, how often they depart 
in some respects, how often they are in discord with their social group on 
all items, are matters which we shall have to follow up with more precise 
calculations at some later date. For the present we shall have to be satis- 
fied with comparisons of such general pictures as may be recognized for 
whole groups, through calculations of means, medians, incidences, and 
other statistics. 


PATTERNS IN SUCCESSIVE GENERATIONS 

In order to ascertain how much stability there may be in patterns of 
sexual behavior, and something of the changes that may occur in patterns 
within the lives of individuals and between successive generations of indi- 
viduals, we have made two sorts of calculations for the present chapter. 
The first has involved a comparison of the incidences and average frequen- 
cies in two generations of the same educational level. The second has con- 
cerned the patterns of behavior of individuals, compared with the patterns 
of behavior in the occupational classes to which their parents belonged 

394 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 395 

and in which these individuals have either stayed or from which they have 
moved into some other occupational class. 

Many persons, of course, believe that patterns of sexual behavior have 
changed considerably within the last generation or two. Some persons seem 
to find a masochistic satisfaction in believing that the world is continu- 
ously becoming more evil The upper level critic believes that the younger 
generation is having an increasing amount of pre-marital intercourse, that 
fewer men are faithful to their wives, that promiscuous petting, embodying 
mysteries which older persons do not well understand, is lowering stand- 
ards among youth, and that more unmentionable things are steadily on 
the increase (e,g., Cooper 1939, Rice 1946, McPartland 1947). The lower 
level critic has fewer opportunities to voice his opinions in print, but he is 
as certain that young people are sexually more precocious, and (above all) 
he is convinced that the younger generation of the better educated segment 
of the population is becoming perverted beyond all previous imagining. 
As already indicated (Chapter 10), “perversion,” to this lower level, refers 
to masturbation in an adult, involvement in petting, oral eroticism, the 
use of variety in coital positions, and the homosexual 

If it were not for the fact that there have been similar Cassandras 
throughout the history of the world, one could almost be persuaded to 
believe that these persons possessed scientifically adequate data on the 
sexual behavior of previous generations, which they have been able to 
compare with equally adequate data on the behavior of the present genera- 
tion. There is, certainly, a considerable amount of sexual literature which 
has been kept carefully concealed from laymen and scientists alike; but 
it is also to be emphasized that nine years of research has failed to disclose 
statistically sound data which would justify any objective comparison of 
the behavior of any previous generation with the present one. One is 
inclined to suspect that the amazement of the older generation at the pres- 
ent-day behavior is dependent, at least in part, upon the fact that the older 
generation knew very little about the behavior of the world in which it 
lived when it was young, and that it has only more recently become 
acquainted with the long-established facts of life. Certainly it becomes 
highly desirable to confine any statements about trends in sexual behavior 
to such generalizations as can be established by statistically adequate data. 

We have, therefore, undertaken to make a precise comparison of the 
older and younger generations which have contributed to the present 
study. We have divided the entire male sample into two groups of more or 
less equal size. One group has included all of those persons who were 33 
years of age or older at the time they contributed their histories. Its median 
age is 43.1 years. The other group has included all the cases of persons who 
were younger than 33 at the time of contributing. Its median age is 21.2 
years. The difference between the median ages of the two groups is about 



396 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


22 years. The older group represents the generation that was in its youth 
and therefore sexually most active from 1910 to 1925. These are the indi- 
viduals who fought World War I and were responsible for the reputation 
of the “roaring twenties.” It is quite generally believed by many people 
that this was a period of such sexual laxity as America is supposed never 
to have known before (Allen 1931). Nevertheless, this is the generation 
that is quite convinced that youth today is still wilder in its behavior. Most 
of the younger group used in the present calculation was at its peak of ac- 
tivity between 1930 and the present time. 

A more precise comparison would involve a successive breakdown of the 
population according to the year in which each individual was born, and 
a grouping into generations separated by twenty-year spans. This finer 
analysis is not possible with the number of histories now at hand, but 
should be undertaken as this project expands. 

Comparisons of Accumulative Incidences. Tables 98 to 103 and Figures 
108 to 121 compare the accumulative incidence data for the older and 
younger segments of the population in regard to masturbation, nocturnal 
emissions, pre-marital petting experience, petting to the point of orgasm, 
total pre-marital intercourse, pre-marital intercourse with prostitutes, 
extra-marital intercourse, and the homosexual. Tables 104-105 and Fig- 
ures 122-123 show the average frequencies and the active incidences in 
comparable age periods and at comparable social levels of the older and 
the younger segments of the population. 

An examination of the accumulative incidence curves (Figures 108-114) 
will show that the number of persons ultimately involved, and the ages at 
which they became involved, are almost exactly the same for the older 
and the younger generations in the following groups and for the following 
types of behavior: 

College level — masturbation 
College level — nocturnal emissions 
College level — heterosexual intercourse 
College level — ^total pre-marital intercourse 
College level — intercourse with prostitutes 
Grade school level — intercourse with prostitutes 
College level — homosexual outlet 

Comparisons of the accumulative incidence curves (Figures 115, 116) 
indicate that the same number of persons is ultimately involved in the 
two generations, but that the younger generation appears to become active 
a year or two earlier, in the following cases : 

Grade school level — heterosexual intercourse 
. Grade school level — ^pre-marital intercourse 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


397 


Comparisons of the accumulative incidence curves (Figures 117-121) 
indicate that more individuals of the younger generation are involved, and 
that these individuals begin their activity at an earlier age, in the following 
cases : 

College level — petting experience 
College level — petting to climax 
Grade school level — masturbation 
Grade school level — nocturnal emissions 
Grade school level — petting experience 

In general, the sexual patterns of the younger generation are so nearly 
identical with the sexual patterns of the older generation in regard to so 
many types of sexual activity that there seems to be no sound basis for the 
widespread opinion that the younger generation has become more active 
in its socio-sexual contacts. The only instances in which a larger number of 
the younger generation is involved at an earlier age apply to such activities 
(masturbation, nocturnal emissions, and petting) as are not ordinarily 
considered when the charge is made that the younger generation is becom- 
ing increasingly immoral. The charge more often concerns pre-marital 
intercourse with companions and with prostitutes, and homosexual con- 
tacts. On all of these latter points, however, the records for the older and 
the younger generation are, by the admission of the older generation when 
it contributes its own histories, so nearly identical that no significant 
differences can be found in the accumulative incidence curves. And as for 
the homosexual, if a larger number of the younger generation is becoming 
involved, we have failed to find any evidence of it. These questions are of 
such social significance that it is high time that scientific data replace the 
loose statements and easy conclusions drawn by persons who find some 
sort of advantage in bewailing the ways of the world. 

It is notable that in those instances where the younger generation seems 
to become involved at an earlier age, it is the lower educational level that 
is concerned, and this is probably the product of the better sanitation, 
better medical care, and better standards of nutrition which have brought 
improvements in the general health of that group within the last thirty 
years. It will be recalled (Chapter 5) that there is evidence that the younger 
generation of the lower social level is becoming adolescent a year or so 
sooner than the boys of the same level a generation or two ago. There is 
no evidence that the better educated portion of the population is becoming 
adolescent any earlier, probably because the upper level was not so poorly 
nourished in the past. 

Finally, it should be emphasized that the younger generation has materi- 
ally modified its behavior only in respect to items (masturbation and pet- 
ting) which were first accepted by the upper social levels, whose attitudes 



398 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Accumulative Incidence, Two Generations 
Educational Level 13 + 



masturbation 

nocturnal emissions 

AGE 



1 




1 



Older 

Generation 

Younger 

Generation 

Older 

Generation 

Younger 

Generation 


Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

382 

0.0 

2433 

0.0 

381 

0.0 

2430 

0.0 

9 

382 

0.5 

2433 

0.3 

381 

0.0 

2430 

0,0 

10 

382 

2.9 

2433 

2.2 

381 

0.0 

2430 

0.6 

11 

382 

9.9 

2433 

8.8 

381 

3.9 

2430 

3.0 

12 

382 

22.8 

2433 

28.6 

381 

12.9 

2430 

11.4 

13 

382 

47.9 

2433 

53.7 

381 

32.5 

2430 

28.6 

14 

382 

72.5 

2433 

72.2 

381 

57.7 

2430 

51.3 

15 

382 

78.5 

2433 

80.5 

381 

73.5 

2430 

68.2 

16 

382 

83.2 

2432 

84.4 

381 

85.3 

2429 

80.0 

17 

382 

85.6 

2430 

87.2 

381 

89.5 

2427 

86.7 

18 

382 

88.0 

2354 

89.1 

.381 

91.6 

2351 

90.9 

19 

382 

88.7 

2190 

90.3 

381 

92.7 

2187 

92.6 

20 

382 

90.6 

1955 

91.2 

381 

94.2 

1952 

93.5 

21 

382 

91.1 

1649 

92.2 

381 

95.3 

1646 

94.3 

22 

382 

92.1 

1288 

93.0 

381 

95.8 

1287 

94.4 

23 

382 

92.7 

1014 

93.6 

381 

96.1 

1014 

94.4 

24 

382 

92.7 

769 

93.4 

381 

96.9 

769 

95.2 

25 

382 

93.2 

620 

94.4 

381 

97.9 

620 

95.2 

26 

382 

93.7 

502 

95.8 

381 

97.9 

502 

95.4 

27 

382 

94.5 

392 

96.2 

381 

97.9 

392 

94.6 

28 

382 

94.8 

317 

95.9 

381 

97.9 

317 

95.0 

29 

382 

94.8 

252 

95.2 

381 

97.9 

252 

95.2 

30 

382 

95.3 

191 

96.3 

381 

98.4 

191 

94.8 

31 

382 

95.3 

147 

95.2 

381 

98.4 

147 

94.6 

32 

‘ 382 

95.5 

110 

95.5 

381 

98.4 

110 

94.5 

33 

382 

95.5 

66 

93.9 

381 

98.4 

66 

92.4 


Table 98. Comparisons of older and younger generations of the college level: 
masturbation, and nocturnal emissions 

Accumulative incidence data based on the life span. Median difference of age between 
the two generations is 22 years. 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


399 


seem to have infiltrated into the younger generation of the lower level 
today. There is considerable evidence at many other points that ideas and 
attitudes may be modified long before there are differences in overt be- 
havior, and especially in overt socio-sexual contacts. 




Figures 108-109. Comparisons of accumulative incidence for older and younger 
generations of college level : masturbation, nocturnal emissions 

Showing percentage of males with experience at any time in the life-span. Median age 
difference between the two generations is 22 years. 

Comparisons of Frequencies, In regard to frequencies in each of these 
types of sexual activity, the comparisons of the two generations are more 
complex. An examination of Table 104 and Figure 122 leads to the follow- 
ing generalizations : 

1. TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET. The frequencies of total outlet are very close 
for the college level of the population among single males at every age 

u 




400 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Accumulative Incidence, Two Generations 
Educational Level 13 + 



TOTAL INTERCOURSE 

PRE-MARITAL 

INTERCOURSE 

age 



1 




1 



Older 

Generation 

Younger 

Generation 

Older 

Generation 

Younger 

Generation 


Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

382 

0.0 

2435 

0.0 

382 

0.0 

2435 

0.0 

9 

382 

0.0 

2435 

0.0 

382 

0.0 

2435 

0.0 

10 

382 

0.0 

2435 

0.0 

382 

0.0 

2435 

0.0 

11 

382 

0.0 

2435 

0.2 

382 

0.0 

2435 

0.3 

12 

382 

0.5 

2345 

1.1 

382 

0.5 

2435 

1.1 

13 

382 

3.7 

2435 

3.0 

382 

3.7 

2435 

3.0 

14 

382 

6.3 

2435 

5.9 

382 

6.3 

2435 

5.9 

15 

382 

9.4 

2435 

9.5 

382 

9.4 

2435 

9.5 

16 

382 

14.9 

2434 

15.6 

382 

14.9 

2434 

15.6 

17 

382 

21.5 

2432 

23.4 

381 

21.3 

2431 

23.4 

18 

382 

27.2 

2356 

31.5 

381 

27.0 

2353 

31.4 

19 

382 

32.5 

2192 

39.3 

380 

31.8 

2185 

39.0 

20 

382 

40.1 

1957 

46.8 

376 

38.8 

1932 

45.4 

21 

382 

44.5 

1651 

52.4 

369 

42.5 

1611 

50.6 

22 

382 

52.4 

1290 

60.0 

363 

48.2 

1218 

55.8 

23 

382 

58.9 

1015 

64.5 

342 

52.9 

907 

58.4 

24 

382 

66.0 

770 

70.3 

320 

55.3 

647 

61.2 

25 

382 

72.8 

620 

76.5 

292 

62.0 

469 

65.9 

26 

382 

79.3 

502 

80.7 

262 

63.0 

341 

66.9 

27 

382 

82.5 

392 

82.7 

214 

64.0 

218 

66.1 

28 

382 

84.8 

317 

85.5 

185 

65.9 

144 

67.4 

29 

382 

87.4 

252 

86.5 

164 

67.7 

96 

66.7 

30 

382 

89.3 

191 

90.1 

142 

69.7 

60 

63.3 

31 

382 

90.8 

147 

91.2 

123 

69.1 



32 

382 

91.9 

110 

89.1 

106 

69.8 



33 

382 

92.1 

66 

84.8 

96 

69.8 




Table 99. Comparisons of older and younger generations of the college level: 
total intercourse, and total pre-marital intercourse 

Accumulative incidence data. Pre-marital intercourse based on single males, including 
intercourse with companions and with prostitutes. Total intercourse based on life span, 
including pre-marital, marital, extra-marital, and post-marital relations with companions 
and with prostitutes. Median difference of age between the two generations is 22 years. 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


401 


between adolescence and thirty, and among married males of the college 
level between the ages of 21 and 30 (which is as far as the sujBScient data 
go). 

Among single males of the lower educational levels, the frequencies of 
total outlet are rather materially higher for the younger generation at 



AGE 



Figures 110-111. Comparisons of accumulative incidence for older and younger 
generations of college level; total and pre-marital intercourse 

Data for total intercourse include all coital experience, regardless of marital status' 
Median age dijfference between the two generations is 22 years. 

every age between adolescence and 30; and they similarly are higher for 
the married males of the younger generation at all ages for which there 
are sufficient data. The increase in the reported frequency is greatest for 
the grade school group, and not so great for the high school group. It is 
possible that these data represent a reality— that the younger generation 




402 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Accumulative Incidence, Two Generations 


AGE 

HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 
EDUC. LEVEL 13-f- 

INTERC. WITH PROSTITUTES 
EDUC. LEVEL 13-f 

INTERC. WITH PROSTITUTES 
EDUC. LEVEL 0-8 

Older 

Gener. 

Younger 

Generation 

Older 

Gener. 

Younger 

Generation 

Older 

Gener. 

Younger 

Generation 


% with 

Cases 

% with 

%with 

Cases 

% with 

% with 

Cases 

% with 


Exper. 

Exper. 

Exper. 

Exper. 

Exper. 

Exper. 

8 

0.0 

2435 

0.0 

0.0 

2434 

0.0 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

9 

0.3 

2435 

0.0 

0.0 

2434 

0.0 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

10 

1.3 

2435 

0.3 

0.0 

2434 

0.0 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

11 

3.7 

2435 

1.6 

0.0 

2434 

0.0 

0.0 

475 

0.2 

12 

7.6 

2435 

6.0 

0.0 

2434 

0.0 

0.6 

475 

0.6 

13 

12.6 

2435 

11.5 

0.3 

2434 

0.1 

0.9 

475 

1.1 

14 

19.9 

2435 

17.7 

0.5 

2434 

0.8 

4.0 

472 

3.2 

15 

23.3 

2435 

20.8 

1.6 

2434 

2.4 

7.7 

466 

7.5 

16 

24.3 

2434 

22.8 

4.2 

2433 

4.9 

19.4 

447 

15.9 

17 

25.4 

2432 

23.9 

9.7 

2431 

9.0 

30.6 

405 

25.9 

18 

27.2 

2356 

25.3 

13.1 

2355 

13.7 

42.6 

373 

38.3 

19 

28.5 

2192 

26.4 

15.2 

2191 

17.8 

47.5 

333 

42.9 

20 

29.3 

1957 

27.3 

18.3 

1956 

21,0 

51.9 

299 

47.8 

21 

29.8 

1651 

28.3 

21.2 

1650 

22.5 

56.8 

274 

50.4 

22 

30.9 

1290 

29.5 

24.9 

1290 

24.9 

59.3 

243 

55.6 

23 

31.4 

1015 

31.5 

25.7 

1015 

25.7 

61.4 

222 

55.4 

24 

32.2 

770 

32.1 

27.0 

770 

26.5 

62.3 

198 

60.1 

25 

32.7 

620 

33.2 

28.3 

620 

28.9 

65.7 

173 

63.6 

26 

32.7 

502 

33.1 

29.1 

502 

29.7 

66.4 

158 

67.1 

27 

32.7 

392 

34.7 

.29.3 

392 

31.6 

67.0 

141 

68.1 

28 

33.0 

317 

35,0 

30.6 

317 

33.8 

67.3 

126 

68.3 

29 

33.0 

252 

34.5 

31.2 

252 

33.3 

67.9 

100 

66.0 

30 

33.8 

191 

33.5 

32.2 

191 

35.1 

70.7 

80 

66.3 

31 

33.8 

147 

35.4 

32.2 

147 

38.8 

71.3 

58 

63.8 

32 

33.8 

no 

30.0 

32.7 

110 

37.3 

71.3 



33 

33.8 

66 

34.8 

33.0 

66 

34.8 

71.6 




Table 100. Comparisons of older and younger generations : homosexual outlet, 
and intercourse with prostitutes 

Accumulative incidence data based on total life span, including pre-marital, extra- 
marital, and post-marital contacts. Median difference in age between the two 
generations is 22 years. In the older generation, for the homosexual data and for the 
data on intercourse with prostitutes at “Educ. Level 13-}-'’ there are 382 cases for each 
and every age. In the older generation for intercourse with prostitutes at “Educ. Level 
0-8” there are 324 cases. 




AGE 



AGE 


Figures 112-114. Comparisons of accumulative incidence for older and younger 
generations : homosexual outlet, intercourse with prostitutes 


All curves based on total life span, irrespective of marital status. The first two figures, 
112 and 113, show the homosexual outlet and intercourse with prostitutes for males of 
the college level (13-1-). Figure 114 shows intercourse with prostitutes for males of the 
grade school level (0-8). 


403 




404 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Accumulative Incidence, Two Generations 
Educational Level 0-8 


TOTAL INTERCOURSE 


PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


AGE 

Older 

Generation 

Younger 

Generation 

Older 

Generation 

Younger 

Generation 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

324 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

322 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

9 

324 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

322 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

10 

324 

0.3 

476 

0.0 

322 

0.3 

476 

0.0 

11 

324 

0.9 

475 

1.5 

322 

0.9 

475 

1.5 

12 

324 

2.5 

475 

10.3 

322 

2.5 

475 

10.1 

13 

324 

6.2 

475 

21.1 

322 

6.2 

474 

20.9 

14 

324 

20.7 

472 

35.0 

322 

20.8 

471 

34.8 

15 j 

324 

34.3 i 

466 

51.5 

322 

34.5 

464 

51.1 

16 

324 

48.5 

447 

67.1 

322 

48.1 

443 

66.8 

17 

324 

60.8 

405 

76.3 

321 

60.4 

394 

75.1 

18 

324 

74.7 

373 

83.9 

315 

73.3 

343 

81.9 

19 

324 

79.6 

333 

87.7 

296 

77.7 

287 

85.4 

20 

324 

84.9 

299 

89.6 

275 

82.2 

231 

86.6 

21 

324 

88.0 

274 

90.5 

252 

83.7 

192 

85.9 

22 

324 

91.4 

243 

91.8 

215 

83.3 

142 

85.9 

23 

324 

92.6 

222 

92.3 

177 

85.9 

115 

85.2 

24 

324 

94.4 

198 

95.5 

161 

86.3 

94 

89.4 

25 

324 

96.0 

173 

95.4 

144 

90.3 

72 

88.9 

26 

324 

96.9 

158 

96.2 

122 

91.8 

62 

88.7 

27 

324 

97.8 

141 

96.5 

112 

92.0 

54 

90.7 

28 

324 

98.1 

126 

96.0 

98 

93.9 



29 

324 

98.1 

100 

' 95.0 

86 

93.0 



30 

324 

98.5 

80 

95.0 

80 

93.8 



31 

324 

98.8 

58 

93.1 

73 

94.5 



32 

324 

98.8 



71 

94.4 



33 

324 

98.8 



67 

94.0 



34 

324 

98.8 



67 

94.0 




Table 101. Comparisons of older and younger generations of grade school level: 
total intercourse, and total pre-marital intercourse 

Accumulative incidence data for pre-marital intercourse based on single males, in- 
cluding intercourse with companions and with prostitutes. Data for total intercourse 
based on life span, including pre-marital, marital, extra-marital, and post-marital rela- 
tions with companions and with prostitutes. 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


405 


of the lower level is actually more active, again because of its improved 
nutrition. On the other hand, it is to be noted that older individuals of these 
lower educational levels, especially of the grade school group, are often in 
very poor condition physically and jnentally by the time they reach 45 or 
50 years of age, and their reports of past events are not as reliable as those 




AGE 


Figures 115-116. Comparisons of accumulative incidence for older and younger 
generations of grade school level : total and pre-marital intercourse 

The first figure, 1 15, is based on the total male population, irrespective of the marital 
status, and shows the percentage of males with any coital experience. Figure 116 shows 
the percentage of single males with experience in pre-marital intercourse. 

of the teen-age boys. Again, it is not impossible that there is more cover- 
up among older males of a generation in which the social pretense appears 
to have departed further from the actual behavior than it does today. It 
will take a larger sample than we yet have to enable final analysis to be 
made of these data. 





406 


3EXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Accumulative Incidence, Two Generations 
Educational Level 13 + 



1 TOTAL petting EXPERIENCE 

i . _ 

PETTING TO CLIMAX 

age 

Older 

Generation 

Younger 

Generation 

Older 

Generation 

Younger 

Generation 


Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 


381 

0.0 

2433 

0.0 

306 

0.0 

1290 

0.0 

9 

381 

0.0 

2433 

0.0 

306 

0.0 

1290 

0.0 

10 

381 

0.3 

2433 

0.3 

306 

0.0 

1290 

0.0 

11 

381 

i 0.8 

2433 

1.4 

306 

0.0 

1290 

0.0 

12 

381 

3.7 

2433 

6.1 

306 

0.7 

1290 

0.2 . 

13 

381 

12,9 

2433 

17.6 

306 

2.9 

1290 

1.3 

14 

381 

25.5 

2433 

35.0 

306 

4.2 

1290 j 

4.0 

15 

381 

36.5 

2433 

48.9 

306 

6.9 

1290 1 

8.4 

16 

381 

47.8 

2432 

66.7 

306 

13.4 

1290 

16.0 

17 

380 

55.8 

2429 

78.4 

305 

19.3 

1288 

23.9 

18 

380 

64.7 

2351 

85.8 

305 

24.6 

1229 

32.8 

19 

379 

71.8 

2183 

90.4 

305 

31.5 

1169 

40.2 

20 

375 

75.7 

1930 

92.7 

304 

37.2 

1085 

48.5 

21 

368 

77.4 

1609 

93.7 

299 

41.5 

941 

52.8 

22 

362 

80.4 

1217 

94.3 

294 

44.2 

753 

56.2 

23 

341 

83.3 

906 

94.2 

281 

44.8 

591 i 

59.2 

24 

319 

86.5 

646 

94.3 

264 

48.1 

424 

61.3 

25 

291 

86.9 

468 

95.7 

240 

53.3 

303 

62.0 

26 

261 

88.9 

340 

95.3 

216 

56.0 

221 

65.2 

27 

213 

87.3 

217 

94.9 

177 

55.9 

144 

57.6 

28 

184 

86.4 

144 

93.1 

153 

56.9 

102 

55.9 

29 

163 

87.1 

96 

93.8 

137 

59.9 

67 

58.2 

30 

141 

87.2 

60 

93.3 

115 

59.1 



31 

123 

86.2 



99 

59.6 



32 

106 

84.9 

i 


86 

58.1 



33 

96 

83.3 



80 

57.5 




Table 102. Comparisons of older and younger generations of college level: total 
petting experience, and petting to climax 


Accumulative incidence data based on single males. 

2. MASTURBATION. Among males of the college level, and among all 
but the youngest group of high school males, frequencies of masturbation 
in the two generations are, again, close. However, for the younger genera- 
tions of the lower educational levels they are distinctly higher. There 
seems in actuahty to be a greater utilization of masturbation in these lower 
levels today. 

3. NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS. The frequencies of nocturnal emissions as 
recorded by both upper and lower educational groups are nearly identical 


STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


407 




Figures 117-118. Comparisons of accumulative incidence for older and younger 
generations of college level : petting 

The first figure, 117, shows the data for any kind of petting experience. Figure 118 
shows petting experience to the point of orgasm. Median age difference between the two 
generations is 22 years. 

in every age group. If one notes that the older group had to recall the events 
of a period which extended, on an average, over 22 years more than the 
younger groups were recalling, it is all the more impressive to secure such 
similar results. 

4. PETTING. Petting to climax shows a slight increase in frequency for 
all social levels in the younger age groups, when the calculation is based 
upon the total populations in each group. The frequencies are still more 
nearly identical when the calculations are based upon the persons who are 
actively involved. 




408 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Accumulative Incidence, Two Generations 
Educational Level 0-8 


age 

MASTURBATION 

NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 

PETTING EXPERIENCE 

Older 

Gener. 

Younger 

Generation 

Older 

Gener. 

Younger 

Generation 

Older I 

Generation 

Younger 

Generation 


% with 

Cases 

% with 

1 % with 

Cases 

%with 

Cases 

% with 

Cases 

% with 


Exper. 

Exper. 

Exper. 

Exper. 


Exper. 


Exper. 

8 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

320 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

9 

0.0 

476 

0.2 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

320 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

10 

0.0 

476 

1.5 

0.0 

476 

0.0 

320 

0.3 

476 

0.0 

11 

1.2 

475 

4.6 

0.0 

475 

0.6 

320 

0.9 

475 

1.7 

12 

8.7 

475 

20.4 

0.9 

475 

1.3 

320 

3,4 

475 

10.7 

13 

23.0 

475 

44.0 

1.6 

475 

5.1 

320 

8.4 

474 

27.0 

14 

49.7 

472 

70.1 

9.7 

472 

13.8 

320 

25.3 

471 

45.9 

15 

67.4 

466 

86.7 

23.2 

466 

28.3 

320 

40.3 

464 

62.7 

16 

77.3 

447 

91.9 

35.1 

447 

42.7 

320 

54.4 

443 

75.2 

17 

80.7 

405 

93.1 

40.8 

405 

52.8 

319 

64.6 

394 

81.0 

18 

84.2 

373 

94.9 

50.8 

373 

61.7 

314 

72.6 

343 

85.4 

19 

85.1 

333 

94.6 

53.3 

333 

66.1 

295 

74.9 

287 

87.5 

20 

85.4 

299 

95.0 

58.6 

299 

70.9 

274 

78.8 

231 

86.1 

21 

85.4 

274 

95.3 

59.9 

274 

73.4 

251 

79.3 

192 

88.0 

22 

85.4 

243 

95.5 

61.4 

243 

77.0 

214 

78.5 

142 

88.7 

23 

85.4 

222 

95.9 

62.4 

222 

79.7 

176 

77.8 

115 

87.8 

24 

85.7 

198 

96.5 

63.3 

198 

82.8 

160 

78.8 

94 

90.4 

25 

86.0 

173 

96.5 

65.8 

173 

83.2 

143 

80.4 

72 

90.3 

26 

86.0 

158 

96.2 

66.8 

158 

84.2 

121 

78.5 

62 

88.7 

27 

86.0 

141 

96.5 

67.4 

141 

85.1 

111 

77.5 

54 

88.9 

28 

86.0 

126 

96.0 

69.3 

126 

83.3 

98 

77.6 



29 

86.3 

100 

95.0 

70.2 

100 

86.0 

86 

75.6 



30 

86.3 

80 

95.0 

72.1 

80 

86.3 

80 

76.3 



31 

86.3 

58 

93.1 

72.1 

58 

86.2 

73 

76.7 



32 

86.3 



72.7 



71 

76.1 



33 

86.3 



73.0 



67 

74.6 



34 

86.3 



73.4 



67 

76.1 




Table 103. Comparisons of older and younger generations of grade school level: 
masturbation, nocturnal emissions, and heterosexual petting 

Accumulative incidence data for masturbation and nocturnal emissions based on the 
life span. Data for petting experience, with or without climax, based on single males. 
Data for older generation on masturbation are based on 322 cases, on nocturnal emis- 
sions on 319 cases, at each and every age. 




MASTURBATION 

EOUC LEVEL 0-8 


20 25 30 35 

A6E 


0=0 OLDER 
■ ^ lii qi YOUNGER I 



NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS - 

I EDUC LEVEL 0-8 


10 15 


20 25 

AGE 


0=0 OLDER 

YOUNGER 


PETTING EXPERIENCE 

EDUC LEVEL 0-8 


Figures 119-121. Comparisons of accumulative incidence for older and younger 
generations of grade school level: masturbation, nocturnal emissions, petting 

The first two figures, 119 and 120, show masturbation and nocturnal emissions dur- 
ing the life span. Figure 121 shows any kind of pre-marital petting experience. 

409 





educational 

LEVEL 


Comparisons of Frequencies in Two Generations 
Mean Frequencies for Total Population 


AGE 

GROUP 

OLDER 

GENER. 

YOUNG. 

GENER. 

OLDER YOUNG. 

GENER. GENER. 

OLDER 

GENER. 

YOUNG. 

GENER. 

OLDER 

GENER. 

YOUNG. 

GENER. 

Single Males 

Outlet 

No, of cases 

Total outlet 

Masturbation 

Nocturn. emiss. 

Educ. level 0-8 








Adol.-15 

315 

397 

2.07 3.77 

1.07 

1.94 

0.05 

0.06 

16-20 

346 

374 

2.31 4.05 

0.78 

1.08 

0.13 

0.17 

21-25 

229 

132 

2.35 4.53 

0.59 

0.69 

0.12 

0.21 

Educ. level 9-12 








Adol.-15 

144 

462 

2.49 3.61 

1.58 

2.04 

0.13 

0.15 

16-20 

146 

461 

2.82 3.76 

1.15 

1.35 

0.20 

0.23 

21-25 

109 

154 

2.41 3.42 

0.70 

0.98 

0.21 

0.25 

Educ. level 13+ 








AdoL-15 

462 

2337 

2.76 2.84 

1.97 

2,27 

0.44 

0.32 

16-20 

476 

2385 

2.69 2.70 

1.71 

1.81 

0.49 

0.41 

21-25 

456 

1442 

2.57 2.47 

1.30 

1.31 

0.41 

0.38 

26-30 

269 

218 

2.68 2.43 

1.24 

1.09 

0.34 

0.27 


Petting 
to climax 


Single Males 

Total pre- 
marit. interc. 


Interc. with 
prostitutes 


Homosexual 


Educ. level 0-8 | 
Adol.-15 
16-20 
21-25 

Educ. level 9-12 
Adol.-15 
16-20 
21-25 

Educ. level 13+ 
AdoL-15 
16-20 
21-25 
26-30 


0.47 0.94 
1.17 1.58 
1.13 1.33 

0.12 0.07 
0.28 0.27 
0.58 0.45 
0.81 0.62 


Married Males 



Outlet 

No, of cases 

Total outlet 

Marit. interc. 

Extra-marit. 

interc. 

Educ. level 0-8 
16-20 

75 ^ 

83 

3.75 

5.50 

3.07 

4.35 

0.37 

0.69 

21-25 

204 

120 

3.08 

5.61 

2.70 

4.28 

0.19 

1.04 

2<^30 

227 ; 

65 

3.02 

5.22 

2.65 

4.21 

0.16 

0.61 

Educ. level 9-12 









21-25 

71 

93 

3.89 

4.35 

3.24 

3.43 

0.37 

0.50 

26-30 

83 

52 

3.43 

3.76 

2,76 

3.07 

0.32 

0.32 

Educ. level 13+ 









21-25 

144 

296 

3.86 

3.62 

3.25 

2.98 

0.16 

0.04 

26-30 

317 

215 

3.33 

2.99 

2.76 

2.40 

0.12 

0.05 


Tablet 104. Comparisons of mean frequencies of sexual activities in older and 
younger generations 

Median difference of age between the two groups is 22 years. 

410 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


411 


5. PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS. AmOUg males of the 
Upper educational levels, coitus before marriage occurs with frequencies 
that are, again, duplicates for the two generations. The only marked 
differences come in early adolescence, where the record is rather materially 
higher for the younger generation. For pre-marital intercourse at lower 
educational levels, the younger generation reports definitely higher fre- 
quencies in every age group, although the differences are more marked 
for the younger ages. As noted above, this increased activity among 
younger males of the lower educational level is apparently correlated with 
the earlier maturation of the boys of that group in the present day; but all 
of these lower level data may be affected by the poor memory of the older 
men who supply the record for the older group. 


MASTURBATION 


EDUC 

LEVEL 

OLDER 

0-8 

YOUNGER 


OLDER 

9-12 

YOUNGER 


OLDER 

13-#- 

YOUNGER 



TOTAL PRE-MARITAL 
INTERCOURSE 

(' ' 



INTERCOURSE WITH 
PROSTITUTES 




Figure 122. Comparisons of frequencies of sexual activity in older and younger 

generations 


Comparisons of mean frequencies for males 16-20 years of age, showing data for 
masturbation, total pre-marital intercourse, and intercourse with prostitutes, for three 
educational levels. Median age difference between the two generations is 22 years. 


6. PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES. The frequencies of pre- 
marital sexual relations with prostitutes are more or less constantly lower 
in the younger generations of all educational levels. There are no excep- 
tions to be observed in Table 104. In most cases the average frequencies 
of intercourse with prostitutes are down to two-thirds or even to one- 
half of what they were in the generation that was most active 22 years ago. 
This is undoubtedly the result of the extensive educational campaigns 
which have associated the prostitute with venereal disease, and of the legal 
drives which have been made against organized prostitution. In the 22-year 
period which has elapsed between the two generations which are involved 
here, most of the state laws against prostitution have come into existence 
or have been considerably strengthened. In particular localities, there has 
been an increasing public interest in controlling organized prostitution. 



EDUCATIONAL 

LEVEL 

AND 

Comparisons of Incidences in Two Generations 
Percents of Total Population Involved 

AGE 

GROUP 

OLDER 

gener. 

young, 
gener. I 

OLDER young. 

gener. gener. 

OLDER YOUNG. 

gener. GENFR. 

OLDER 

GENER. 

YOUNG. 

GENER. 

Single Males 

Outlet 

No. of cases 

' Total outlet 

Masturbation 

Nocturr 

1 . emiss. 

Educ. level 0-8 







Adol.-15 

315 

397 

85.4 96.0 

76.8 90.9 

22.9 

27.5 

16-20 

346 

374 

96.2 98.7 

78.0 90.4 

55.2 

57.5 

21-25 

229 

132 

96.1 97.7 

59.4 67.4 

57.6 

62.9 

Educ. level 9-12 







AdoL-15 

144 

462 

92.4 96.3 

84.7 91.6 

1 41.0 

39.2 

16-20 

146 

461 

100.0 99.6 

84.9 90.2 

69.2 

71.1 

21-25 

109 

154 

98.2 100.0 

67.0 83.1 

70.6 

71.4 

Educ. level 13+ 







AdoL-15 

462 

2337 

95.5 95.8 

80.3 82.8 

72.7 

69.3 

16-20 

476 

2385 

99.8 99.7 

85.7 89.2 

92.2 

91.0 

21-25 

456 

1442 

99.6 99.9 

83.6 88.1 

90.1 

86.1 

26-30 

269 

218 

99.6 100.0 

81.0 85.8 

86.6 

83.5 


Single Males 


Outlet 

Petting 
to climax 

i Interc. with 
companions 

Interc. with 
prostitutes 

Homosexual 

Educ. level 0-8 








Adol.-15 

6.7 

18.6 

37.1 

57.4 

9.8 7.8 

17.1 

29.0 

16-20 

13.0 

28.9 

74.3 

87.7 

51.7 44.9 

17.9 

33.7 

21-25 

11.4 

22.7 

72.5 

87.9 

65.5 52.3 

16.6 

32.6 

Educ. level 9-12 








Adol.-15 

12.5 

21.6 

31.3 

47.2 

9.7 7.4 

23.6 

35.3 

16-20 

32.2 

34.9 

71.2 

73.8 

45.2 40.1 

26.7 

45.3 

21-25 

32.1 

26.0 

73.4 

68.8 

54.1 36.4 

29.4 

43.5 

Educ. level 13+ 








Adol.-15 

9.7 

14.6 

9.3 

9.3 

1.7 2.4 

25.1 

20.8 

16-20 

39.3 

47.1 

37.0 

39.2 

18.7 19.4 

16.4 

15.9 

21-25 

51.8 

51.9 

56.4 

54.1 

23.7 15.1 

12.7 

8.5 

26-30 1 

46.1 

42.7 

59.9 

55.5 

20.4 11.5 

16.4 

18.3 


Married Males 


Outlet 

No. of cases 

Total outlet 

Marit. interc. 

Extra-mant. 

interc. 

Educ. level 0-8 
16-20 

75 

83 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

32.0 

55.4 

21-25 

204 

120 

100.0 

100.0 

99.5 

99.2 

27.9 

45.0 

26-30 

227 

65 

100.0 

96.9 

100.0 

96.9 

32.2 

44.6 

Educ. level 9-12 









21-25 

71 

93 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

36.6 

46.2 

26-30 

83 

52 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

38.6 

53.8 

Educ. level 13+ 









21-25 

144 

296 

100.0 

100.0 

98.6 

100.0 

18.8 

13.2 

26-30 

317 

215 

100.0 

100.0 

99.1 

99.5 

24.3 

18.1 


Table 105. Comparisons of incidences of sexual activities in older and younger 

generations 


Median difference of age between the two groups is 22 years. 
412 




STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


413 


There is no doubt that the openly run organized house of prostitution has 
thereby been eliminated in a great many instances, although our specific 
data make it doubtful that the number of girls involved in prostitution has 
been very much decreased. As indicated above (Table 100, Figures 1 13, 1 14), 
the number of males going to prostitutes at some time in their lives seems 
not to have been affected by these restrictive measures, but the frequency 
data do indicate that they do not return as often as they did before these 
educational and legal moves were made against prostitution. 

7. TOTAL PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE. Comparing frequencies among the 
older and the younger generations, the sum total of the pre-marital inter- 
course which is had with companions and with prostitutes today remains 
about the same in the college level, has definitely increased in the grade 
school group, and has somewhat increased in the high school group. The 


EPUC 

L€V£L 

OLDER 

0-8 

YOUNGER 


OLDER 

9-12 

YOUNGER 


OLDER 

134 ^ 

YOUNGER 

Figure 123. Comparisons of active incidence data for older and younger 

generations 

Data for the age period 16-20. Median age difference between the two generations is 
22 years. 

drives against prostitution have succeeded in diverting a third to a half of 
the intercourse that males used to have with prostitutes to pre-marital 
activities with other girls. 

8. HOMOSEXUAL OUTLETS. Frequencies in the homosexual show, on the 
whole, very little change in older age groups of the two generations 
(Table 104 and other data not in the table). In the youngest adolescent 
period there seems to be a definite increase in frequencies for the younger 
groups, but after 16 or 20 years of age there are no constant changes. There 
are particular educational levels at particular ages where the younger 
generation seems to be more active, and there are other groups where the 
older generation seems to have had the lead. There is, at best, only a slight 
substantiation for the oft-repeated assertion that “sexual perversion” is on 
the increase. It is difficult to understand what sufficient basis there can be 



414 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


for that opinion. It cannot be due to any increase in the obvious public 
display of homosexuality; for if there is one change between generations 
which is certainly established by the data, it is to the effect that public 
displays were more frequent in an older day. Certainly the police in many 
of the larger cities have made particular efforts to reduce street and tavern 
exhibitions of such activity. Evidently many individuals of the older genera- 
tion were unaware of the extent of homosexual activity during their 
younger years. It is possible that the freer discussion of the homosexual 
today, both in technical and in popular print, has made the public more 
conscious of sexual activity that has always been a part of the pattern of 
the human animal. 

9. MARITAL INTERCOURSE. One would presume that the frequencies of 
marital intercourse should show no material differences between the two 
generations, and nothing in the data on the high school and college levels 
would give any reason for believing that such changes have really occurred. 
If there is any real change, it is in the direction of increased frequencies of 
marital intercourse among males of the grade school level. 

10. EXTRA-MARITAL INTERCOURSE. In the grade school and high school 
segments of the population, both the frequencies and the incidences of 
extra-marital intercourse are higher in the younger generation, at least 
during the early years of marriage. For the college group, on the other hand, 
the older generation has much the higher frequencies and somewhat higher 
incidences. There are too few cases where the samples are of sufficient size 
to make the results certain. 

These comparisons of the sexual activities of older and younger genera- 
tions provide striking evidence of the stability of the sexual mores. They 
provide scant justification for the opinion harbored by some persons that 
there are constant changes in such mores, or at least a constant flux — per- 
haps an “evolution” toward something better, or a constant degeneration 
in behavior. 

Some persons have expressed a fear that a long-time sex study of the 
sort in which we are currently engaged will fall into error if it averages 
histories obtained early in the study with histories obtained ten or twenty 
years later. There are persons who have regretted the fact that it was not 
possible to complete this study before World War II. They indicate that it 
is not correct to compare data obtained before the war and data obtained 
since, for patterns change so much in times of war and during post-war 
adjustments that we probably should begin the study anew. Not only do 
the press, propaganda agencies, and moral and law enforcement groups 
encourage this notion, but scientists have been inclined to accept it. There 
are persons who have suggested that we should rule out all histories of men 
who have been in the armed forces, inasmuch as their patterns of behavior 
have, inevitably, been so changed that they are no longer representative of 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


415 


a peace-time population. There are persons who have thought that the 
publication of the present volume might so affect the patterns of behavior 
for whole segments of the population that we could no longer find histories 
that would be representative of the conditions that existed before these 
data were made available. There are more persons who have thought that 
it would be important for us to get re-takes on histories of subjects who 
had previously given histories— not for the sake of testing the validity of 
memory and the extent of the cover-up (as we have in actuality used such 
re-takes), but for the sake of recording the presumably great changes in 
behavior that must follow such discussions of sexual matters as are involved 
in the contribution of one’s history. 

These persons do not seem to have realized the ancient origins of our 
current patterns and their deep foundations in the basic thinking of each 
cultural group. We have repeatedly pointed out that many of our present- 
day attitudes on sex are matters which were settled in the religious phil- 
osophy of the authors of the Old Testament and even among more ancient 
peoples, and there is no evidence that scientific analyses will quickly 
modify such deep-rooted behavior. 

The changes that have occurred in 22 years, as measured by the data 
given in the present chapter, concern attitudes and minor details of 
behavior, and nothing that is deeply fundamental in overt activity. There 
has been nothing as fundamental as the substitution of one type of outlet 
for another, of masturbation for heterosexual coitus, of coitus for the 
homosexual, or vice versa. There has not even been a material increase or 
decrease in the incidences and frequencies of most types of activity. In 
these 20 to 30 years, there appear to have been as material social changes 
as in any period of history. The expansion of manufactured utilities, the 
extension of means of locomotion and of all types of communication, the 
increase in educational programs, the political upheavals, the changes of 
attitude on matters of religion, have been extreme in this period. There 
have been two wars on such a world-wide scale as has never before been 
known. Twice in this period a high percentage of the young men of the 
country was drafted into military service and brought into contact with 
the sexual patterns of persons representing the full range of social levels in 
our own nation, and with the sexual patterns of many of the other nations 
of the world. Following these two wars there have been periods in which 
many persons thought they saw unprecedented moral breakdowns. There 
have been periods of wild inflation, the jazz age, periods of prosperity, 
periods of depression. Millions of dollars have been spent by certain organi- 
zations for the express purpose of changing the sexual habits of the nation. 
This period has seen much new legislation on matters of sex. For the first 
time in American history, Federal agents have been used to enforce sex 
laws on a national scale. And the sum total of the measurable effects on 



416 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


American sexual behavior are slight changes in attitudes, some increase in 
the frequency of masturbation among boys of the lower educational levels, 
more frequent nocturnal emissions, increased frequencies of pre-marital 
petting, earlier coitus for a portion of the male population, and the trans- 
ference of a percentage of the pre-marital intercourse from prostitutes to 
girls who are not prostitutes. 

There is not even evidence that patterns of sexual behavior are materially 
altered among men in the armed forces during a period of war. Precise 
calculations will have to come later, but the available data now indicate 
that it is a small portion of the men who go into the Army or the Navy 
who materially modify their patterns of behavior after they leave home. 
The data are conclusive that such patterns in the case of the male are 
largely established by the age of 16, and no sort of circumstance, however 
catastrophic, materially alters them for more than a very few persons in 
their later years. It is true that many a man has had his first experience in 
heterosexual coitus after he got into the armed forces; but most of these 
men would have begun coitus at about that age if they had stayed at home. 
The men who have the most coitus after getting into the armed forces are, 
for the most part, the men who would have had the most coitus if they had 
stayed at home. The men who find most of their war-time coitus with pros- 
titutes are the men who would have found most of their experience with 
prostitutes at home; and the men who avoid prostitutes in the Army avoid 
them for the same reasons that they would have avoided them at home. 

The public is much more conscious of the behavior of a man in uniform 
than it is of a man in civilian clothes. The civilian who walks down the 
street with a girl does not attract nearly so much attention as the uniformed 
male who walks down the same street with the same girl. The high officer 
who complained that too many mothers thought that the Army had in- 
vented sex had considerable justification for his complaint. 

There is a ready assumption that men in segregated groups, as in the 
Army and the Navy, turn to the homosexual more often than they would 
at home; but it is to be recalled that the active incidence of the homosexual 
in the peace-time U. S. population among men of Army and Navy age is 
nearly 30 per cent (Table 90), and one would have to show that the incidence 
among men in the Army and the Navy is higher than that, or that the fre- 
quencies of contact are higher, in order to prove that patterns for these men 
had been changed in any way. There are, of course, men who have their 
first homosexual experience while in the Army or the Navy, but there are 
men of the same age who would have had their first experience at home 
if there had never been a war. 

Similarly, the married men in the armed forces turn to extra-marital 
intercourse, or avoid extra-marital intercourse, largely in accord with the 
patterns that have guided their behavior previously in their lives. 



StABILlTY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


417 


Patterns of sexual behavior may persist in a social group even though 
many persons may move into it from other groups that have totally differ- 
ent patterns of behavior (Table 106). Twenty-five or thirty years ago, about 
5 per cent of the American males went to college. The 1940 census shows 
about 1 5 per cent of the males of this younger generation receiving such 
advanced education. In the 22 years which have elapsed between our older 
and younger generations, the college population has increased three times. 
But in spite of the fact that the original college population has been 
enlarged by a group twice as large as its original self, the college pattern of 
sexual behavior has remained practically unchanged. In fact, the nearest 
identities between the older and the younger generations (Tables 98-105) 
are at this college level. This is a remarkable tribute to the stability of the 
sexual mores. 

With the return of the veterans from the recent war, and with the sub- 
sidization of their education from public funds, there has come such a 
sudden invasion of the college group as has never before been known. The 
research investigator concerned with human behavior today needs to be 
especially careful to understand the background of the college student 
whom he is interviewing. Many of the veterans who are now attending 
college would have done so if there had been no war, and this is the group 
which, by and large, has sexual histories of the sort that have been reported 
here for the college level. But there are many others who would never have 
gone beyond high school except for the present governmental program. 
Many males of this group are contributing histories of a sort which is not 
usually found within college halls. There are high frequencies of pre- 
marital intercourse with large numbers of companions. There are lower 
level attitudes about masturbation in this group. What will the outcome be 
upon the patterns of sexual behavior among college levels? Will this 
sudden influx of lower level patterns overwhelm the traditions of the upper 
level groups? Will the lower level individuals have their patterns changed 
by their college contacts? Certainly no scientist could have conceived a 
more remarkable experiment for testing the effect of the intermingling of 
social groups. We have found that patterns are largely determined by the 
time of adolescence or at some still earlier age. We find (as reported later 
in this chapter) that, in such migrations as do occur between social levels, 
the changes in patterns do not come in the lifetime of an individual, but 
by way of the next generation. In the light of these considerations, it will 
be interesting to observe what the outcome of this GI invasion of the 
colleges may be. 

VERTICAL MOBILITY: AT AN EARLY AGE 

There are, of course, persons who are born into one social level and who 
move into some other level or levels in the course of their lives. Sorokin 
(1927) and other writers in the social sciences have referred to this as social 



418 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


or vertical mobility. Increasingly common instances are to be found today 
in lower level homes from which children go on to high school and in some 
cases to college. As noted in the previous chapter, such an improvement of 
social position has been an increasingly common phenomenon for some 
decades now in this country. The data (Table 106) show that about 39 
per cent of the subjects in the present study have stayed in the same occupa- 
tional class as their parents, 21 per cent of the population regressed to 


occur. 

CLASS 


Occupational Class of Parent 


OF 

SUBJECT 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

7 

8 

CLASS OF 

SUBJECT 

2 

% 

56 

/o 

21 

7o 

6 

% 

3 

% 

1 

% 

0 

% 

0 

430 

3 

29 

44 

23 

11 

1 

1 

2 

609 

4 

4 

7 

14 

5 

1 

0 

0 

162 

5 

6 

14 

24 

28 

16 

9 

7 

500 

6 

3 

7 ' 

17 

24 ‘ 

40 

23 

24 

556 

7 

2 

7 

16 

29 

40 

65 

46 

671 

8 

0 

0 

0 

0 

1 

2 

21 

17 

Total % 

100 

100 

100 

100 

100 

100 

100 ! 

2945 

Cases in pa- 
rental class 

419 

673 

485 

566 

567 

193 

42 

2945 


CASES IN 
OCCUP. 


Table 106. Stability and mobility of occupational classes 

Showing (in bold face) percent of each parental class whose male offspring stay within 
the same class. And showing (in regular type) what percent of the offspring of each 
parental class moves into other occupational classes. Parental classes represent all rat- 
ings held by parent while subject lived in the home. Subject classes represent only the 
highest ratmg ever held by subject up to time of reporting. 


occupational classes lower than those in which their parents raised them, 
and 40 per cent have risen to social positions superior to those held by their 
parents. There is, obviously, a considerable shifting of occupational classes 
and social position in our American society, and it is of interest to know 
how sexual patterns are affected when such changes occur in social classes. 

Two sorts of situations are involved. The first includes those cases where 
the subject breaks with the parental patterns while he is stiU Hving with 
his parents. This is much the commoner sort of case, accounting for nearly 
all of the movement of the 61 per cent of the population which does not 
stay in its parental class. On tWs type of case we have an abundance of 
data. 

The second type is one in which the individual stays within his parental 
class until some later time in his life, at least until his late teens, and only 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


419 


finally moves into some other occupational group. This is the rarer type of 
case. Consequently there are only a few instances of this sort on which we 
can draw for illustration. It is unwarranted, at the present time, to attempt 
statistical analyses of these few data. 

From the tabulations in Tables 107 to 114, it is possible to compare the 
sexual histories of males who have stayed in the parental occupational 
class with the histories of males who have moved out of the parental class 
into some other group which is either higher or lower. In Table 115 it is 
possible to compare the sexual histories of males who have arrived at the 
same occupational class, even though the parents of these several males 
belonged to a variety of occupational classes. 

In general, it will be seen that the sexual history of the individual accords 
with the pattern of the social group into which he ultimately moves, rather 
than with the pattern of the social group to which the parent belongs and 
in which the subject was placed when he lived in the parental home. Indi- 
viduals originating from different parental classes have much the same 
histories, if they ultimately arrive at the same occupational rating. A half 
dozen persons who come from the same parental occupational class may 
have a half dozen difierent sorts of histories if they finally locate in that 
many different classes. These statements are, of course, based on averages 
for whole groups, and it may be anticipated that particular individuals in 
each and every one of these groups will depart from any average. Neverthe- 
less, so many individuals do fit into this general description that the means 
and medians calculated for these several populations are quite distinct. 

The most significant thing shown by these calculations (Tables 107-115) 
is the evidence that an individual who is ever going to depart from the 
parental pattern is hkely to have done so by the time he has become 
adolescent. (See Chapter 5 on pre-adolescent sexual development.) In com- 
paring the sexual histories with the educational backgrounds of each indi- 
vidual (Chapter 10), we have already reached the conclusion that the 
patterns of behavior are largely laid down by age 16, and that relatively few 
persons change their patterns of behavior at any later time in their lives. 
Now the analyses made for the occupational classes of the parent and the 
subject fully and abundantly confirm this generalization. The patterns of 
the several occupational classes are remarkably distinct in the group that is 
16 to 20 years of age. It is evident that a high proportion of the individuals 
are conforming to the general pattern. Between adolescence and 15 years 
of age, the groups are not yet as distinct. Nevertheless, it is amazing that 
distinctions are at all evident in these early adolescent years. These facts 
have considerable scientific and social significance. 

Unfortunately, analyses that are based upon frequencies of orgasm do 
not provide a basis for measuring pre-adolescent activity. It will, however, 
be of the utmost significance to obtain a considerable number of histories 



420 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 



OCCUR. 


Total Outlet and Occupational Class 




1 




AGE 

GROtJP 

CLASS 

REACHED 

BY 

CASES 

total population 

active population 


SUBJECT 


Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 


Parental Occupational Class 2 


Adol.-15 

2 

231 

2.71 =t0.19 

1.83 

95.7 

2.83 ±0.19 

1.92 


3 

126 

2.68 ±0.30 

1.40 

90.5 

2.96 ±0.33 

1.69 

16-20 

2 

198 

3.08 ±0.18 

2.36 

99.5 

3.10±0.18 

2.38 


3 

116 

3.46 ±0.37^ 

2.11 

; 99.1 

3.49 ±0.37 

2.14 

21-25 

2 

92 

2.50 ±0.24 

1.89 1 

98.9 

2.53 ±0.24 

1.93 


Parental Occupational Class 3 


Adol.-15 


2 


140 

2.67 

±0.27 

1.73 

91.4 

2.92 ±0.291 

1.95 



3 


384 

3.10 

±0.18 

1.95 

93.0 

3.33 ±0.19 1 

2.20 



5 


205 

2.85 

±0.19 

2.06 

95.1 

2.99 ±0.19 

2.19 


6 

+ 

7 

92 

2.88 

±0.30 

2.19 

96.7 

2.97 ±0.30 

2.28 

16-20 


2 


138 

2.85 

±0.24 

2.20 

97.1 

2.93 ±0.24 

2.30 



3 


318 

3.50 

±0.21 

2.45 

98.1 

3.57 ±0.21 

2.51 



5 


201 

2.88 

±0.20 

2.05 

99.5 

2.89 ±0.20 

2.06 


6 

+ 

7 

94 

3.12 

±0.26 

2.54 

98.9 

3.15 ±0.26 

2.57 

21^25 


2 


64 

2.45 

±0.41 

1.55 

98.4 

2.49 ±0.42 

1.60 



3 


120 

3.41 

±0.41 

2.07 

97.5 

3.50 ±0.42 

2.14 



5 


124 

2.73 

±0.22 

1.93 

99.2 

2.75 ±0.22 

1.95 


6 

+ 

7 

87 

2.67 

±0.20 

2.40 

100.0 

2.67 ±0.20 

2.40 


Parental Occupational Class 4 


Adol.-15 

3 

107 

3.62 ±0.35 

2.50 

95.3 

3.80 ±0.35 

2.68 


4 

158 

2.56 ±0.21 

1.85 

94.3 

2.71 ±0.21 

1.95 


5 

304 

3.00 ±0.17 

2.29 

94.1 

3.18 ±0.17 

2.49 


6 

109 

2.85 ±0.25 

2.36 

96.3 

2.96 ±0.25 

2.50 


7 

74 

3.14 ±0.39 

1.96 

97.3 

3.23 ±0.40 

2.06 

16-20 

3 

105 

3.82 ±0.34 

2.64 

99.0 

3.86 ±0.34 

2.68 


4 

104 

2.73 ±0.24 

2.09 

100.0 

2.73 ±0.24 

2.09 


5 

305 

2.97 ±0.16 

2.36 

99.0 

3.00±0.16 

2.40 


6 

111 

3.01 ±0.24 

2.32 

100.0 

3.01 ±0.24 

2.32 


7 

75 

3.23 ±0.32 

2.43 

100.0 

3.23 ±0.32 

2.43 

21-25 

5 

183 

2.86 ±0.22 

2.09 

99.5 

2.87 ±0.22 

2.10 


6 

98 

2.55 ±0.21 

1.94 

100.0 

2.55 ±0.21 

1.94 


7 

71 

2.51 ±0.23 

2.00 

100.0 

2.51 ±0.23 

2.00 


{Table continued on next page) 


STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


421 





Total Outlet and Occupational Class 


occur. 

CLASS 

REACHED 
BY j 

SUBJECT 



1 

1 



AGE 

CASES 

1 TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

GROUP 

s 







Mean 

Median 

! 

Incid. 

Mean 

Median 




Frequency 

Freq. 

0/ 

/o 

Frequency 

Freq. 


Parental Occupational Class 5 


Adol.-15 

3 

65 

3.38 ±0.48 

2.33 

93.8 

3.60 ±0.49 

2.50 

5 

563 

2.77 ±0.10 

2.21 

94.1 

2.95 ±0.11 

2.37 


6 

228 

2.86 ±0.21 

2.15 

95.6 

2.99 ±0.22 

2.26 


7 

152 

2.70 ±0.23 

1.81 

95.4 

2.83 ±0.24 

1.96 

16-20 

3 

64 

3.03 ±0.42 

2.42 

95.3 

3.17 ±0.43 

2.56 


5 

516 

2.63 ±0.10 

2.05 

99.0 

2.65 ±0.10 

2.08 


6 

230 

2.72 ±0.16 

1.98 

99.6 

2.73 ±0.16 

1.98 


7 

155 

2.50±0.17 

1.89 

100.0 

2.50 ±0.17 

1.89 

21-25 

5 

262 

2.32±0.13 

1.76 

99.2 

2.33 ±0.13 

1.78 


6 

178 

2.56 ±0.18 

1.86 

100.0 

2.56 ±0.18 

1.86 


7 

143 

2.43 ±0.18 

1.84 

100.0 

2.43 ±0.18 

1.84 


Parental Occupational Class 6 


Adol.-15 

5 

98 

3.76 ±0.34 

3.03 

98.0 

3.84 ±0.34 

3.09 


6 

1048 

2.86 ± 0.09 

2.29 

96.2 

2.97 ±0.09 

2.41 


7 

244 

3.00 ±0.22 

1 2.07 

98.4 

3.05 ±0.22 

2.13 

16-20 

5 

100 

3.80±0.31 

3.19 

99.0 

3.84 ±0.31 

3.22 


6 

1021 

2.58 ±0.07 

2.11 

99.7 

2.58 ±0.07 

2.12 


7 

246 

3.18 ±0.19 

2.66 

100.0 

3.18 ±0.19 

2.66 

21-25 

5 

71 

3.13 ±0.32 

2.86 

98.6 

3.17 ±0.32 

2.89 

6 

5^ 

2.38 ±0.08 

1.88 

100.0 

2.38 ±0.08 

1.88 


7 

236 

2.88 ±0.19 

2.03 

100.0 

2.88 ±0.19 

2.03 

26-30 

6 

103 

2.34 ±0.21 

1.73 

100.0 

2.34 ±0.21 

1.73 

7 

97 

2.90 ±0.38 

1.89 

100.0 

2.90 ±0.38 

1.89 

Married 






3.73 ±0.27 

2.97 

21-25 

6 

113 

3.73 ±0.27 

2.97 

100.0 

7 

76 

4.41 ±0.40 

3.71 

100.0 

4.41 ±0.40 

3.71 

26-30 

6 

112 

3.11 ±0.27 

2.48 

100.0 

3.11 ±0.27 

2.48 

7 1 

135 

3.54 ± 0.25 

2.97 ! 

100.0 

3.54 ±0.25 

2.97 


Parental Occupational Class 7 


Adol.-15 J 

7 

414 

3.05 ± 0.14 

2.46 

96.6 

3.16 ±0.14 

2.54 

16-20 

7 

416 

3.04 ± 0.13 

2.49 

99.8 

3.04 ±0.13 

2.50 

21-25 

7 

266 

2.71 ± 0.15 

2.09 

100.0 

2.71 ±0.15 

2.09 

Mart ied 
21-25 

7 

63 

3.64 ± 0.26 

3.27 

100.0 

3.64 ± 0.26 

3.27 

26-30 

7 

72 

i 

3.46 ± 0.24 

3.17 

100.0 i 

3.46 ±0.24 

3.17 


Table 107. Total outlet in relation to occupational class of parent and of subject 


AH data based on single males, except where indicated as "‘married.” 



422 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Masturbation and Occupational Class 


OCCUP. 




! 



— ’ 

CLASS 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

REACHED 

CASES 







BY 








SUBJECT 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

7oOf 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Parental Occupational Class 2 


AdoL-15 

2 

231 

1.47 0.11 

0.91 

56.07 

89.6 

1.65 =i=0.12 

1.04 


3 

126 

1.40=5=0.15 

0.90 

53.07 

87.3 

1.60=i=0.16 

1.18 

16-20 

2 

198 

1.15^0.10 

0.63 

38.06 

84.8 

1.36=5=0.11 

0.83 


3 

116 

1.01 =i= 0.12 

0.48 

28.95 

89.7 

1.13 =5=0.13 

0.65 

21-25 

2 

92 

0.72^0.10 

0.33 

29.86 

72.8 

0.99 =5=0.13 

0.58 


Parental Occupational Class 3 


Adoi.-15 


2 


140 ! 

1.61 

zii 

0.18 

0.84 

61.87 

86.4 

1.87 

=k 

0.20 

1.04 



3 


384 

1.57 

db 

0.10 

0.95 

51.99 

88.8 

1.77 

=fc 

0.10 

1.16 



5 


205 

2.07 

=fc 

0.17 

1.38 

73.84 

82.4 

2.52 

d= 

0.18 

1.80 


6 

4- 

7 

92 

2.25 

=b 

0.26 

1.61 

81.23 

83.7 

2.69 

=b 

0.29 

2.11 

16-20 


2 


138 

0.99 


0.12 

0.43 

35.15 

83.3 

1.19 


0.14 

0.61 



3 


318 

1.00 

d= 

0.07 

0.48 

29.02 

86.8 

1.16 

=b 

0.08 

0.64 



5 


201 

1.57 

d= 

0.14 

0.85 

55.57 

87.6 

1.79 

d= 

0.15 

1.02 


6 

+ 

7 

94 

2.13 


0.22 

1.66 

68.47 

88.3 

2.41 

=fc 

0.24 

2.00 

21-25 


2 


64 

0.47 

=h 

0.12 

0.09 

19.49 

62.5 

0.75 

=i: 

0.18 

0.35 



3 


120 

0.56 

zfc 

0.08 

0.17 

16.91 

65.0 

0.86 


0.12 

0.42 



5 


124 

1.09 

db 

0.13 

0.49 

41.00 

87.9 

1.24 

=fc 

0.14 

0.68 


6 

■4 

7 

87 

i 

1.40 

d= 

0.16 

0.73 

53.00 

90.8 

1.54 

=fc 

0.17 

0.88 


Parental Occupational Class 4 


Adol.-15 

3 

107 

1.94=1=0.21 

1.29 

54.63 

86.9 

2.24=1=0.22 

1.54 


4 

158 

1.84=b0.15 

1.31 

72.58 

88.6 

2.07 =fc 0.16 1 

1.61 


5 

304 

2.18 =i=0.13 

1.64 

73.93 

84.5 

2.57±0.14 

2.08 


6 

109 

2.18 =t= 0.21 1 

1.71 j 

77.39 

82.6 

2.64=1=0.22 

2.18 


7 

74 : 

2.56 =fc 0.38 

1.38 

82.13 

82.4 

3.11 =1=0.40 

2.25 

16-20 

3 

105 

0.98 =fc= 0.12 

0.49 

25.58 

85.7 

1.14=1=0.13 

0.66 


4 

104 

1.21 =fc0.14 

0.70 

44.31 

89.4 

1.35 =1=0.15 

0.89 


5 

305 

1.64^0.11 

1.02 

55.86 

^ 88.2 

1.86 =1=0.12 

1.43 


6 

111 

1.71 =t 0.17 

0.98 

57.31 

1 85.6 

2.00=1=0.18 

1.50 


7 

75 

2.21 =1=0.24 

1.60 

68.38 

1 96.0 

2.31 =1=0.24 

1.75 

21-25 

5 

183 

1.17=t0.12 

0.62 

41.50 

84.2 

1.39=1=0.14 

0.86 


6 

98 

1.10^0.13 

0.65 

43.15 

; 84.7 

1.30=1=0.14 

0.80 


7 

71 

i 

1.37 0.16 

0.96 

54.04 

i 95.8 

1 

1.43 =1=0.17 

1.03 


{Table continued on next page) 


STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


423 





Masturbation and Occupational Class 


OCCXJP. 




1 

1 



AGE 

GROUP 

CLASS 


total population 

active population 

REACHED 

CASES 







BY 









SUBJECT 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Parental Occupational Class 5 


AdoL-15 

3 

65 

1.81 =5=0.24 

j 1.18 

54.18 

92.3 

1.96 ±0.25 

1.30 


! 5 

563 

2.13 =b0.09 

1 1.62 

78.07 

85.1 

2.51 =5=0.10 

1 1.94 


6 

228 

2.17 =5=0.15 

1.69 

76.69 

83.3 

2.60 =5=0.17 

2.08 


7 

152 

2.05 =5=0.22 

1.10 

78.48 

80.3 

2.55 =5=0.25 

1.67 

16-20 

3 

64 

0.88 =5=0.12 

0.57 

29.35 

89.1 

0.99 =t0.13 

0.68 


5 

516 

1.68 0.08 

1.10 

64.37 

88.8 

1.90 =5=0.09 

1.36 


6 

230 

1.82 =5=0.13 

1.27 

67.29 

88.3 

2.06 ^0.14 

1.57 


7 

155 

1.60=5=0.16 

0.85 

64.24 

86.5 

1.86 =5=0.17 

1.16 

21-25 

5 

262 

1.22=5=0.10 

0.64 

52.95 

84.4 

1.45 =5=0.11 

0.85 


6 

178 

1.43 =±=0.14 

0.65 

57.18 

85.4 

1.67 =5=0.16 

0.91 


7 

143 

1.18 =5=0.12 

0.57 

49.68 

85.3 

1.38 =5=0.14 

0.79 


Parental Occupational Class 6 


Adol.-15 

5 

98 

2.74=5=0.28 

1.83 

74.71 

91.8 

2.98 =5=0.29 

1.98 


6 

1048 

2.29=5=0.08 

1.72 

81.20 

85.1 

2.69 =5=0.09 

2.13 


7 

244 

2,24=5=0.19 

1.34 

76.23 

79.1 

2.84 =5=0.22 

2.00 

16-20 

5 

100 

1.94=5=0.20 

1.55 

52.46 

90.0 

2.16 =5=0.20 

1.72 


6 

1021 

1.73 ±0.06 

1.18 

66.93 

90.0 

1.92 =5=0.06 

1.44 


7 

246 

2.17=5=0.17 

1.58 

68.26 

89.0 

2.44 :i=0.19 

1.81 

21-25 

5 

71 

1.27 =5=0.16 

0.78 

41.97 

81.7 

1.56 =5=0.18 

1.25 


6 

554 

1.27=5=0.07 

0.68 

54.80 

87.5 

1.45 =5=0.07 

0.87 


7 

236 

1.38=5=0.13 

0.74 

48.66 

86.9 

1,59 =5=0.14 

0.91 

26-30 

6 

103 

0.94=5=0.13 

0.46 

39.84 

84.5 

1.11 =5=0.14 

0.64 


7 

97 

1.17=5=0.25 

0.44 

39.22 

79.4 

1.48 =5=0.30 

0.67 


Parental Occupational Class 7 


Adol.-15 

7 

414 

2.39=5=0.12 

1.85 

79.29 

79.0 

3.03 =5=0.13 

2.42 

16-20 

7 

416 

2.12=5=0.11 

1.66 

70.34 

88.9 

2.39 =b0.12 

1.87 

21-25 

7 

266 

1.51 =5=0.11 

0.82 

56.56 

88.3 

1.71 =5=0.11 

0.99 


Table 108. Masturbation in relation to occupational class of parent and of subject 
All data based on single males. 




424 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

GROUP 

OCCUP. 

CLASS 

REACHED 

BY 

SUBJECT 

i 

CASES 

Nocturnal Emissions and Occupational Class 

total population 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

7oOf 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

Parental Occupational Class 2 

Adol.-15 

2 

231 

0.05 =b0.01 

0.00 

1.81 

26.0 

0.18 ±0.05 

0.08 


3 

126 

0.06 =5=0.03 

0.00 

2.46 

21.4 

0.30 ±0.14 

0.09 

16-20 

2 

198 

0.10 ±0.02 

0.01 

3.28 

52.5 

0.19 ±0.03 

0.08 


3 

116 

0.16 ±0.03 

0.03 

4.62 

59.5 

0.27 ±0.05 

0.10 

21-25 

2 

92 

0 12 ±0.02 

0.03 

4.81 

58.7 

0.20 ±0.03 

0.10 


Parental Occupational Class 3 


2 

140 

0.05 ±0.02 

0.00 

1.99 

25.7 

0.20 ±0.05 

0.08 

3 

384 

0.11 ±0.02 

0.00 

3.76 

31.3 

0.36 ±0.07 

0.09 

5 

205 

0.27 ±0.04 

0.02 

9.64 

54.1 

0.50 ±0.07 

0.24 

6 + 7 

92 

0.35 ±0.07 

0.09 

12.55 

71.7 

0.49 ±0.09 

0.24 

2 

138 

0.11 ±0.02 

0.02 

3.95 

56.5 

0.20 ±0.03 

0.09 

3 

318 

0.21 ±0.02 

0.05 

6.12 

65.1 

0.33 ±0.03 

0.13 

5 

201 

0.35 ±0.04 

0.14 

12.48 

82.6 

0.43 ± 0 04 

0.24 

6 + 7 

94 

0 47 ±0.07 

0.26 

15.08 

93.6 

0.50 ± 0.07 

0.29 

2 

64 

0.13 ±0.03 

0.04 

5.39 

64.1 

0.20 ±0.05 

0.09 

3 

120 

0.19 ±0.03 

0.04 

5.74 

63.3 

0.30 ±0.05 

0.11 

5 

124 

0.31 ±0.04 

0.11 

11.82 

73.4 

0.43 ±0.06 

0.27 

6 + 7 

87 

0.38 ±0.06 

0.22 

14.45 

90.8 

0.42 ±0.06 

0.26 


Parental Occupational Class 4 


Adol.-15 

3 

107 

0.07 ±0.02 

0.00 

2.08 

31.8 

0.23 ±0.06 

0.11 


4 

158 

0.07 ±0.02 

0.00 

2.77 

36.1 

0.19 ±0.04 

0.09 


5 

304 

0.28 ±0.04 

0.06 

9.64 

63.2 

0.45 ±0.06 

0.21 


6 

109 

0.33 ±0.05 

0.08 

11.75 

67.0 

0.50 ±0.07 

0.32 


7 

74 

0.45 ±0.09 

0.19 

14.39 

81.1 

0.55 ±0.10 

0.29 

16-20 

3 

105 

0.24 ±0.05 

0.06 

6.22 

65.7 

0.36 ±0.06 

0.18 


4 

104 

0.14 ±0.03 

0.04 

5.07 

64.4 

0.21 ±0.03 

0.10 


5 

305 

0.38 ±0.04 

0.15 

12.87 

87.2 

0.43 ±0.04 

0.22 


6 

111 

0.48 ±0.06 

0,29 

16.20 

88.3 

0.55 ±0.06 

0.35 


7 

75 

0.55 ±0.09 

0.29 

17.04 

90.7 

0.61 ±0.10 

0.33 

21-25 

5 

183 

0.35 ±0.05 

0.15 

12.38 

84.2 

0.42 ±0.06 

0.23 


6 

98 

0.46 ±0.07 

0.25 

17.85 

86.7 

0.53 ±0.07 

0.32 


7 

71 

0.39 ±0.07 

0.19 

15.41 

83.1 

0.47 ±0.08 

0.28 


{Table continued on next page) 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


425 


Nocturnal Emissions and Occupational Class 


AGE 

GROUP 


OCCUP. 





1 



CLASS 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

REACHED 

CASES 







BY 








SUBJECT 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

7oOf 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Parental Occupational Class 5 


Adol.-15 

3 

65 

0.05 

d= 

0.02 

0.00 

1.38 

30.8 

0.15 

=fc 

0.04 

0.08 


5 

563 

0.31 

=b 

0.03 

0.05 

11.24 

60.9 

0.50 

=b 

0.05 

0.23 


6 

228 

0.43 

=i= 

0.08 

0.12 

15.04 

68.9 

0.62 


0.12 

0.30 


7 

152 

0.43 

=i= 

0.06 

0.22 

16.47 

78.3 

1 

0.55 

db 

0.08 

0.33 

16-20 

3 

64 

0.14 


0.04 

0.03 

4.53 

60.9 

0.22 

=h 

0.05 

0.08 


5 

516 

0.37 

=b 

0.03 

0.17 

14.17 

85.1 

0.44 

dr 

0.03 

0.25 


6 

230 

0.47 

=b 

0.05 

0.26 

17.25 

89.6 

0.52 

d= 

0.05 

0.31 


7 

155 

0.47 

d= 

0.06 

0.29 

18.70 

92.3 

0.51 

dr 

0.06 

0.32 

21-25 

5 

262 

0.30 

=fc 

0.03 

0.16 

13.01 

82 4 

0.36 

dr 

0.04 

0,24 


6 

178 

0.43 

=h 

0.05 

0.24 

16.99 

86.0 

0.49 

dr 

0.05 

0.30 


7 

143 

0.40 

dr 

0.06 

0 23 

16.70 

86.7 

0.46 

1 

rb 

0.07 

0.29 


Parental Occupational Class 6 


Adol.-15 

5 

98 

0.20 ±0.04 

0 04 

5.54 

59.2 

0.34 ± 0.06 

0.19 


6 

1048 i 

0.28 ±0.02 

0.08 

10.09 

66 9 

0.43 ± 0.02 

0.26 


7 1 

244 

0.44 ±0.04 

0.25 

15.07 

84.0 

0.53 ±0.04 

0.33 

16-20 

5 

100 

0.35 ±0.05 

0.17 

9.52 

85.0 

0.41 =t 0.06 

0.24 


6 

1021 

0.38 ±0.02 

0.23 

14.77 

90.8 

0.42 ± 0.02 

0.27 


7 

246 

0.54 ±0.04 

0.33 

16.88 

93.5 

0.58 ± 0.04 

0.35 

21-25 1 

5 

71 

0,31 ±0.05 

0.16 

10.23 

78.9 

0.39 ± 0.06 

0.26 


6 

554 

0.37 ±0.02 

I 0.22 

15.85 

87.5 

0.42 ± 0.02 

0.28 


7 

236 

0.43 ±0.04 

1 0.26 

15.12 

88.1 

0.49 ±0.04 

0.32 

26-30 

6 

103 

0.31 ±0.04 

i 0.20 

13.17 

84.5 

0.37 ±0.04 

0.27 


7 

97 

0.30 ±0.04 

0.20 

10.18 

83.5 

0.36 ±0.04 

0.27 


Parental Occupational Class 7 


Adol.~15 

7 

414 

0.40 ±0.04 

0.18 

13.30 

76.6 

0.52 ±0.05 

0.30 

16-20 

7 

416 

0.43 ±0.03 

0.27 

14.38 

88.9 

0.49 ±0.03 

0.32 

21-25 

7 

266 

0.43 ±0.04 

0.26 

15.95 

86.8 

0.49 ±0.05 

0.31 


Table 109. Nocturnal emissions in relation to occupational class of parent and 

of subject 

All data based on single males. 


426 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


from very young boys, in order to examine the possibility that movements 
between social classes begin in pre-adolescence. It is now certain that such 
movements are well under way by the earliest adolescent years, and that 
they are completed long before most boys ever leave the parental home. 

More detailed analyses of the data shown in Tables 107 to 115 may be 
summed up as follows: 

Occupational Classes 2 and 3. Class 2 includes the day laborers and 
class 3 includes the semi-skilled laborers. It will be seen that the sexual 
patterns of these two groups are very similar, whether measured by inci- 
dences or frequencies for particular sources of outlet. Class 3 does have 
somewhat higher frequencies of total outlet, primarily because class 2 
(like the grade school group in Chapter 10) has its averages pulled down by 
the undue number of physically poor and mentally dull individuals who 
are in the group. 

Occupational class 2 is one of the most stable in the social organization. 
About 56 per cent of the males who were born in class 2 stay in that class 
throughout their lives (Table 106). The median number of years of school- 
ing which this group has is 6.8 (Table 80), and it is only 23 per cent of the 
group which ever goes into high school. Consequently, the sexual pattern 
for most of the group is very close to that which has been described pre- 
viously (Chapter 10) for those boys who never go beyond grade school. 
The single males of the group depend primarily upon heterosexual inter- 
course, utilize masturbation to a lesser degree, have an absolute minimum 
of nocturnal emissions, rarely pet to the point of climax, and are involved 
in the homosexual more frequently than the males in any other occupa- 
tional class (Tables 107-115). 

To apply the description to a specific age period, namely that between 
ages 16 and 20, the statement is that these males who never belong to any 
occupational class that is higher than a 2 or a 3 average pre-marital inter- 
course with frequencies that are 6 to 8 times as high as the frequencies 
among boys who go further along in school and who ultimately belong to 
occupational class 6 (Figures 101-102). Between 16 and 20 they masturbate 
only about half as often as the boys who end up in occupational class 6 
(Figure 98). The nocturnal emissions of the males of class 2 occur one- 
quarter as often as nocturnal emissions among the males of class 6 or 7 
(Figure 99). Class 3 has emissions more frequently than class 2. Petting to 
climax between ages 16 and 20 occurs only a half or a third as often among 
these boys who are in class 2 and class 3 (Figure 100). Frequencies of the 
homosexual among males of class 2, between the ages of 16 and 20, are 11 
times higher than among the males who ultimately arrive in occupational 
class 7 ; and, next to class 2, the males of class 3 have the highest frequen- 
cies in the homosexual (Figure 105). 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


427 


About 90 per cent of the males of occupational class 2 who have contrib- 
uted to the present study had parents who belonged to either occupational 
class 2 or 3 (Table 106). There has not been much movement between 
classes here. On the other hand, the males of occupational class 3, which 
includes the semi-skilled workmen, were derived from parents of the same 
occupational class in 44 per cent of the cases (Table 106), and in the 
remainder of the cases derived more or less equally from parents of occupa- 
tional class 2 and from parents of higher social rating. About 38 per cent of 
the males of occupational class 3 represent individuals who regressed from 
the level reached by their parents. In a number of cases, the fathers were 
skilled workmen whose offspring were not equally skilled, but in 1 5 per cent 
of the cases these males in class 3 were derived from parents who belonged 
to white collar classes, even including professional and top business groups. 
However, in spite of the diverse origins of the males in class 3, there is 
most remarkable agreement between the sexual histories of those who came 
from occupational class 2 and those who came from occupational classes 

4 and 5. This agreement becomes striking by the time the male has reached 
his late teens, but it is already quite apparent in the histories of the youngest 
adolescent boys. 

Occupational Class 4. This class includes the skilled workmen. It is the 
least stable of all the occupational classes. There is a continual influx into 
the group from persons who originated in occupational classes 2 and 3. 
On the other hand, the offspring of the group move on into higher occupa- 
tional ratings in 57 per cent of the cases. The group has a much better 
economic status than most of the other laboring groups, and includes a 
good many persons of superior ability. The group continuously aspires to 
higher levels and a considerable proportion of its children go to college. 
About 40 per cent of the group stops with some high school education, 
and 7 per cent gets some work in college. Next to the white collar and pro- 
fessional classes, this is the class that supplies the largest number of college 
students (Table 80). Because of this migration the group does not perpet-. 
uate itself. It has been a considerable problem in industry to persuade the 
sons of skilled workmen to become interested in the trades in which their 
fathers work. 

In their patterns of sexual behavior, the males of occupational class 4 
seem to be more or less intermediate between males of the lower occupa- 
tional classes and males of the lower white collar group (Tables 107-115). 
The children in the homes which belong to occupational class 4 present an 
amazing assemblage of patterns of behavior, because some of them finally 
regress to class 3, and a great many of them move on to occupational classes 

5 and 6, and, in a fair number of cases, to the professional class 7. By 
early adolescence, the boys from class 4 homes who are destined to reach 
class 7 may already be identified by their high frequencies of masturbation 



428 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

GROUP 

OCCUP. ‘ 

CLASS 

REACHED 

BY 

SUBJECT 

CASES 

Petting to Climax, and Occupational Class 

total population 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%0f 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

/o 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

Parental Occupational Class 2 

AdoL-15 

2 

231 

0.05 0.02 

0.00 1 

1.83 

14.3 

0.34 =±=0.11 

0.07 


3 

126 

0.03 0.01 

0.00 

0.95 

14.3 

0.18 =t0.10 

0.06 

16-20 

2 

198 

0.07 =1=0.02 

0.00 

2.13 

25.8 

0.25 =±=0.08 

0.07 


3 

116 

0.04 =L 0.02 

0.00 

1.14 

24.1 

0.17 =1=0.07 

0.06 

21-25 

2 

92 

0.07 =t 0.04 

0.00 

2.72 

17.4 

0.38 ±0.20 

0.09 


Parental Occupational Class 3 


Adol.-15 

2 

140 

0.03 =b 0.02 

0.00 

1.24 

11.4 

0.28 =1=0.14 

0.08 


3 ‘ 

384 

0.04 =fc 0.01 

0.00 

1.27 

17.4 

0.22 =1=0.06 

0.06 


5 

205 

0.03 =fc0.01 

0.00 

0.95 

12.2 

0.22 =1=0.07 

0.08 


6 + 7 

92 

0.02 =1=0.01 

0.00 

0.74 

14.1 

0.15 ±0.04 

0.09 

16-20 

2 

138 

0.08 =1=0.03 

0.00 

2.89 

22.5 

0.36 ±0.11 

0.09 


3 

318 

0.07 =b 0.02 

0.00 

2.10 

29.6 

0.25 ±0.06 

0.06 


5 

201 

0.10 =1=0.02 

0.00 

3.57 

34.8 

0.29 ±0.05 

0.11 


6 + 7 

94 

0.15 =1=0.03 

0.00 

4.66 

45.7 

0.32 ±0.05 

0.21 

21-25 

2 i 

64 

0.07 =1=0.05 

0.00 

3.05 

14.1 

0.52 ±0.36 

0.08 


3 

120 

0,07 =1=0.03 

0.00 

2.02 

17.5 

0.38 ±0.19 

0.07 


5 

124 

! 0.11 =1=0.02 

0.00 

4.01 

36.3 

0.29 ±0.06 

0.10 


6 + 7 

87 

0.21 =1=0.041 

0.06 

! 

7.92 

67.8 

0.31 ±0.05 

0.14 


Parental Occupational Class 4 


Adol.-15 

3 

107 

0.04 ±0.02 

0.00 

1.14 

25.2 

0.16 ±0.06 

0.06 


4 

158 

0.04 ±0,02 

0.00 

1.58 1 

13.9 

0.29 ±0.12 

0.07 


5 

304 

0.03 ±0.01 

0.00 

1.01 

11.8 

0.25 ±0.06 

0.08 


6 

109 

0.04 ±0.02 

0.00 

1.46 

9.2 

0.45 ±0.23 

0.09 


7 

74 

0.04 ±0.03 

0.00 

1.21 

8.1 

0.47 ±0.27 

0.30 

16-20 

3 

105 

0.06 ±0.02 

0.00 

1.53 

36.2 

0.16 ±0.05 

0.06 


4 

104 

0.08 ±0.03 

0.00 

2.91 

24.0 

0.33 ±0.13 

0.08 


5 

305 

0.11 ±0.02 

0.00 

3.72 

43.9 

0.25 ±0.03 

0.08 


6 

111 

0.20 ±0.05 

0.01 

6.75 

51.4 

0.39 ±0.10 

0.09 


7 

75 

0.12 ±0.04 

0.00 

3.82 

41.3 

0.30 ±0.10 

0.10 

21-25 

5 

183 

0.14 ±0.03 

0.00 

4.94 

47.0 

0.30 ±0.06 

0.08 


6 

98 

0.27 ±0.07 

0.02 

10.37 

56.1 

0.47 ±0.11 

0.09 


7 

71 

0.17 ±0.04 

0.03 

6.53 

56.3 

0.29 ±0.06 

0.20 


[Table continued on next page) 




STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


429 





Petting to Climax, and Occupational Class 


OCCUP. 





1 



AGE 

GROUP 

CLASS 


TOTAL population 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

REACHED 

CASES 







BY 









SUBJECT 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Indd. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Parental Occupational Class 5 


Adol.-15 

3 

65 

0.03 

d= 

0.01 

0.00 

0.83 

16.9 

0.16 

=h 

0.07 

0.08 


5 

563 

0.04 


0.01 

0.00 

1.57 

13.1 

0.33 

d= 

0.06 

0.09 


6 

228 

0.04 

=h 

0.02 

0.00 

1.50 

10.5 

0.40 

sfc 

0.12 

0.13 


7 

152 

0.06 

=1= 

0.02 

0.00 

2.12 

12.5 

0.44 

db 

0.11 

0.32 

16-20 

3 

64 

0.05 

d= 

0.03 

0.00 

1.79 

21.9 

0.25 

=b 

0.13 

0.07 


5 

516 

0.14 

=t: 

0.02 

0.00 

5.19 

40.3 

0.34 

d= 

0.04 

0.09 


6 

230 

0.15 

db 

0.03 

0.00 

5.42 

40.4 

0.36 

zb 

0.06 

0.09 


7 

155 

0.17 

db 

0.03 

0.003 

6.62 

50.3 

0.33 

db 

0.06 

0.16 

21-25 

5 

262 

0.14 

d= 

0.02 

0.00 

6.04 

45.8 

0.30 

=b 

0.04 

0.09 


6 

178 

0.21 

db 

0.04 

0.01 

8.35 

52.8 

0.40 

d= 

0.06 

0.09 


7 

143 

0.17 


0.03 

0.02 

7.08 

53.1 

0.32 

zb 

0.05 

0.21 


Parental Occupational Class 6 


Adol.-15 

5 

98 

0.03 =fc 0.02 

0.00 

0.86 

12.2 

0.26 ±0.12 

0.08 


6 

1048 

0.05 =±=0.01 

0.00 

1.84 

16.7 

0.31 ±0.04 

0.09 


7 

244 

0.02 ±0.01 

0.00 

0.74 

9.0 

0.24 ±0.10 

0.08 

16-20 

5 

100 

0.12 ±0.04 

0.00 

3.36 

34.0 

0.37 ±0.10 

0.08 


6 

1021 

0.14 ±0.01 

0.00 

5.47 

46.4 

0.30 ±0.02 

0.09 


7 

246 

0.14 ±0.02 

0.00 

4.48 

48.0 

0.30 ±0.04 

0.09 

21-25 

5 

71 

0.21 ±0.07 

0.00 

6.96 

42.3 

0.50 ±0.15 

0.09 


6 

554 

0.16 ± 0.02 

0.00 

7.02 

46.2 

0.35 ±0.03 

0.09 


7 

236 

0.23 ±0.03 

0.04 

7.93 

61.0 

0.37 ±0.05 

0.10 

26-30 

6 

103 

0.10 ±0.03 

0.00 

4.36 

39.8 

0.26 ±0.06 

0.08 


7 

97 

0.15 ±0.03 

j 0.004 

5.15 

50.5 

0.31 ±0.05 

0.09 


Parental Occupational Class 7 


Adol.-15 

7 

414 

0.07 ±0.02 

0.00 

2.21 

15.0 

0.45 ±0.08 

0.13 

16-20 

7 

416 

0.18 ±0.03 

0.00 

5.99 

49.5 

0.37 ±0.05 

0.09 

21-25 

7 

266 

0.23 ±0.05 

0.02 

8.73 

55.6 

0.42 ±0.08 

0.15 


Table 1 10. Petting to climax, in relation to occupational class of parent and of 

subject 

All data based on single males. 




430 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

GROUP 

OCCUP. 

CLASS 

REACHED 

BY 

SUBJECT 

CASES 

Non-marital Intercourse and Occupational Class 

TOTAL POPULATION 

active POPULATION 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- ] 
dian ' 
Freq. 

%0f 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

Parental Occupational Class 2 

Adol.-15 

2 

231 

0.59 ±0.08 

0.00 

22.57 

46.8 

1.27 =1=0.15 

0.75 


3 

126 

0.90 =±=0.17 

0.00 

34.33 

46.0 

1.95 ±0.31 

1.13 

16-20 

2 

198 

1.20 =t: 0.12 

0.51 

39.74 

79.3 

1.52 ±0.14 

0.88 


3 

116 

j 1.90=1=0.32 

0.55 

54.47 

85.3 

2.23 =±=0.36 

0.86 

21-25 

2 

92 

0.95 ±0.17 

0.34 

39.28 

77.2 

1.23 =±0.21 

0.50 


Parental Occupational Class 3 


Adol.-15 


2 


140 

0.56 =±0.11 

0.00 ; 

21.39 ! 

41.4 

1.35 =±0.22 

0.83 



3 


384 

1.00 =±0.11 

0.00 

32.95 

45.6 

2.19 =±0.22 

1.13 



5 


205 

0.22 =±0.07 

0.00 1 

7.81 

17.1 

1.29 =±0.33 

0.37 


6 

+ 

7 

92 

0.06 =±0.03 

0.00 

2.05 

5.4 

1.05 =±0.43 

1.00 

16-20 i 


2 


138 

1.20 =±0.12' 

0.56 

42.45 

79.0 

1.52 =±0.14 

1.11 



3 


318 

1.76 =±0.17 

0.50 

50.81 

77.4 

2.27 =±0.20 

1.07 



5 


201 

0.46 =±0.11 

0.00 

16.17 

42.8 

1.07 ±0.24 

0.31 


6 

+ 

7 

94 

0.29 =±0.09 

0.00 

9.46 

41.5 

0.71 ±0.19 

0.19 

21-25 


2 


64 

1.19 =±0.22 

0.38 

49.33 

75.0 

1.58 ±0.27 

0.88 



3 


120 

1.90 =±0.31 

0.58 

57.40 

81.7 

2.33 ±0.37 

0.92 



5 


124 

0.65 =±0.13 

0.04 

24.50 

55.6 

1.17 ±0.21 

0.39 


6 

+ 

7 

87 

0.58 =±0.11 

0.10 

21.79 

64.4 

0.89 ±0.15 

0.44 


Parental Occupational Class 4 


Adol.-15 

3 

107 

1.17 ±0.20 

0.16 

32.87 

53.3 

2.19 ±0.31 

1.38 


4 i 

158 

0.48 ±0.12 

0.00 

18.97 

36.1 

1.33 ±0.32 

0.43 


5 

304 

0.29 =±0.09 

0.00 

9.69 

20.1 

1.42 =± 0.44 

0.34 


6 ! 

109 

0.03 ±0.02 

0.00 

0.89 

4.6 

0.55 ±0.44 

0.10 


7 

74 

S 

d 

■H 

o 

o 

d 

0.00 

0.06 

4.1 

0.05 =± 

0.07 

16-20 

3 

105 

2.17 =±0.28 

1.00 

56.84 

92.4 

2.35 ±0.29 

1.50 


4 

104 

1,01 =±0.20 

0.14 

37.24 

66.3 

1.53 ±0.28 

0.63 


5 

305 

0.56 =±0.11 

0.00 

19.14 

47.2 

1.19 ±0.22 

0.31 


6 

111 

0.29 ±0.08 

0.00 

9.62 

37.8 

0.76 ±0.18 

0.21 


7 

75 

0.30 =±0.13 

0.00 

9.34 

38.7 

0.78 ±0.33 

0.16 

21-25 

5 

183 

0.78 =±0.18 

0.06 

27.50 

59.0 

1.32 ±0.29 

0.35 

- 

6 

98 

'0.44 ±0.08 

0.06 

17.07 

i 62.2 

0.70 ±0.11 

0.33 


7 

71 

0.58 ±0.14 

0.07 

22.94 

56.3 

1.03 ±0.22 

0.46 


{Table continued on next page) 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


431 


I Non-marital Intercourse and Occupational Class 

occur. 

CLASS 

REACHED CASES 
BY 

SUBJECT 


Parental Occupational C lass 5 

36.51 53.8 2.26 ±0.53 1.25 

2.74 14.9 0.50 ±0.08 0.25 

2.14 7.5 0.81 ± 0.30 0.33 

0.40 7.9 0.13 ± 0.04 0.08 

50.76 79.7 1.91 ±0.39 0.94 

9.39 38.8 0.63 ± 0.07 0.27 

5.86 40.4 0.39 ±0.06 0.19 

8.08 39.4 0.51 ±0.09 0.25 

15.87 50.0 0.73 ±0.10 0.34 

12.97 59.0 0.55 ±0.08 0.27 

23.29 67.1 0.82 ±0.12 0.39 

Class 6 

Adol.-15 5 98 0.21 ±0.07 0.00 5.78 20.4 1.04 ± 0.28 0.65 

6 1048 0.09 ±0.02 0.00 3.06 10.1 0.85 ±0.21 0.26 

7 244 0.12 ±0.07 0.00 4.20 7.8 1.59 ±0.81 0.20 

16-20 5 100 0.58 ±0.15 0.00 15.70 49.0 1.19 ± 0.29 0.46 

6 1021 0.23 ±0.03 0.00 9.04 39.4 0.59 ±0.07 0.20 

7 246 0.23 ±0.04 0.00 7.10 37.8 0.60 ±0.09 0.27 

21-25 5 71 0.53 ± 0.15 0.02 17.57 52.1 1.02 ±0.26 0.54 

6 554 0.39 ±0.04 0.02 16.72 52.2 0.74 ±0.07 0.32 

7 236 0.71 ±0.11 0.07 24.89 63.1 1.12 ±0.16 0.36 

26-30 6 103 0.68 ±0.17 0.05 28.76 55.3 1.22 ± 0.28 0.46 

7 97 1.15 ±0.21 0.26 38.35 63.9 1.80 ±0.31 1.08 

Married 

21-25 6 113 0.11 ±0.09 0.00 3.05 15.9 0.70 ±0.54 0.13 

7 76 0.16 ±0.12 0.00 3.55 17.1 0.93 ±0.66 0.23 

26-30 6 112 0.05 ±0.01 0.00 1.46 27.7 0.16 ± 0.03 0.08 

7 135 0.18 ± 0.07 0.00 5.02 28.1 0.63 ± 0.23 0.15 


Parental Occupational Class 7 


Adol.-15 

7 

414 

0.08 ±0.03 

0.00 

2.62 

7.5 

1.06 ±0.40 

0,19 

16-20 

7 

416 

0.23 ±0.05 

0.00 

7.59 

35.3 

0.65 ±0.14 

0.21 

21-25 

7 

266 

0.46 ±0.09 

0.003 

17.20 

50.4 

0.91 ±0.17 

0.22 


Table 111. Non-marital intercourse in relation to occupational class of parent 

and of subject 


All data based on single males except where indicated as “married.” Data for single 
males cover pre-marital intercourse with companions only. Data for married males 
cover extra-marital intercourse with both companions and prostitutes. 


Adol.-15 

3 

65 

1.22 ±0.32 

0.10 


5 

563 

0.08 ±0.01 

0.00 


6 

228 

0.06 ±0.03 

0.00 


1 7 

152 

1 

0.01 ±0.004 

0.00 

16-20 

3 

64 

1.52 ±0.33 

0.53 


5 

516 

0.25 ±0.03 

0.00 


6 

230 

0.16 ±0.03 

0.00 


7 

155 

0.20 ±0.04 

0.00 

21-25 

5 

262 

0.37 ±0.06 

0.001 


6 

178 

0.32 ±0.05 

0.05 


7 

143 

0.55 ±0.09 

0.14 


Parental Occupational 


AGE 

GROUP 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


433 


and by their very low frequencies of intercourse. Conversely, the boys from 
class 4 homes who will ultimately drop back into a group of semi-skilled 
workmen, masturbate less frequently and have a considerable amount of 
pre-marital intercourse before they are 15 (Tables 108, 111). Because group 
4 is so unstable, it should provide the very best material to be found for the 
study of the forces which control the development of sexual patterns, and 
particularly those forces which lead an individual to diverge from the 
patterns of his parents. 

Occupational Qass 5. This is the lower white collar group. It includes 
persons who work for the most part indoors at positions demanding some 
mental ability but usually not a great deal of training. The educational 
background of the group (Table 80) is a high school education of some sort 
in a high proportion of the cases, and at least some college work in 44 per 
cent of the cases. The group is more stable than occupational class 4, but 
it is less stable than any other white collar group. About 19 per cent of its 
children drop back into laboring groups and into the trades. However, 
53 per cent of the homes in class 5 send their children onto college and into 
occupations which give them higher social status. 

The sexual patterns of class 5 represent close approximations to the 
patterns of the upper white collar classes 6 and 7, as regards masturbation 
and nocturnal emissions (Tables 108, 109). The group has a good deal more 
pre-marital intercourse than the males of occupational classes 6 and 7 
(Table 111) and it has a great deal more homosexual activity than classes 
6 and 7 (Table 1 14), but it does not match the high frequencies which 
lower occupational classes have in heterosexual coitus and in the homo- 
sexual. 

Occupational Class 6. This is an upper white collar group whose mem- 
bers have college or graduate school training in about 90 per cent of the 
cases. Obviously, this was not so in past generations, but there will be an 
increasing amount of college training for this group in the future. Class 6 
is a remarkably stable group, with 40 per cent of its offspring remaining 
in the same class and another 40 per cent moving up into professional 
class 7 (Table 106). Since the group is so exclusively college in its educa- 
tional background, its pattern is typical of that described in Chapter 10 
for the college level. This means that it depends primarily upon mastur- 
bation for its pre-marital outlet, but has pre-marital intercourse with 
frequencies that are only one-sixth or one-eighth as high as those among 
the boys of corresponding age in class 3 (Tables 108, 111). The males of 
occupational class 6 are derived from parental homes which rate anything 
from 2 to 8 ; but irrespective of the origins of these males, the fact that they 
are headed for class 6 is abundantly evident in their early adolescent years, 
if not before. 



434 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Intercourse with Prostitutes and Occupational Class 


AGE 

GROUP 


OCCUP. 








CLASS 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

REACHED 

CASES 







BY 








SUBJECT 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Indd. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Parental Occupational Class 2 


Adol.-15 

2 

231 

0.03 

=t0.02 

0.00 

1.26 

6.1 

0.55 ±0.28 

0.09 


3 

126 

0.01 

=fc:0.007 

0.00 

0.41 

5.6 

0.19 ±0.11 

0.08 

16-20 

2 

198 

0.14 

±0.03 

0.00 

4.60 

39.4 

0.35 ±0.06 

0.10 


3 

116 

0.16 

±0.04 

0.00 

4.53 

40.5 

0.39 ±0.09 

0.13 

21-25 

2 

92 

0.39 

if 

o 

b 

0.07 

15.99 

66.3 

0.58 ±0.12 

0.31 


Parental Occupational Class 3 


Adol.-15 


2 


140 

0.04 

±0.03 

0.00 

1.62 

6.4 

0.66 ±0.40 

0.30 



3 


384 

0.01 

±0.004' 

0.00 

0.47 

8.1 

0.18 ±0.04 

0.08 



5 


205 

0.01 

±0,005 

0.00 

0.35 

2.9 

0.34 ±0.10 

0.35 


6 

+ 

7 

92 

0.004=^0.003 

0.00 

0.13 

2.2 

0.18±0.13 

0.30 

16-20 


2 


138 

0.15 

±0.03 

0.00 

5.41 

41.3 

0.37 ±0.07 

0.12 



3 


318 

0.17 

±0.03 

0.00 

4.85 

45.3 

0.37 ±0.06 

0.10 



5 


201 

0.05 

±0.01 

0.00 

1,82 

25.9 

0.20 ±0.05 

0.07 


6 

4 - 

7 

94 

0.02 

±0.01 

0.00 

0.70 

17.0 

0.13 ±0.05 

0.07 

21-25 


2 


64 

0.32 

±0.09 

0,01 

1 13.25 

51.6 

0.62 ±0.17 

0.28 



3 


120 

0.35 

±0.08 

0.03 

10.66 

55.8 

0.63 ±0.14 

0.30 


1 

5 


124 

0.07 

±0.02 

0.00 

2.51 

21.0 

0.32 ±0.09 

0.08 


! 6 

4 * 

7 

87 

0.02 

±0.01 

0.00 

0.80 

' 25.3 

0.08 ±0.02 

0.06 


Parental Occupational Class 4 


AdoL-15 

3 

107 

0.01 

±0.02 

0.00 

0.34 

10.3 

0.12 

ds 

0.04 

0.08 


4 

158 

0.01 

±0.007 

0.00 

0.52 

4.4 

0.30 


0.14 

0.10 


5 

304 

0.003 ±0.002 

0.00 

0.10 

3.0 

0.11 

:±= 

0.04 

0.07 


6 

109 

0.003 ±0.003 

1 

0.00 

0.09 

0.9 

0.30 

d= 


0.50 

16-20 

3 

105 

0.10 

±0.02 

0.02 

2.62 

57.1 

0.18 


0.03 

0.08 


4 

104 

0.17 

±0.05 

0.00 

6.28 

44.2 

0.39 

=b 

0.09 

i 0.09 


5 , 

305 

0.04 

±0.01 

0.00 

1.46 

27.2 

0.16 

=b 

0.04 

0.07 


6 i 

111 

0.03 

±0.01 

0.00 

0.89 

17.1 

0.16 

=±= 

0.07 

0.07 


7 

75 

0.01 

±0.006 

0.00 

0.43 

14.7 

0.10 

1 


0.03 

0.07 

21-25 

5 

183 

0.06 

±0,02 

0.00 

2.25 

26.2 

^0.24 

=b 

0.07 

0.08 


6 

98 

0.03 

±0.01 

0.00 

1.09 

15.3 

0.18 

=i= 

0.05 

0.09 


7 

71 

0.01 

±0.004 

0.00 

0.38 

12,7 

0.08 

=b 

0.01 

0.06 


{Table continued on next page) 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


435 





Intercourse with Prostitutes and Occupational Class 

AGE 

GROUP 

OCCUR. 


TOTAL population 

ACTIVE population 

CLASS 

CASES 








REACHED i 

BY 

SUBJECT 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Indd. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Parental Occupational Class 5 


AdoL-15 

3 

65 

0.01 ± 0.007 

0.00 

0.32 

6.2 

0.18 ± 0.08 

0.20 


5 

563 

0.006 ± 0.003 

0.00 

0.22 

3.6 

0.18 ± 0.07 

0.08 


6 

228 

0 . 001 ± 









0.0004 

0.00 

0.02 

1.3 

0.05 

0.07 


7 

152 

0 . 0003 ± 









0.0003 

0.00 

0.01 

0.7 

; o . o5 

0.10 

16-20 

3 

64 

0.11 ± 0.03 

0.01 

3.75 

53.1 

, 0.21 ± 0.04 

0.09 


5 

516 

0.03 ± 0.004 

0.00 

1.04 

23.8 

0.12 ± 0.02 

0.06 


6 

230 

0.02 ± 0.01 

0.00 

0.75 

14.8 

0.14 ± 0.06 

0.06 


7 

155 

0.02 ± 0.01 

0.00 

0.60 

14.2 

0.11 ± 0.06 

0.06 

21-25 

5 

262 

0.06 ± 0.02 

0.00 

2.40 

24.8 

0.22 ± 0.06 

0.08 


6 

178 

0.01 ± 0.005 

0.00 

0.53 

; 12.9 

0.10 ± 0.04 

0.06 


7 

143 

0.02 ± 0.01 

0.00 

0.98 

18.9 

0.12 ± 0.06 

0.06 


Parental Occupational Class 6 


Adol.-15 

5 

98 

0.005 ± 0.003 

0.00 • 

0.12 ' 

4.1 

0.11 ± 0.07 

0.08 


6 

1048 

0 . 005 ± 0.003 

0.00 j 

0.18 

2.5 

0.21 ± 0.09 

0.06 


7 

244 

0 . 001 ± 









0.0005 

0.00 

0.04 

2.5 

0.05 

0.06 

16-20 

5 

100 

0.02 ± 0.005 

0.00 

0.54 

25.0 

0.08 ± 0.02 

0.06 


6 

1021 

0.02 ± 0.004 

0.00 1 

0.90 

20.4 

0.11 ± 0.02 

0.06 


7 

246 

0.04 ± 0.01 

0.00 

1.11 

17.9 

0.20 ± 0.05 

0.08 

21-25 

5 

71 

0.03 ± 0.01 

0.00 

0.99 

! 25.4 

0.12 ± 0.03 

0.07 


6 

554 

0.03 ± 0.005 

0.00 

1.15 

18.2 

0.15 ± 0.02 

0.07 


7 

236 

0.08 ± 0.05 

0.00 

2.87 

21.6 

! 0.38 ± 0.22 

0.07 

26-30 

6 

1 

103 

0.04 ± 0,02 

0.00 

1.84 

18.4 

0,23 ± 0.10 

0.08 


7 

97 

j0.20 ± 0.16 

0.00 

6.63 

16.5 

1.20 ± 0.96 

0.09 


Parental Occupational Class 7 


Adol.-isi 

7 

414 

0 . 004 ± 0.002 

0.00 

0.12 

2.9 

0.13 ± 0.04 

0.08 

16-20 

7 

416 

0.02 ± 0.005 

0.00 

0.70 

15.9 

0.13 ± 0.03 

0.06 

21-25 

7 

266 

0.02 ± 0.006 

0.00 

0.78 

13.5 

0.15 ± 0.04 

0.07 


Table 113. Intercourse with prostitutes in relation to occupational class of parent 

and of subject 


All data based on single males. 



436 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Occupational Class 7. This is the professional group which, by definition, 
has better than college training in 99 per cent of the cases (Table 106). 
About 65 per cent of the offspring of this group go into the professions, and 
consequently belong to the same occupational class as their parents ; but 
nearly one-fourth of the offspring of the group drops back to occupational 
class 6. Only a small portion of the persons in occupational class 7 are 
derived from homes which are anything but class 6 or 7 (Table 106). It is, 
nevertheless, intensely interesting to find that those males who do get into 
class 7 out of parental homes which rated 4 and 5, have class 7 patterns 
early in their teens. Indeed, the class 4 males who ultimately arrive at class 
7 have the most restrained socio-sexual histories in this whole group, and 
depend upon masturbation more exclusively than the class 7 males who 
are derived from any other parental background (Table 115). It is as 
though the bigger the move which the boy makes between his parental 
class and the class toward which he aims, the more strict he is about lining 
up his sexual history with the pattern of the group into which he is going 
to move. If this were done consciously, it would be more understandable; 
but considering that the boy in actuality knows very little about the sexual 
behavior of the social group into which he is moving, it is all the more 
remarkable to find that these patterns are laid down at such an early age. 

VERTICAL MOBILITY: AT LATER AGES 

It is a relatively small number of individuals who start with the sexual 
pattern of the parental social level, stay with it through their teens and 
perhaps for some years beyond, and finally move into some other social 
level. 

There are some cases of males who have dropped back into a distinctly 
lower social level, after they had been well started in the parental class. 
Such cases are relatively few. These males are the “black sheep” of the 
community, who amount to something less than what was expected of 
them, or the persons who become involved in some maze of social circum- 
stances which brings economic or social disaster. Men in Salvation Army 
homes or over-night hotels have supplied a number of histories of this 
sort. The underworld occasionally contributes the history of a man with 
a degree of Ph.D. or of M.D. who has turned to illicit activities and to 
loafing for an occupation. 

Vertical mobility which did not start upward until after the late teens is 
found occasionally among males who stop school, find employment as 
laborers or in the trades for a period of years, and only later decide to go 
to college. These are the individuals who come into contact with some per- 
son or persons, or with some particular circumstance which encourages 
them to go back to school some time after they have left it. These are the 
persons who have enough ability to succeed in business and who are thus 
able to achieve social position because of their acquired financial status. 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


437 


These are the persons who are encouraged or forced, by some particular 
circumstance, to consider the future in terms which had never appeared 
in their previous thinking, and who may be given specific aid for such an 
undertaking. Many GFs who are attending the colleges and universities 
of the country would not be going to college now if they were not subsidized 
by public funds, and many of them would still not be going to college if 
there were not a considerable public sentiment in this country for the GI 
to utilize the most of his opportunities. 

The sexual records of these males are most significant, but we do not 
yet have enough cases to warrant a statistical manipulation of the data. 
However, it is safe to generahze so far as to say that males who have lower 
level patterns in their early adolescent years, and who keep their lower level 
patterns through their teens, usually retain their lower level patterns when 
they finally go to college or professional school, and throughout the rest 
of their lives. Even though they may subsequently engage in the professions 
and acquire considerable social position, they do not usually adopt the 
upper level sexual patterns. A male from this group may keep his lower 
level pattern even though he may subsequently become a judge on the 
bench, a physician, a psychiatrist, or a successful business man. This is, of 
course, exactly in line with the conclusions drawn for those males who 
departed from the parental pattern in their early years. In both cases, it is 
a matter of patterns of behavior being laid down by early or middle 
adolescence; and of practically nothing, either in the parental background 
or in the subsequent migration of the individual to other social levels, 
modifying those patterns in subsequent years. The judge with the lower 
level background excuses pre-marital intercourse and objects to mastur- 
bation, even though all of his colleagues on the same bench may have 
different, upper level ideas on the subject. The successful business man who 
has risen from lower levels never gives up his early acceptance of pre- 
marital intercourse, but continues to condemn what he calls the sophisti- 
cated sexual techniques of the upper level into which he has moved. The 
physician whose own history began with a lower level pattern expects to 
find pre-marital intercourse in the histories of his patients, and may recom- 
mend intercourse to them as a matter of therapy. He has a greater tolerance 
of extra-marital intercourse; but he may lecture before the local high 
school on the dangers of masturbation. He may assure his patients that 
petting as a substitute for coitus is likely to lead to all sorts of nervous dis- 
orders and neurotic disturbances. He condemns mouth-genital contacts, 
and insists that simple and direct heterosexual coitus provides the only 
normal sex fife. Such physicians may imply that they have scientific author- 
ity for these opinions, when in actuality they are merely verbalizing the 
standards of the social level in which they were raised. 



438 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

GROUP 

OCCUP. 

CLASS 

REACHED 

BY 

SUBJECT 

CASES 

Homosexual Outlet and Occupational Class 

total population 

active population 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%Of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

Parental Occupational Class 2 

Adol.-15 

2 

231 

0,37 =fc0.06 

0.00 

14.25 

32.9 

1.14±0.14 

0.73 


3 

126 

0.20 ±0.08 

0.00 

7.51 

27.0 

0.73 ±0.30 

0.23 

16-20 

2 

198 

0.33 =fc0.05 

0.00 

10.85 

34.3 

0.96 ±0.11 

0.66 


3 

116 

0.21 ±0.08 

0.00 

6.06 

26.7 

0.79 ±0.27 

0.23 

21-25 

2 

92 

0.18 ±0.05 

0.00 

7.29 

26.1 

0.68 ±0.16 

0.36 


Parental Occupational Class 3 


Adol.-15 

2 

140 

0.27 ±0.07 

0.00 

10.50 

30.0 

0.91 ±0.21 

0.36 


3 

384 

0.26 ±0.05 

0.00 

8.66 

29.4 

0.89 ±0.15 

0.37 


5 

205 

0.16 ±0.04 

0.00 

5.53 

25.4 

0.61 ±0.15 

0.10 


6 + 7 

92 

0.09 ±0.05 

0.00 

3.05 

22.8 

0.37 ±0.22 

0.08 

16-^20 

2 

138 

0.23 ±0.06 

0.00 

8.27 

29.0 

0.81 ±0.17 

0.41 


3 

318 

0.22 ±0.05 

0.00 

6.32 

27.4 

0.80±0.16 

0.27 


5 

201 

0.27 ±0.09 

0.00 

9.67 

24,4 

1.12 ±0.35 

0.28 


6 + 7 

94 

0.04 ±0.02 

0.00 

1.41 

13.8 

0.32 ±0.14 

0.10 

21-25 

2 

64 

0.21 ±0.10 

0.00 

8.93 

15.6 

1.38 ±0.52 

0.66 


3 

120 

0.23 ±0.13 

0.00 

6.95 

15.0 

1.53 ±0.80 

0.45 


5 

124 

0.43 ±0.14 

0.00 

16.13 

21.8 

1.97 ±0.57 

1.00 


6 + 7 

87 

0.05 ±0.03 

0.00 

2.00 

8.0 

0.66 ±0.26 

0.50 

Parental Occupational Class 4 

Adol.-15 

3 

107 

0.31 ±0.10 

0.00 

8.80 

28.0 

1.12 ±0.30 

0.33 


4 

158 

0.09 ±0.02 

0.00 

3.49 

31.0 

0.28 ±0.06 

0.09 


5 

304 

0.16 ±0.03 

0.00 

5.32 

22.7 

0.69 ±0.12 

0.24 


6 

109 j 

0.22 ±0.08 

0.00 

7.78 

26.6 

0.83 ±0.29 

0.20 


7 

74 

0.05 ±0,03 

0.00 

1.47 

10.8 

0.43 ±0.27 

0.17 

16-20 

3 

105 

0.27 ±0.11 

0.00 

7.08 

31.4 

0.86 ±0.32 

0.22 


4 

104 

0.11 ±0.06 

0.00 

4.15 

28.8 

0.39 ±0.19 

0.08 


5 

305 

0.20 ±0.04 

0.00 

6.77 

21.6 

0.92 ±0.16 

0.27 


6 

111 

0.27 ±0.10 

0.00 

9.18 

20.7 

1.32 ±0.39 

0.30 


7 

75 

0.03 ±0.02 

0.00 

0.90 

12.0 

0.24 ±0.14 

0.08 

21-25 

5 

183 

0.32 ±0.09 

0.00 

11.40 

21.3 

1.51 ±0.38 

0.83 


6 

98 

0.27 ±0.10 

0.00 

10.43 

18.4 

1.45 =±=0.44 

0.35 


7 

71 

0.02 ± 0.01 

0.00 

0.66 

1 5.6 

0.30 ±0.18 

0.30 


(Table continued on next page) 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


439 




i 

Homosexual Outlet and Occupational Class 


OCCUP. 


i 


1 




AGE 

GROUP 

CLASS 


total population 

1 ACTIVE POPULATION 

REACHED 

CASES 







BY 









SUBJECT 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Parental Occupational Class 5 


Adol.-15 

3 

65 

0.15 =l0.06 

0.00 

4.47 

29.2 

0.51 ±0.18 

0.10 


5 

563 

0.15 ±0.02 

0.00 

5.31 

26.6 

0.55 ±0.06 

0.18 


6 

228 

0.09 ±0.03 

0.00 

3.21 

19.7 

0.46 ±0.14 

0.09 


7 

152 

0.06 ±0.02 

0.00 

2.37 

22.4 

0.28 ±0.08 

0.08 

16^20 

3 

64 

0.23 ±0.09 

0.00 

7.50 

35.9 

0.63 ±0.24 

0.10 


5 

516 

0.14 ±0.02 

0.00 

5.34 

21.5 

0.65 ±0.09 

0.12 


6 

230 

0.07 ±0.03 

0.00 

2.69 

15.2 

0.48 ±0.15 

0.09 


7 

155 

0.04 ± 0.02 

0.00 

1.65 

11.6 

0.36 ±0.13 

0.08 

21-25 

5 

262 

0.21 ±0.06 

0.00 

9.07 

15.6 

1.34±0.36 

0.50 


6 

178 

0.10 ±0.04 

0.00 

3.84 

9.6 

1.01 ±0.34 

0.34 


7 

143 

0.05 ±0.03 

0.00 

2.22 

7.7 

0.69 ±0.29 

0.30 


Parental Occupational Class 6 


Adol.-15 

i 5 

98 

0.45 ±0.12 

0.00 

12.34 

41.8 

1.08 ±0.25 

0.32 


6 

1048 

0.08 ±0,01 

0.00 ! 

2.77 

1 22.7 

0.34 ±0.04 

0.08 


7 

244 

0.10 ±0.03 

0.00 

3.37 

22.1 

0.45 ±0.12 

0.10 

16-20 

5 

100 

0.62 ±0.15 

0.00 

16.85 

44.0 

1.42 ±0.29 

0.88 


6 

1021 

0.06 ±0.01 

0.00 

2.24 

16.7 

0.35 ±0.06 

0.09 


7 

246 

0,07 ±0.03 

0.00 

2.10 

14.2 

0.47 ±0.20 

0.08 

21-25 

5 

71 

0.67 ±0.19 

0.00 

22.21 

32.4 

2.08 ±0.47 

1.70 


6 

554 

0.10 ±0.02 

0.00 

4.20 

10.6 

0,91 ±0.19 

0.28 


7 

236 

0.01 ±0.005 

0.00 

0.49 

6.8 

0.21 ±0.07 

0,09 

26-30 

6 

103 

0.27 ±0.09 ' 

0.00 

11.68 

24.3 

1.13 ±0.30 

0,42 


7 

97 1 

0.01 ±0.01 1 

1 

0.00 

0.43 

5.2 

0.25 ±0.15 

0.10 


Parental Occupational Class 7 


Adol.-15 

7 

414 

0.07 ±0.01 

0.00 

2.38 

23.2 

0.31 ±0.05 

0.09 

16-20 

7 

416 

0,03 ±0,01 

0.00 

0.94 

13.7 

0.21 ±0.05 

0.07 

21-25 

7 

266 

0.02 ±0,01 

0.00 

0.74 

5.6 

0.35 ±0.25 

0.07 


Table 114. Homosexual outlet in relation to occupational class of parent and 

of subject 

All data based on single males. 



440 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


TRANSMISSION OF SEXUAL MORES 

When we understand the processes by which the sexual mores are 
stabilized in each social group, and transmitted to each and all of the mem- 
bers of the group, we shall have gone a considerable way toward under- 
standing some of the most fundamental of social phenomena. If we under- 
stood the forces which lead some boys to ignore the attitudes and expressed 
sexual philosophies of their parents, and even of their companions in the 
community in which they are raised, we should have the key to problems 
that are basic in genetic psychology. If we knew by what processes a boy 
acquires the patterns of a social level in which he is not living and into 
which he will only ultimately move, we should know a great deal more than 
we do today. 

It is a far simpler matter to understand how children acquire their habits 
in regard to dressing, eating, and other behavioral activities. It is much 
simpler to discern the processes by which they learn to speak the mother 
tongue. But since there is a minimum of verbal instruction on matters of 
sex, since the child is rarely lectured in regard to attitudes on sex, and since 
it almost never observes adult sexual activity, sex education is a subtle 
process which, nevertheless, is powerful enough to force most children, 
somewhere during pre-adolescent or early adolescent years, into becoming 
conforming machines which rarely fail to perpetuate the mores of the 
community. 

We can record the fact of vertical mobility in the social organization; we 
can figure statistics on the number of persons who make such moves and 
the directions in which they move. In all of psychology and sociology there 
is, however, next to no information on the factors which affect this move- 
ment from out of a parental group into a new social status. That a con- 
siderable number of individuals should aspire to move into levels that have 
greater prestige is quite understandable; but that does not explain why 
certain individuals rather than others are the ones who make these moves. 
We have been able to show that sexual attitudes and overt experience in 
sexual activities are closely correlated with the educational and occupa- 
tional class into which an individual ultimately moves, after he has broken 
with his parental background but very often before he has ever left the 
parental home. But this still falls short of identifying the impetus which 
stirs that individual to make such moves. 

As yet we have only hypotheses about the sources of the inspiration 
which leads this boy to make the break with his parental pattern, and as 
yet we can only cite specific instances in support of our preliminary think- 
ing. We can point to the father whose contacts with the upper level lead 
him to associate upper level sexual patterns with upper level success in 
social and business affairs. His contacts may not affect his own sexual per- 
formance, but they may be significant enough to lead him to encourage a 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


441 


pattern for his son which differs from his own. It is probable that the 
mother is even more often responsible for the boy’s sexual restraint. It is 
often she who encourages him to associate with proper, well-behaved, and 
similarly restrained upper level companions. On the other hand, there are 
cases of boys who make these moves in the face of parental objections. 
Some boys complete high school only over the parental protest and 
ultimately go to college without parental support and sometimes in the 
face of considerable opposition from their homes. The boy’s companions 
in school, in church, and elsewhere, may take him away from his com- 
panions in the community in which he actually lives. Sometimes adults 
other than the parents have something to do with the boy’s acquisition of 
new attitudes and ideals. We shall need a great deal of additional informa- 
tion before we can appraise the relative significances of these several sources 
of influence, and of still others which we may not yet have recognized. 

Psychologists and psychiatrists will be inclined to suggest that the begin- 
nings of this conditioning should be searched for in very early childhood, 
and what few data we do have confirm such a theory. As noted earher in 
the present volume (Chapter 5), we have recently undertaken to secure 
sexual data from very young children and plan to publish a volume con- 
cerned entirely with these processes of learning. Although the data are not 
yet abundant enough to analyze statistically, we can make the following 
generalizations at this time: 

1. Some of the most fundamental distinctions between the social levels 
are already discernible in pre-adolescents as young as 3 and 4. The ease or 
embarrassment with which such a child discusses genitaha, excretory 
functions, anatomical distinctions between males and females, the possi- 
bility that there has been self manipulation of genitaha, the possibihty that 
there has been genital exhibition or genital play with other children, the 
question of the origin of babies, the merely social companionship with his 
own or the opposite sex, questions about kissing his parents and about 
kissing companions of his own or of the opposite sex — and kindred items 
— vindicate in practically every instance that the 3- or 4-year old child 
has already acquired something of the social attitudes on at least some of 
these issues. 

Social approval or disapproval means a great deal to a child of that age. 
It may not take more than a single adverse experience to make a child feel 
that he must not expose himself again to the laughter, the specific repri- 
mand, or physical punishment which accompanied his first performance. 
The disdainful ridicule of other children, the angry withdrawal of com- 
panions who disapprove of the child’s overt activity, the nervous amuse- 
ment of adults, are things that even the 3-year old does not wish to experi- 
ence again. 



442 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 



1 PAREN- 


Outlets and Parental Occupational Class 

Mean Frequencies, Total Population 

AGE 

GROUP 

TAL 

' OCCUP. 
CLASS 

CASES 

Total 

Outlet 

1 Mastur- 
bation 

Noctur- 
nal Emis- 
sions i 

1 

Petting ^ 
to 

Climax 

Coitus, 

Com- 

panions 

Coitus, 

Pros- 

titutes 

Homo- 

sexual 


Subject: Occupational Class 3 


Adol.-15 

2 

126 

2.68 

1.40 

0.06 

0.03 

0.90 

0.01 

0.20 

3 

384 

3.10 

1.57 

0.11 

0.04 

1.00 

0.01 

0.26 


4 

107 

3.62 

1.94 

0.07 

0.04 

1.17 

0.01 

0.31 


5 

65 

3.38 

1.81 

0.05 

0.03 

1.22 

0.01 

0.15 

16-20 

2 

116 

3.46 

1.01 

0.16 

0.04 

1.90 

0.16 

0.21 


3 

318 

3.50 

1.00 

0.21 

0.07 

1.76 

0.17 

0.22 


4 i 

105 I 

3.82 

0.98 

0.24 

0.06 

2.17 

0.10 

0.27 


5 

64 1 

3.03 

0.88 

0.14 

0.05 

1.52 

0.11 

0.23 


Subject: Occupational Class 5 


Adol.-15 

3 

205 

2.85 

2.07 

0.27 

0.03 

0.22 

0.01 

0.16 


4 

304 

3.00 

2,18 

0.28 

0.03 

0.29 

0.003 

0.16 


5 

563 

2.77 

2.13 

0.31 

0.04 

0.08 

0.006 

0.15 


6 

98 

3.76 

2.74 

0.20 

0.03 

0.21 

0.005 

0.45 

16-20 

3 

201 

2.88 

1.57 

0.35 

0.10 

0.46 

0.05 

0.27 


4 

305 

2.97 

1.64 

0.38 

0.11 

0.56 

0.04 

0.20 


5 

516 

2.63 

1.68 

0.37 

0.14 

0.25 

0.03 

0.14 

1 

6 

100 

3.80 

1.94 

0.35 

0.12 

0.58 

0.02 

0.62 

21-25 

3 

124 

2.73 

1.09 

0.31 

0.11 

0.65 

0.07 

0.43 


4 

183 

2.86 

1.17 

0.35 

0.14 

0.78 

0.06 

0.32 


5 

262 

2 32 

1.22 

0.30 

0.14 

0.37 

0.06 

0.21 


6 

71 

3.13 

1.27 

0.31 

0.21 

0.53 

0.03 ! 

0.67 


Subject: Occupational Class 6 


Adol.-15 

4 

109 

2,85 

2.18 

0.33 

0.04 

0.03 

0.003 

0.22 


5 

228 

2.86 

2.17 

0.43 

0.04 

0.06 

0.001 

0.09 


6 

1048 

2.86 

2.29 

0.28 

0.05 

0.09 

0.005 

0.08 

16-20 

4 

111 

3.01 

1.71 

0.48 

0.20 

0.29 

0.03 

0.27 


5 

230 

2.72 

1.82 

0.47 

0.15 

0.16 

0.02 

0.07 


6 

1021 

2.58 

1.73 

0.38 

0.14 

0.23 

0.02 

0.06 

21-25 

4 

98 

2.55 

1,10 

0.46 

0.27 

0.44 

0.03 

0.27 


5 

178 

2.56 

1.43 

0.43 

0.21 

0.32 

0.01 

0.10 


6 

554 

2.38 

1.27 

0.37 

0.16 

0.39 

0.03 

0.10 


Subject: Occupational Class 7 


Adol.-15 

4 

74 

3.14 

2.56 

0.45 

0.04 

0.002 


0.05 


5 i 

152 

2.70 

2.05 

0.43 

0.06 

0.01 

0.0003 

0.06 


6 

244 

3.00 

2.24 

0.44 

0.02 

0.12 

0.001 

0.10 


7 

414 

3,05 

2.39 

0.40 

0.07 

0.08 

0.004 

0.07 

16-20 

4 

75 

3.23 

2.21 

0.55 

0.12 

0.30 

0.01 

0.03 


5 

155 

2.50 

1.60 

0.47 

0.17 

0.20 

0.02 

0.04 


6 

246 

3.18 

2.17 

0.54 

0.14 

0.23 

0.04 

0.07 


7 

416 

3.04 

2.12 

0.43 

0.18 

0.23 

0.02 

0.03 

21-25 

4 

71 

2.51 

1.37 

0.39 

0.17 

0.58 

! 0.01 

0.02 


5 

143 

2.43 

1.18 

0.40 

0.17 

0.55 

i 0.02 

! 0.05 


6 

236 

2.88 

1.38 

0.43 

0.23 

0.71 

t 0.08 

i 0.01 


7 

266 

2.71 

1.51 

0.43 

0.23 

0.46 

0.02 

0.02 


Table 115. Similarity of sexual frequencies of persons belonging to the same 

occupational class 

Emphasizing near identity of histories of subjects who reach the same occupational 
class, irrespective of the diverse occupational classes of their parents. Showing mean 
frequencies for total populations. Medians, incidences, data on active populations, 
standard deviations of means, etc., shown for same populations in previous tables 
in this chapter. 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


443 


Questioned concerning his behavior, the young child may deny that he 
has ever kissed or been kissed, that he has exposed his genitalia, that he 
has touched his own genitalia, that he has allowed other persons to touch 
his genitalia, or that he has touched the genitalia of other children. His 
denials are made with a nervous haste and apparent discomfort which make 
it apparent that he wants to leave the subject and not discuss such things 
further. The history of the army colonel who denied that he had ever had 
homosexual experience unless it happened at night, when he did not know 
anything about it, is matched by the history of the 4-year old boy who 
insisted that no other boys had touched him except when he was asleep. 
One is concerned not so much with ascertaining the actuality of the child’s 
overt experience, but rather with getting some measure of the nature of 
his emotional responses; for in those responses one may learn what values 
the child has already acquired, and how those values will shape his future 
behavior. 

2. Social attitudes are acquired long before the child may know that 
there is any significance to genital stimulation, much less intercourse. The 
so-called sex instruction which is given by parents and schools usually 
consists of a certain amount of information concerning the anatomy and 
mechanics of reproduction. As far as our present information goes, this 
has a minimum of any effect upon the development of patterns of sexual 
behavior and, indeed, it may have no effect at all. Patterns of behavior 
are the products of attitudes ; and attitudes may begin shaping long before 
the child has acquired very much, if any, factual information. 

3. Traditional attitudes toward heterosexual and homosexual relation- 
ships have been apparent in some of the 3- and 4-year old histories. The 
older pre-adolescent boys from upper social levels, however, were often 
more willing to admit their homosexual experience, less often willing to 
admit their heterosexual relationships. It is apparent that the attitudes of 
companions who consider it sissy to play with girls are predominant factors, 
both in the development of the child’s attitudes and in the shaping of his 
overt activity. By early adolescence, however, it is more difficult to obtain 
homosexual data from an upper level group, and simpler to obtain data of 
heterosexual contacts. The group has begun to attach values to hetero- 
sexuality, it has begun to recognize the taboos which older persons place 
on the homosexual. It is the attitude of the group that has changed, and 
not the independent thinking of the child. 

4. The lower level interest in heterosexual intercourse and frank accep- 
tance of it as a pre-marital activity is apparent in the histories of a high 
proportion of the 7- and 8-year old boys from those groups; and in some 
instances it is well developed as early as age 4. By ages 7 or 8 the lower 
level boy knows that intercourse is one of the activities in which most of 
his companions, at least his slightly older companions, are engaging; and 



444 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


SUBJECT OCCUPATIONAL CLASS 5 


PARENT 

CLASS MASTURBATION 

3 

S — I 


NOCTURNAL 

EMISSIONS 


^ s.' 


□ 


PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 



SDBJKT OCCUPATIONAL CLASS 5 



SUBJECT OCCUPATIONAL CLASS 7 



Figiire 124. Comparisons of sexual patterns of males of same occupational 
class but originating from diverse parental classes 

Comparing mean frequency data for the age period 16-20, for three sources of outlet, 
for single males of three occupational classes. 




STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


445 


he has already learned that intercourse is one of the things that are con- 
sidered highly desirable by those companions. Meanwhile, the 10-year old 
boy from the upper level home is likely to confine his pre-adolescent sex 
play to the exhibition and manual manipulation of genitalia, and he does 
not attempt intercourse because, in many instances, he has not yet learned 
that there is such a possibility. 

5. Children are the most frequent agents for the transmission of the 
sexual mores. Adults serve in that capacity only to a smaller extent. This 
will not surprise sociologists and anthropologists, for they are aware of 
the great amount of imitative adult activity which enters into the play gf 
children, the world around. In this activity, play though it may be, children 
are severe, highly critical, and vindictive in their punishment of a child who 
does not do it ‘This way,” or “that way.” Even before there has been any 
attempt at overt sex play, the child may have acquired a considerable 
schooling on matters of sex. Much of this comes so early that the adult has 
no memory of where his attitudes w^ere acquired. 

6. The mores may be imposed by the children of the community in 
defiance of the attempts of adults to impose other patterns. Lower level 
parents may punish their children for attempting intercourse, but the lower 
level 7-year old assures us with wide open eyes that he cannot understand 
why his mother should punish him, and he does not consider it wrong to 
attempt intercourse, because all of the other boys are doing it. Upon 
securing the history of the boy’s mother, it becomes apparent that the 
punishment she gave was quite perfunctory, and that deep in her own think- 
ing she does not exactly disapprove of pre-marital sexual relations, any- 
way. Even when the parents are sincere in their attempts to impose ideas 
that differ from those of the community, the children may triumph over the 
parents. Sometimes parents attempt to impose patterns which are stricter 
than those in the community. Sometimes they attempt to be more liberal, 
and try to raise the child without having it acquire fears and inhibitions 
concerning sex. In some cases the parents succeed, but in many cases they 
do not. In the further study of this problem it will be important to accumu- 
late specific data in such abundance that it will be possible, ultimately, 
to measure the relative importance of companions, parents, and other 
adults in the establishment of the child’s attitudes and patterns of overt 
behavior. 

Children are, on the whole, conformists. Their initial experiences with a 
particular object or event lead them to believe that the world is made in a 
particular way, and they are likely to conclude that the whole world should 
be made that way. Any departure in the placement of furniture, in the 
style of clothing which is worn, in the way in which food is served, or in 
the schedule of the day — the routine which is followed upon getting up in 
the morning or upon going to bed at night — may bring protests that “that 



446 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


is not the way to do it.” This is the sort of conformance that children are 
continually forcing upon each other in regard to all matters, including sex. 

7. The record given in this chapter makes it clear that exceedingly few 
males modify their attitudes on matters of sex or change their patterns of 
overt behavior in any fundamental way after their middle teens. Many 
individuals do acquire certain details of activity in their later years, and 
some individuals think that they have acquired entirely new attitudes on 
matters of sex, at some late period in their lives. Upper level individuals 
like to think that they have become more liberal, sexually emancipated, 
free of their former inhibitions, rational instead of traditional in their 
behavior, ready to experiment with anything. It is notable, however, that 
such emancipated persons rarely engage in any amount of actual behavior 
which is foreign to the pattern laid down in their youth. Such an individual 
may publicly discuss his changed attitudes, and may go so far as to engage 
in such a public display of petting as leads the community to believe that 
there is considerably more going on; but the actual history is not likely to 
contain more than a minimum of non-marital intercourse. The upper level 
male who comes back from an army experience with tales of the wild 
places where he has been, the freedom of the girls in the tropics, the endless 
chances he had for experience of every sort, the record of the particular 
girl with whom he became acquainted in this station, and the girl with 
whom he got in trouble in another station — may have to admit, when he 
contributes an objective record to a scientific study, that he never did 
bring himself to having actual intercourse with a single one of the girls. 
This is a long way from the sort of promiscuous pattern which is common- 
place in lower level histories. 

8. While the behavior of the adult is thus controlled by what he calls his 
conscience, he is also influenced by such social forces as public opinion. 
Among adults, this operates in much the same subtle way that community 
attitudes are passed on to the children. The tone of voice in which gossip 
is relayed warns the individual to avoid becoming a subject for similar 
gossip. The care and circumlocution with which certain matters of sex are 
avoided in books, in the press, and in other public communications, con- 
stantly remind the individual of the state of public opinion on these things. 
Discussions of such things as divorce, marital discord, the sexual scandals 
of the community, and the gossip about public characters probably have 
more influence in controlling the individual’s behavior than any specific 
action that society may take or any legal penalties that are attached to 
those things. 

9. The church and the other organizations that are chiefly concerned 
with problems of morals are, basically, the source of a good deal of the 
sexual philosophy of the community (Chapter 13). On occasion the church 
specifically condemns departures from its sex code, but more often it 



STABILITY OF SEXUAL PATTERNS 


447 


depends upon the less tangible concepts of purity, cleanliness, sin, un- 
cleanliness, degradation. The very indefiniteness of these characterizations 
makes them more inclusive. Each individual categorizes himself in accord- 
ance with the standards that are set up. He is often more severe to himself 
than his fellows would be if they were judging his record. To the religiously 
devout, moral values are considerable forces. Nevertheless, the patterns 
of the social levels are even more influential than the mandates of a religion 
(Chapter 13). 

10. The written legal codes and the proscriptions of the common law are 
much less influential in controlling the sexual behavior of the human 
animal. Patterns of behavior are established long before the child is likely 
to have any comprehension of the nature of the legal formalization of our 
codes. 

These observations may contribute to our understanding of the fact 
that individuals in our American society rarely adopt totally new patterns 
of sexual behavior after their middle teens. It would appear that the changes 
that do occur represent departures made by pre-adolescent and adolescent 
children from the patterns of their parents. We have at least progressed in 
our understanding of social forces when we have recognized these very 
early years as fundamental in the development of both individual and 
community patterns of sexual behavior. 






Total Outlet: Rural, Urban 


AGE 

RURAL- 

URBAN 

CASES 

TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

GROUP 

GROUP 


Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

Incid. Mean 

% Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 


Single Males: Educational Level 0-8 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

245 

2.41 ±0.18 

1.33 

89.4 

2.69 ± 0.20 

1.70 


Urban 

401 

3.40 ±0.21 

1.98 

92.0 

3.69 ± 0.22 

2.34 

16-20 

Rural 

259 

2.81 ±0.20 

1.85 

96.5 

2.91 ±0.20 

1.95 


Urban 

397 

3.43 ±0.19 

2.37 

98.2 

3.49 ±0.19 

2.43 

21-25 

Rural 

141 

2.80 ±0.29 

1.56 

95.0 

2.95 ±0.30 

1.78 


Urban 

188 

3.29 ±0.30 

1.97 

97.9 

3.36 ± 0.31 

2.01 

26-30 

Rural 

61 

3.01 ±0.53 

1.40 

98.4 

3.06 ± 0.54 

1.45 


Urban 

88 

3.04 ±0.34 

2.08 

98.9 

3.07 ± 0.34 

2.10 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

124 

2.81 ±0.30 

1.70 

94.4 

2.98 ± 0.31 

1.86 


Urban 

459 

3.50±0.16 

2.51 

95.6 

3.66 ±0.17 

2.64 

16-20 

Rural 

124 

3.17 ±0.27 

2.43 

99.2 

3.20 ± 0.27 

2.45 


Urban 

458 

3.60±0.15 

2.82 

99.8 

3.61 ±0.15 

2.82 

21-25 

Rural 

50 

2.53 ±0.37 

1.55 

98.0 

2.58 ± 0.37 

1.60 


Urban 

209 

3.17 ±0.21 

2.55 

99.5 

3.19 ± 0.21 

2.56 


Single Males: Educational Level 13-f 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

352 

2.94 ±0.16 

2.28 

96.3 

3.05 ± 0.16 

2.38 


Urban 

2587 

2.84 ±0.06 

2.20 

95.9 

2.96 ± 0.06 

2.32 

16-20 

Rural 

363 

2.52 ±0.10 

2.01 

99.7 

2.53 ±0.11 

2.02 


Urban 

2640 

2.77 ±0.05 

2.18 

99.7 

2.78 ±0.05 

2.19 

21-25 

Rural 

266 

2.17±0.11 

1.64 

99.2 

2.19 ± 0.11 

1.66 


Urban 

1753 

2.59 ±0.06 

1.93 

99.9 

2.59 ±0,06 

1.93 

26-30 

Rural 

85 

2.17 ±0.23 

1.46 

98 8 

2.20 ±0.23 

1.48 


Urban 

445 

2.64 ±0.13 

1.91 

100.0 

2.64 ± 0.13 

1.91 


Mamed Males: Educational Level 0-8 


21-25 

Rural 

128 

3.36 ±0.34 

2.31 

100.0 

3.36 ± 0.34 

2.31 


Urban 

162 

4.41 ±0.38 

2.85 

100.0 

4.41 ± 0.38 

2.85 

26-30 

Rural 

117 

3.29 ±0.38 

2.09 

100.0 

3.29 ± 0.38 

2.09 


Urban 

148 

3.35 ±0.26 

2.58 

98.6 

3.40 ±0.26 

2.63 

31-35 

Rural 

93 

2.75 ±0.33 

1.74 

100.0 

2.75 ±0.33 

1.74 


Urban 

109 

2.98 ±0.27 

2.34 

100.0 

2.98 ±0.27 

2.34 

36-40 

Rural 

84 

2.48 ±0.31 

1.70 

100.0 

2.48 ±0.31 

1.70 


Urban 

75 

2.53 ±0.25 

2.19 

100.0 

2.53 ±0.25 

2.19 


Married Males: Educational Level 13 + 


21-25 

Rural 

63 

3.85 ±0.46 

3.06 

100.0 

3.85 ±0.46 

3.06 


Urban 

428 

3.70 ±0.12 

3.12 

100.0 

3.70 ±0.12 

3.12 

26-30 

Rural 

86 

3.01 ±0.35 

2.41 

100.0 

3.01 ±0.35 

2.41 


Urban 

516 

3.36±0.11 

2.76 

100.0 

3.36 ±0.11 

2.76 

31-35 

Rural 

76 

2.73 ±0.29 

2.24 

100.0 

2.73 ±0.29 

2.24 


Urban 

402 

2.96 ±0.12 

2.35 

100.0 

2.96 ±0.12 

2.35 

36-40 

Rural 

50 

2.26 ±0.26 

2.09 

98.0 

2.31 ±0.26 

2.11 


Urban 

281 

2.70 ±0.15 

2.09 

100.0 

2.70 ± 0.15 

2.09 


Table 1 16. Total outlet and rural-urban background 


448 


Chapter 12 

RURAL-URBAN BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


The city boy’s failure to understand what life can mean to a boy who is 
raised on a farm, and the farm boy’s idea that there is something glamor- 
ous about the way in which the city boy lives, apply to every avenue of 
human activity, including the sexual. This popular interest in knowing how 
another group lives is projected into the sociologist’s invariable search for 
basic differences between the mores of city groups and the mores of farm 
groups ; and this accounts for the fact that the few data which have been 
available on the sexual hfe of the rural male have commanded widespread 
attention. 

Unfortunately, the only specific comparisons of the sexual activities of 
rural versus urban groups come from a small study made by Pearl in 1925. 
The study covered a limited number of sexual items on 174 older males, of 
whom 39 were farmers. The calculations derived from these few cases 
seemed to show that the farmers were sexually more active than the 
merchants and the bankers, and they in turn were more active than the 
professional men. These conclusions have been quoted many times in the 
sociological literature, although the data are, of course, altogether too 
scant to warrant any generalizations concerning such a tremendous popula- 
tion as the rural American group constitutes. It is unfortunate that such 
poorly established conclusions should have gained such wide credence, 
and particularly unfortunate because the conclusions are diametrically 
opposed to what now appears to be the fact. The Pearl series was not 
broken down for educational backgrounds or any other social measures of 
the individuals involved. Since lower social levels have higher frequencies 
of total sexual outlet, particularly of marital intercourse (Table 88), and 
since marital intercourse was the only sexual outlet for which Pearl had 
data, it is probable that the farmers in the Pearl study rated high in sexual 
activity because they belonged to lower educational levels. Conversely, the 
merchants, bankers, and professional groups, which constituted the major 
portion of Pearl’s urban sample, were from upper white collar and pro- 
fessional classes, and these always have lower rates of marital outlet. 

Reference to Chapter 3 in the present volume will show the definitions 
by which the subjects in the present study have been classified as rural or 
urban. It is unfortunate that the limited rural sample which is now avail- 
able has made it impossible to make the complete breakdowns which are 
shown in Chapter 3, and the analyses in the present chapter have been made 

449 



450 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 



j 


Masturbation: 

Rural, Urban 


AGE 

GROUP 

RURAL- 


TOTAL population 

ACTIVE population 

URBAN 

CASES 



1 




GROUP 

1 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean j 
Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 



Single Males : Educational Level 0-8 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

245 

1.28 =i=0.11 

0.75 ! 

55.0 

80.8 

1.59 =t:0.13 

1.05 


Urban 

401 

1.76 =i=o.ll 

0.94 j 

52.3 

86.8 

2.03 =i=0,13 

1.19 

16-20 

Rural 

259 

0.94 0.07 

0.48 

33.6 

84.9 

1.11 =1=0.08 

0.66 


Urban 

397 

0.98 0.07 

0.45 

28.7 

84.6 

1.16 ± 0.08 

0.62 

21-25 

Rural 

141 

0.67 0.09 

0.21 

24.3 

63.8 

1.05=1=0.13 

0.57 


Urban 

188 

0.64 0.08 

0.18 

19.5 

63.3 

1.01 =b0.12 

0.48 

26-30 

Rural 

61 

0.58 =fc:0.12 

0.05 

19.6 

54.1 

1.07±0.19 

0.71 


Urban 

88 

0.65 =t=0.13 

0.11 

22.0 

64.8 

1.01 =1=0.19 

0.48 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


AdoL-15 

Rural 

124 

1.66 =1=0,19 

0.94 

60.8 

88.7 

1.87=1=0.20 

1.19 


Urban 

459 

2.07 =1=0.11 

1.34 

60.3 

90.2 

2.29 =±=0.11 

1.56 

16-20 

Rural i 

124 

1.13 =1=0.15 

0.61 

35.8 

85.5 

1.32 =1=0.17 

0.82 


Urban 

458 

1.33 =fc 0.07 

0.74 

37.2 

90.2 

1.47 =±=0.07 

0.87 

21-25 

Rural 

50 

0.68 =±=0.16 

0.24 

27.4 

66.0 

1.03 =1=0.23 

0.61 


Urban 

209 

0.92=1=0.09 

0.41 

30.0 

78.9 

1.17=1=0.10 

0.66 


Single Males: Educational Level 13+ 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

Urban 

352 

2587 

2.28 =1=0.13 
2.24 =fc 0.05 

1.76 

1.60 

79.1 

79.9 

84.1 

82.1 

2.71 =1=0.14 
2.73 =1=0.06 

2.17 

2.08 

16-20 

Rural 

Urban 

363 

2640 

1.67 ±0.08 
1.86 ±0.04 

1.18 

1.22 

67.3 

67.0 

89.0 

88.9 

1.88 =t: 0.09 
2.09 =±=0.05 

1.44 

1.54 

21-25 

Rural 

Urban 

266 

1753 

1.19 =1=0.08 
1.35=1=0.04 

0.79 

0.67 

56.4 

52.9 

86.1 

87.5 

1.38 =1=0.08 
1.54=1=0.05 

0.95 

0.85 

26-30 

Rural 

Urban 

85 

445 

1.09=1=0.16 
1.15 =1=0.08 

0.48 

0.49 

50.6 

43.5 

78.8 

85.6 

1.39 =t 0.19 
1.34=1=0.09 

0.94 

0.68 


Table 117. Masturbation and rural-urban background 


RURAL-URBAN BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 451 

on a simpler basis. They must be taken as indications of trends which will 
have to be analyzed more precisely when more material is available. 

Throughout this chapter persons have been classified as rural if they 
ever belonged to the rural-urban group which is numbered 3 (Chapter 3), 
either alone or in conjunction with some other rating which they held at 
some other period of their fives. This means that they have been considered 
rural if they lived on an operating farm for an appreciable portion of the 
years between 12 and 18. This is the late pre-adolescent and adolescent 
period which is so important in the shaping of sexual patterns (Chapter 1 1). 
Persons have been classified as urban if they ever belonged to rural-urban 
groups 0, 1, or 4, or to some combination of these groups, without ever 
belonging to class 3. This means they have been rated as urban if they never 
had more than incidental residence in rural areas, or if their rural residence 
occurred only after the age of 18, which is the age by which most of the 
patterns of sexual behavior are already laid down (Chapter 1 1). Rural- 
urban group 2 was not used because the sample was too small. As treated 
in the present chapter, the rural group is very definitely rural, but the 
urban group involves some individuals who have had chiefly city residence 
but some less significant rural contacts. It is unfortunate that no finer 
breakdown could be made with the material available at this time. 

FREQUENCIES OF TOTAL OUTLET 

For the population as a whole, it has been shown that frequencies of 
sexual outlet depend upon the age of the individual, the age at which he 
became adolescent, his educational background and occupational class, 
and his religious background. This is equally true of the rural portion of 
the population and of the urban portion of the population, and no com- 
parison of the frequencies of total sexual outlet or of the sexual outlet from 
the several sorts of sexual activity can mean much unless there is a prelimi- 
nary breakdown on most of these other factors. 

An examination of Table 116 will show that the differences between the 
total outlet of the rural males and the total outlet of the urban males are 
never very great. In general, the differences would not be particularly 
significant if they did not all fie in the same direction, which is almost 
without exception in the direction of a lower frequency of total sexual out- 
let for the rural males. The differences are most marked in the lower 
educational level, where the rural males may not have more than three- 
fourths as frequent activity as the urban males. Differences are less 
for the males of the high school level and among the boys who go to 
college. 

City-bred persons might expect the farm boy to have higher rates of 
outlet, inasmuch as he sees sexual activity among animals and hears free 
discussion of sex from the time he is very young; but the specific data do 



452 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 





Nocturnal Emissions: Rural, Urban 

AGE 

GROUP 

RURAL- 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

URBAN 

CASES 







GROUP 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 



Single Males : Educational Level 0-8 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

245 

0.05 0.015 

0.00 

2.3 

22.9 

0.24 ±0.056 

0.08 


Urban 

401 

0.06 ±0.011 

0.00 

1.7 

27.4 

0.21 ± 0.038 

0.08 

16-20 

Rural 

259 

0.13 ±0.019 

0.01 

4.8 

53.3 

0.25 ±0.033 

0.08 


Urban 

397 

0.17 ± 0.019 

0.03 

5.0 

58.2 

0.29 ± 0.030 

0.09 

21-25 

Rural 

141 

0.17 ± 0.030 

0.03 

6.3 

57.4 

0.30 ±0.049 

0.10 


Urban 

188 

0.16 ±0.024 

0.03 

4.7 

60.1 

0.26 ±0.038 

0.09 

26-30 

Rural 

61 

0.18 ±0.041 

0.05 

5.9 

65.6 

0.27 ± 0.059 

0.10 


Urban 

88 

0.19 ±0.039 

0.04 

6.3 

63.6 

0.29 ±0.059 

0.09 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

124 

0.11 ±0.025 

0.00 

4.0 

34.7 

0.31 ± 0.063 

0.15 


Urban 

459 

0.17 ±0.026 

0.00 

4.9 

41,8 

0.40 ± 0.057 

0.12 

16-20 

Rural 

124 

0,20 ±0.031 

0.06 

6.3 

71.8 

*0.28 ±0.039 

0.11 


Urban 

458 

0.24 ±0.022 

0.07 

6.6 

71.0 

0.33 ±0.029 

0.14 

21-25 

Rural 

50 

0.21 ±0.047 

0.07 

8.5 

74.0 

0.29 ±0.059 

0.13 


Urban 

209 

0.25 ±0.026 

0.08 

8.1 

70.3 

0.35 ±0.034 

0.23 


Single Males: Educational Level 13-}- 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

Urban 

352 

2587 

0.27 ±0.031 
0.36 ±0.014 

0.06 

0.12 

9.3 

12.7 

61.9 

71.8 

0.43 ±0.047 
0.50 ± 0.019 

0.25 

0.28 

16-20 

Rural 

Urban 

363 

2640 

0.44 ±0.035 
0.42 ±0.012 

0.26 

0.25 

17.6 

15.2 

92.0 

90.7 

0.47 ±0.038 
0.47 ±0.013 

0.29 

0.29 

21-25 

Rural 

Urban 

266 

1753 

0.39 ±0.038 
0.39 ±0.014 

0.25 

0.22 

18.7 

15.2 

89.5 

86.0 

0.44 ±0.041 
0.45 ±0.016 

0.29 

0.28 

26-30 

Rural 

Urban 

85 

445 

0.29 ±0.030 
0.32 ±0.022 

0.22 

0.18 

13.6 

12.0 

85.9 

84.0 

0.34 ± 0.033 
0.38 ±0.025 

0.08 

0.25 


Table 118. Nocturnal emissions and rural-urban background 



RtJRAL-URBAN BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


453 


not bear out such an idea. In fact, it might be possible to theorize to the 
effect that early and constant acquaintance with sex would reduce the 
farm boy’s erotic responses and leave him less interested; but this still 
remains unproved theory. The average city dweller thinks of the farm boy 
as having more privacy than the city boy has for carrying on socio-sexual 
activities, but the theory does not seem to fit the fact. There is a general 
opinion that rural communities are in general stricter in their religious 
adherence than city communities, and this may be one of the explanations 
of the slightly lower rates of rural groups, but this is not demonstrable with 
the present data. It might be suggested that the city boy has more oppor- 
tunity for making social contacts in general, for dating girls and, conse- 
quently, for obtaining sexual relations with girls ; and this may, or may not, 
be an explanation of the fact that socio-sexual contacts are in actuality 
less frequent for the boy who is raised on the farm. There are other possible 
explanations of the lower rates of the rural males, but none of these is more 
than a possibility which will need investigation when suflScient series of 
cases become available, 

SPECIFIC SEXUAL OUTLETS 

Masturbation. Self-induced orgasm occurs in almost exactly the same 
proportions of the rural and of the urban populations (Table 117). Fre- 
quencies are rather lower for the youngest adolescent group of farm boys 
who never go beyond eighth grade or high school; but at all ages the fre- 
quencies among the boys who will ultimately go to college are practically 
identical for the rural and for the urban groups. Since the total outlet of 
the rural male is a bit lower, and since the actual frequencies of masturba- 
tion are about the same as those of the urban group, the part of the total 
outlet which the farm boy derives from masturbation is a bit higher at all 
ages and in all educational levels. 

Nocturnal Emissions. These occur with much the same incidences and 
frequencies among the rural and the urban groups. This is true at all ages, 
and in all social levels (Table 118). In the few places where the table indi- 
cates some differences, there are no consistent trends. 

Petting to Climax. Orgasm achieved through heterosexual petting occurs 
in a definitely higher percentage of the urban males (Table 119). The fre- 
quencies are somewhat higher for the urban males who do not go beyond 
eighth grade or high school, but at the college level the frequencies of pet- 
ting are 2.5 to 3 times as high among the urban males. Perhaps the farm 
boy is not so often kivolved because girls simply are not so available in 
a rural community; or perhaps he is not so often involved because the 
smaller community has not yet acquired the newer customs that are found 
in the city. The fact will have to be determined by a detailed examination 
of more histories. 



454 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 





Petting to Climax: Rural, Urban 


AGE 

GROUP 

RURAL- 


TOTAL population 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

URBAN 

CASES 







GROUP 



Me- 

%0f 



Me- 





Mean 

dian 

Total 

Incid. 

Mean 

dian 




Frequency 

Freq. 

Outlet 

% 

Frequency 

Freq. 

Single Males: Educational Level 0-8 

Adol.-15 

Rural 

245 

0,017^0.006 

0.00 

0.7 

11.8 

0.14 =b 0.043 

'0.06 


Urban 

401 

0.037=h0.011 

0.00 

1.1 

13.7 

0.27 ±0.078 

0.07 

16-20 

Rural 

259 

0.043=1=0.012 

0.00 

1.5 

17.8 

0.24 ± 0.059' 

0.07 


Urban 

397 

0.058=fc0.015 

0.00 

1.7 ' 

22.4 

0.26 =±: 0.064 

0.07 

21-25 

Rural 

141 

0.031=1=0.013 

0.00 

1.1 

12.8 

0.24 =1=0.085 

0.08 


Urban 

188 

0.047=^=0.023 

0.00 

1.4 

16.5 

0.28 =1=0.070 

0.07 

26-30 

Rural 

61 

0,11 =±=0.065 

0.00 

3.8 

21.3 

0.53 =±=0.28 

0.20 


; Urban 

88 

0.017=1=0.009 

0.00 1 

0.6 

17.0 

0.10 =1=0.049 

0.06 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

124 

0,021=1=0.008 

0.00 

0.8 

17.7 

0.12 =1=0.038 

0.06 


Urban 

459 

0.059 ±0.013 

0.00 

1.7 

19.6 

0,30 =1=0.060 

0.07 

16-20 

Rural 

124 

0.040=1=0.011 

0.00 

1.3 i 

30.6 

0.13 ± 0.0291 

0.07 


Urban 

458 

0.095=1=0.015 

0.00 

2.7 

34.3 

0.28 =1=0.038 

0.08 

21-25 

Rural 

50 

0.034=1=0.012 

0.00 

1.4 

28.0 

0.12 =1=0.032 

0.08 


Urban 

209 

0.094=1=0.019 

0.00 

3.0 

29.2 

0.32 =±0.072 

0.08 


Single Males: Educational Level 13+ 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

352 

0.019=1=0.006 

0.00 

0.7 

11.1 

0.17 =±0.053 

0.07 


Urban 

2587 

0.045±0.005 

0.00 

1.6 

13.9 

0.33 =±0.027 

0.09 

16-20 

Rural 

363 

0,074=1=0.013 

0.00 

3.0 

33.5 

0.21 =±0.034 

0.07 


Urban 

2640 

0.15 =1=0.008 

0.00 

5.6 ^ 

47,7 

0.32 =±0.016 

0.09 

21-25 

Rural 

266 

0.081=1=0.012 

0.00 

3.8 

43.6 

0.19 =±0.025 

0.07 


: Urban 

1753 

0.20 =±0.012 

0.02 

7,9 

54.5 

0.37 =±0.021 

0.11 

26-30 

Rural 

85 

0.063 =±0.014 

0.00 

2.9 

43.5 

0.14 =±0.026 

0.08 


Urban 

445 

0.16 =±0.019 

0.00 

5.9 

47.2 

0.33 =±0.037 

0.10 


Table 119. Petting to climax and rural-urban background 


RURAL-URBAN BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


455 


Pre-marital Intercourse. The diiTerences between rural and urban groups 
are greater in regard to pre-marital intercourse than they are for any of the 
preceding activities. In most age groups and at all educational levels, more 
of the city boys are involved and fewer of the farm boys (Table 120). At the 
grade school level, 91 per cent of the city boys may be involved between the 
ages of 21 and 25, but only 80 per cent of the farm boys. At the college 
level in the same age period, 55 per cent of the city boys have some pre- 
marital intercourse and about 47 per cent of the farm boys. The differences 
in frequencies of pre-marital intercourse between rural and urban groups 
are of about the same order. 

Intercourse with Prostitutes. Pre-marital relations with prostitutes are 
even more distinctively an activity of the city group (Table 121). While it 
is commonly beheved that farm boys are particularly interested in securing 
intercourse with prostitutes when they go into the city, the record indicates 
that fewer of them ever arrive at such experience. The frequency with 
which they have relations with prostitutes is definitely lower than the 
frequency with which city boys have such relations. 

Marital Intercourse. In marital relations, the rural male again has a 
slightly lower rate of outlet than the city male (Table 122). The differences 
are not great but are consistent in several groups, as far as our limited data 
apply. 

Homosexual Outlet. Orgasm effected by contacts with other males is, 
on the whole, less frequent among the farm boys who have contributed 
histories to this study, more frequent among the urban males (Table 123). 
The two groups are most distinct at the grade school and high school 
levels. The differences in incidence are very minor at the college level. 
Among the boys who have not gone beyond grade school, 32 per cent of 
the city boys may be involved between the ages of 16 and 20, but only 21 
per cent of the farm boys. Among males of the high school level, at a 
corresponding age, the figures are 46 per cent for the city boys, 26 per cent 
for the rural. For the boys of the college level, in the same age group, the 
figures are very nearly identical, 16 or 17 per cent in both groups. Differ- 
ences in frequencies are of the same general order, with the city boy having 
the most frequent contacts. 

There is a wide-spread theory among psychologists and psychiatrists 
that the homosexual is a product of an effete and over-organized urban 
civilization. The failure to make heterosexual adjustments is supposed to 
be consequent on the complexities of life in our modern cities ; or it is a 
product of a neuroticism which the high speed of living in the city imposes 
upon an increasing number of individuals. The specific data on the partic- 
ular rural and urban groups which are shown in Table 123 do seem to 
suggest that there is something in city life which encourages the develop- 



Total Non-marttal Intercourse: Rural, Urban 


AGE 

RURAL- 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

GROUP 

URBAN 

GROUP 

Ci^iSES 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

1 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Single Males: Educational Level 0-8 


Adol.-15 

16-20 

21-25 

Rural 

Urban 

Rural 

Urban 

Rural 

Urban 

199 0.67 ±0.12 
414 1.26 ±0.14 

208 1.26 ±0.15 
406 1.99 ±0.16 

106 1.40 ±0.24 
195 2.35 ±0.26 

0.00 

0.03 

0.43 

0.89 

0.49 

0.96 

31.3 

39.9 

50.2 

60.2 

56.2 

69.5 

39.2 
51.4 

81.3 

87.4 

80.2 

90.8 

1.72 ±0.27 
2.45 ±0.24 

1.55 ±0.17 
2.27 ±0.17 

1.75 ±0.29 
2.59 ±0.28 

0.91 

1.39 

0.72 

1.25 

0.83 

1.43 



Single Males: Educational Level 

9-12 



Adol.-15 

Rural 

91 0.57 ±0.12 

0.00 

21.4 J 

38.5 

1.47 ±0.26 

0.86 


Urban 

405 0.87 ±0.12 

0.00 

26.6 

44.2 

1.97 ±0.25 

0.81 

16-20 

Rural 

95 1.48 ±0.18 

0.70 

46.2 

87.4 

1.69 ±0.20 

1.00 


Urban 

405 1.40 ±0.14 

0.41 

40.8 i 

1 1 

72.6 

1.92 ±0.18 

0.87 

Single Males: Educational Level 13+ 

Adol.-15 

Rural 

265 0.08 ±0.03 

0.00 

2.9 

10.6 

0.77 ±0.23 

0.31 


Urban 

2126 0.08 ±0.01 

0.00 

4.2 

9.7 

0.84 ±0.10 

0.29 

16-20 

Rural 

272 0.19 ±0.04 

0.00 

8.0 

36.4 

0.53 ±0.11 

0.10 


Urban 

2172 0.26 ±0.02 

0.00 

13.8 

42.5 

0.62 *0.04 

0.18 

21-25 

Rural 

200 0.34 ±0.07 

0.00 

16.7 

47.0 

0.73 ±0.13 

0.25 


Urban 

1377 0.45 ±0.03 

0.03 

19.3 

55.0 

0.82 ±0.06 

0.30 

26-30 

Rural 

58 0.48 ±0.15 

0.00 

23.4 

44.8 

1.06 ±0.31 

0.54 


Urban 

308 0.68 ±0.09 

0.06 

21.4 

58.8 

1.16 ±0.15 

0.47 


Married Males: Educational Level 0-8 


21-25 

Rural 

128 0.14 ±0.042 

0.00 

4.1 

25.0 

0.55 ±0.14 

0.27 


Urban 

162 0.73 ±0.21 

0.00 

16.1 

40.7 

1.78 *0.50 

0.30 

26-30 

Rural 

117 0.15 ±0.047 

0.00 

4.6 

29.9 

0.51 ±0.14 

0.22 


Urban 

148 0.23 ±0.066 

0.00 

7.0 

35.1 

0.67 ±0.17 

0.12 

31-35 

Rural 

93 0.18 ±0.061 

0.00 

6.5 

22.6 

0.78 ±0.23 

0.43 


Urban 

109 0.10 ±0.024 

0.00 

3.5 

36.7 

0.28 ±0.06 

0.10 


Married Males: Educational Level 13+ 


21-25 

Rural 

63 

0.16 ±0.14 

0.00 

4.0 

9.5 

1.68 ±1.42' 

0.35 


Urban 

428 

0.05 ±0.011 

0.00 

1.4 

15.7 

0.32 ±0.06 

0.10 

26-30 

Rural 

86 

0.12 *0.092 

0.00 

3.9 

9.3 

1.27 ±0,94 

0.40 


Urban 

516 

0.09 ±0.016 

0.00 

2.8 

25.2 

0.37 ±0.06 

0.09 

31-35 

Rural 

76 

0.07 ±0.045 

0.00 

2.5 

10.5 

0.64 ±0.39 

0.30 


Urban 

402 

0.16 ±0.032 

0.00 

5.6 

34.1 

0.48 ±0.09 

0.17 


Table 120. Total non-marital intercourse and rural-urban background 

456 



RURAL-URBAN BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


457 


ment of the homosexual. But the distinctive thing about homosexuality in 
the city is the development of a more or less organized group activity 
which is unknown in any rural area. 

Large cities have taverns, night clubs, restaurants, and baths which may 
become frequented almost exclusively by persons interested in meeting 
homosexual friends, or interested in finding opportunities for discussions 
with others who do not object to the known homosexuality of their com- 
panions. In this city group, the development of an elaborate argot gives a 
sense of belonging which may defend a minority group against the rest of 
society; but it also intensifies a feeling which the group has that it stands 
apart from the rest of the population. Moreover, it is this city group which 
exhibits all the affectations, the mannerisms, the dress, and the other dis- 
plays which the rest of the population take to be distinctive of all homo- 
sexual persons, even though it is only a small fraction of the males with 
homosexual histories who ever display such characteristics. None of these 
city-bred homosexual institutions is known in rural areas, and this may 
well acount for a somewhat lower rate of the homosexual among farm boys. 

On the other hand, the highest frequencies of the homosexual which we 
have ever secured anywhere have been in particular rural communities in 
some of the more remote sections of the country. The boy on the isolated 
farm has few companions except his brothers, the boys on an adjacent 
farm or two, visiting male cousins, and the somewhat older farm hand. 
His mother may see to it that he does not spend much time with his sisters, 
and the moral codes of the rural community may impose considerable 
limitations upon the association of boys and girls under other circum- 
stances. Moreover, farm activities call for masculine capacities, and asso- 
ciations with girls are rated sissy by most of the boys in such a community. 
All of these things are conducive to a considerable amount of homosexual- 
ity among the teen-age males in the most isolated of the rural areas. There 
is much less of it in the smaller farm country of the Eastern United States. 

Beyond this, there is a fair amount of sexual contact among the older 
males in Western rural areas. It is a type of homosexuality which was prob- 
ably common among pioneers and outdoor men in general. Today it is 
found among ranchmen, cattle men, prospectors, lumbermen, and farming 
groups in general — among groups that are virile, physically active. These 
are men who have faced the rigors of nature in the wild. They five on reah- 
ties and on a minimum of theory. Such a background breeds the attitude 
that sex is sex, irrespective of the nature of the partner with whom the 
relation is had. Sexual relations are had with women when they are avail- 
able, or with other males when outdoor routines bring men together into 
exclusively male groups. Such a pattern is not at aU uncommon among 
pre-adolescent and early adolescent males in such rural areas, and it con- 
tinues in a number of histories into the adult years and through marriage. 



458 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 





Intercourse with Prostitutes: 

: Rural, Urban 

AGE 

GROUP 

RURAL- 

URBAN 

CASES 

TOTAL population ' 

ACTIVE population 

GROUP 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

7oOf 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Single Males: Educational Level 0-8 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

245 

0.012±0.005 

0.00 

0.5 

5.3 

0.22 ±0.08 

0.09 


Urban 

401 

0.041^0.012 

0.00 

1.2 

10.0 

0.41 =b0.11 

0.15 

16-20 i 

Rural 

259 

0.15 =fc0.025 

0.00 

5.4 

42.5 

0.35 ±0.05 

0.09 


Urban 

397 

0.23 =b0.029 

0.01 

6.6 

50.9 1 

0.45 ±0.05 

0.20 

21-25 

Rural 

141 

0.31 =1=0.067 

1 0.003 

11.1 

50.4 

0.61 ±0.12 

0.25 


Urban 

1 188 

0.46 =b0.067 

|0.12 

14.0 

66.5 

0.69 ±0.10 

0.36 

26-30 

Rural 

61 

0.50 ±0.14 

1 

0.07 

16.9 

65.6 

%, 

0.76 ±0.20 

0.32 


Urban 

88 

[0.57 ±0.095 

0.25 

19.2 

75.0 

jO.76 ±0.12 

0.42 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

124 

0.004±0.003 

0.00 1 

0.2 

4.8 

0.09 ± 0.041 

0.07 


Urban 

459 

0.017±0.005 

0.00 i 

0.5 

8.1 

0.21 ±0.046 

0.08 

16-20 

Rural 

124 

0.10 ±0.03l' 

0.00 

3.3 

41.1 

0.25 ± 0.070 

0.07 


Urban 

458 

0.097±0.013i 

0.00 

2.7 

40.8 

0.24 ± 0.029 

0,08 

21-25 

Rural 

50 

0.19 ±0.071 

0.00 

7.8 

48.0 

0.40 ±0.14 

0.13 


Urban 

209 

0.12 ±0.019 

0.00 

3.9 

40.7 

0.29 ±0.040 

0.11 


Single Males: Educational Level 13+ 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

352 

0.002±0.001 

0.00 

0.1 

2.0 

0.12 ±0.045 

0.08 


Urban 

2587 

0.003 ±0.001 

0.00 

0.1 

2.2 

0.12 ±0.033 

0.06 

16-20 

Rural 

363 

0.011 ±0.002 

0.00 

0.4 

12.9 

0.08 ±0.012 

0.06 


Urban 

2640 

0.021 ±0.002 

0.00 

0.8 

19.9 

0.11 ±0.008 

0.06 

21-25 

Rural 

266 

0.013±0.003 

0.00 

0.6 

15.8 

0.08 ±0.013 

0.06 


Urban 

1753 

0.032±0.007 

0.00 

1.3 

17.6 

0.18 ±0.040 

0.07 

26-30 

Rural 

85 

0.007±0,004 

0.00 

0.3 

7.1 

0.09 ±0.041 

0.07 


Urban 

445 

0.097±0.040 

0.00 

3.7 

19.1 

0.51 ±0.20 

0.08 


Table 121. Intercourse with prostitutes and rural-urban background 




RURAL-URBAN BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


459 


Such a group of hard-riding, hard-hitting, assertive males would not 
tolerate the affectations of some city groups that are involved in the homo- 
sexual; but this, as far as they can see, has little to do with the question of 
having sexual relations with other men. This type of rural homosexuality 
contradicts the theory that homosexuality in itself is an urban product. 





Marital Intercourse: Rural, Urban 

AGE 

GROUP 

RURAL- 

URBAN 

CASES 

total population 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

GROUP 

' 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%af 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 


Educational Level 0-8 


21-25 

Rural 

128 

3.03 ±0.31 

1.99 

90.6 

100.0 

3.03 ±0.31 

1.99 


Urban 

162 

3.44 =t= 0.28 

2,38 

76.3 

98.1 

3.51 =±=0.28 

2.42 

26-30 

Rural 

117 

2.90=^0.36 

1.83 

88.5 

100.0 

2.90=^0.36 

1.83 


Urban 

148 

2.86 =±=0.21 

2.33 

86.0 

98.6 

2.90 =±=0.21 

2.35 

31-35 

Rural 

93 

2.39 =±=0.29 

1.58 

88.1 

100.0 

2.39 =±=0.29 

1.58 


Urban 

109 

2.73 =i= 0.25 

2.18 

91.9 

100.0 

2.73 =±=0.25 

2.18 

36-40 

Rural 

84 

2.21 =±=0.28 

1.54 

91.4 

100.0 

2.21 =±=0.28 

1 1.54 


Urban 

75 

2.23 =±=0.24 

1.95 

89.2 

98.7 

2.26 =±=0.24 

1.98 


Educational Level 13-f 


21-25 

Rural 

63 

3.34=1=0.47 

2.65 

84.6 

100.0 

3.34=1=0.47 

2.65 


Urban 

428 

3.07 ±0.11 

2.58 

84.1 

99.5 

3.08 =1=0.11 

2.59 

i 

26-30 : 

Rural 

86 

2.49=1=0.29 

1.93 

82.9 

100.0 

2.49 =1=0.29 

1.93 


Urban 

516 

2.77 ±0.10 

2.23 

83.1 

99.2 

2.79 =±=0.10 

2.25 

31-35 

Rural 

76 

2.22=1=0.24 

1.85 

82.2 

100.0 

2.22=^0.24 

1.85 


Urban 

402 

2.38 =±=0.11 

1.91 

81.1 

99.5 

2.40 =±=0.11 

1.92 

36-40 

Rural 

50 

1.77=1=0.22 

1.53 

79.2 

98.0 

1.81 =1=0.22 

1.56 


Urban 

281 

2.06 =±=0.12 

1.66 

76.6 

98.9 

2.09 =±=0.12 

1.67 


Table 122. Marital intercourse and rural-urban background 


Animal Contacts. Sexual relations with animals of other species are, of 
necessity, most often found in rural areas. Ultimately about 17 per cent of 
the farm boys have complete sexual relations with other animals (Table 
124), and perhaps as many more have relations which are not carried 
through to climax. 




460 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 





Homosexual Outlet: Rural, Urban 

AGE 

GROUP 

RURAL- 


total population 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

URBAN 

CASES 







GROUP 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

°/ 

/o 

Mean 

Frequency 

i 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 



Single Males : Educational Level 0-8 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

245 

0.18 0.039 

0.00 

7.7 

18.4 

0.98 ±0.17 

0.46 


Urban 

401 

0.32 ±0.051 

0.00 

9.5 

28.4 

1.12 ±0.16 

0.44 

16-20 

Rural 

259 

0.21 ±0.054 

0.00 

7.4 

21.2 

0.97 ±0.23 

0.37 


Urban 

397 

0.26 ±0.037 

0.00 

7.6 

32.0 

0.81 ±0.10 

0.34 

21-25 

Rural 

141 

0.26 ±0.11 

0.00 

: 9.4 

17.0 

1.52 ±0.61 

0.43 


LTrban 

188 

0.26 ±0.058 

0.00 

! 7.9 

28.7 

0.91 ±0.17 

0.41 

26-30 

Rural 

61 

0.53 ±0.33 

0.00 

18.0 

19.7 

2.71 ±1.57 

0.75 


Urban 

88 

0.34 ±0.097 

0.00 

11.6 

35.2 

0.98 ±0.24 

0.44 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

124 

0.26 ±0.13 

0.00 

9.6 

20.2 

1.30±0.60 

0.10 


Urban 

459 

0.32 ±0.037 

0.00 

9.3 

37.9 

0.84 ±0.09 

0.33 

16-20 

Rural 

124 

0.10 ±0.031^ 

0.00 

3.2 

25.8 

0.39 ±0.10 

0.08 


Urban 

458 

0.50 ±0.060 

0.00 

14.1 

46.7 

1.07 ±0.12 

0.37 

21-25 

Rural 

50 

0.10 ±0.051 

0.00 

3.9 

24.0 

0.40 ±0.19 

0.09 


Urban 

209 

0.69 ±0.13 

0.00 

22.4 

42.6 

1.61 ±0.27 

0.89 


Single Males: Educational Level 13+ 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

352 

0.08 ±0.019 

0.00 

2.7 

21.3 

0.36 ±0.08 

0.09 


Urban 

2587 

0.09 ±0.008 

0.00 

3.2 

21.8 

0.41 ±0.03 

0.09 

16-20 

Rural 

363 

0.05 ±0.013 

0.00 

2.1 

16.8 

0.32 ±0.07 

0.09 


Urban 

2640 

0.07 ±0.008 

0.00 

2.4 

15.8 

0.42 ±0.04 

0.08 

21-25 

Rural 

266 

0.06 ±0.022 

0.00 

2.8 

9.4 

0.62 ±0.21 

0.15 


Urban 

1753 

0.09 ±0.012 

0.00 

3.4 

10.1 

0.85 ±0.10 

0.24 

26-30 

Rural 

85 

0.10 ±0.042 

0.00 

4.9 

15.3 

0.68 ±0.22 

0.39 


Urban 

445 

0.22 ± 0.036 

' 0.00 

8.3 

16.9 

1.30 ±0.16 

0.63 


Table 123. Homosexual outlet and rural-urban background 


RURAL-URBAN BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


461 





Animal Contacts: Rur^l 

AGE 

GROUP 

RURAL- 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

URBAN 

CASES 







GROUP 


Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 

%0f 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Frequency 

Me- 

dian 

Freq. 



Educational Level 0-8 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

245 

0.054=1=0.018 

0.00 

2.3 

8.6 

0.63 =fc:0.16 

0.50 


Urban 

401 

0.011=1=0.006 

0.00 

0.3 

4.0 

0.28 =1=0.13 

0.08 

16-20 

Rural 

259 

0.034^0.012 

0.00 

1.2 

7.3 

0.46 =1=0.13 

0.26 


Urban 

397 

0,012±0,008 

0.00 

0.3 

2.0 

0.58 =b 0.35 

0.09 


Educational Level 9-12 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

124 

0.038=1=0.020 

0.00 

1.4 

10.5 

0.36 =1=0.17 

0.10 


Urban 

459 

0.018=t=0.008 

0.00 

0.5 

3.7 

0.48 =±=0.19 

0.09 

16-20 

Rural 

124 

0.092^0.040 

0.00 

2.9 

10.5 

0.88 =1=0.32 

0.40 


Urban 

458 

0.017=^0.010 

0.00 

0.5 

2.6 

0.65 =1=0.33 

0.09 

21-25 

Rural 

50 

0.013=^0.008 

0.00 

0.5 

6.0 

0.22=^0.08 

0.30 


Urban 

209 

0.014=^0.013 

0.00 

0.5 

1.9 

0.74=1=0.69 

0.08 


Educational Level 13+ 


Adol.-15 

Rural 

352 


Urban 

2587 

16-20 

Rural 

363 


Urban 

2640 

21-25 

Rural 

266 


Urban 

1753 

26-30 

Rural 

85 


Urban 

445 


0.14 =1=0.039 

0.00 

4.7 

0.003=1=0.001 

0.00 

0.1 

0.070=1=0.028 

0.00 

2.8 

0.001=1=0.001 

0.00 

0.02 

0.015=1=0.008 

0.00 

0.7 

0.0001=1= 

0.0004 

0.00 

0.002 

0.009=1=0.009 

0.00 

0.4 

0.0002=±= 

0.00011 

0.00 

0.0004 


27.8 

1.9 

0.49 =1=0.13 
0.14=1=0.05 

0.08 

0.06 

15.4 

0.7 

0.46 =t: 0.18 
0.12 =±=0.07 

0.10 

0.06 

3.8 

0.40=1=0.17 

0.25 

0.1 

0.05 

0.08 

1.2 

0.80 

1.00 

0.4 

0.05 

0.08 


Table 124. Animal contacts and rural-urban background 




462 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


tone 

LEVEL 

RURAL 

0-8 

UR6AM 


RURAL 

9-(Z 

URBAN 


RURAL 

13 + 

URBAN 


MASTURBATION 


PETTING TO CLIMAX 



TOTAL INTERCOURSE 


INTERCOURSE 
WITH PROSTITUTES 


NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 


ANIMAL INTERCOURSE 


RURAL 



Figure 125. Comparisons of sexual activity in rural and urban groups 


Comparing mean frequency data for the age period 1 6-20, for three educational levels. 
Black bars for urban population, shaded bars for rural groups. 


RURAL-URBAN BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


463 


There is, of course, a considerable amount of pre-adolescent contact with 
animals (Chapter 5). Among adolescent and older males of the rural 
groups, the lowest incidences of animal contacts are to be found in Eastern 
areas. The incidences increase considerably in the ranch country of the 
West. For the available rural sample, which is largely Eastern in origin, 
the active incidence figures begin at about 9 per cent in the early adoles- 
cent years of the group which never goes beyond grade school, and grades 
down to about 3 per cent for males of that educational level who are not 
yet married between the ages of 21 and 25. Among the rural boys who go 
into high school but not beyond, the active incidence figures stand at about 
1 1 per cent between adolescence and 20 years of age. For the rural boys 
who will ultimately go to college, about 28 per cent have animal contacts 
between adolescence and 15, about 15 per cent in the late teens, and 4 per 
cent in the early twenties. Sexual contacts with animals are, it will be seen, 
most abundant among boys of the college level during their early and 
later teens. 

Surprisingly enough, in the grade school portion of the urban population 
the boys have an appreciable amount of animal intercourse. About 4 per 
cent of these boys are involved between adolescence and 15. However, 
the frequencies for the city boys are very low, not amounting to more than 
a half to a fifth of the frequencies found among rural males. At the high 
school level, and particularly at the college level, there are fewer (1 to 4 per 
cent) of the city boys who are involved, and the discrepancies between 
the rural and the urban histories become very great on this point. For 
instance, the incidence among males of the college level, at 16-20 years of 
age, is 0.7 per cent for the city-bred boys and 15.4 per cent for the farm 
group. The differences in frequencies are even greater, the farm boys having 
30 to 70 experiences for every one which the city boy has. 

It is, of course, surprising to find that the city boy is ever involved, 
because he does not have such access to animals as the farm boy has. The 
city boy’s contacts usually occur when he is visiting on a farm, and so in 
actuality this still remains rural behavior. In the city itself he may have 
contact with horses or ponies in some stable, or with some other animal 
in a city stockyard; but most of his contacts are with the household pets, 
particularly with pet dogs. 

The absolute frequencies of animal contact are, in actuality, low. In a 
high proportion of the histories they are isolated occurrences, or events 
that happen two or three or a half dozen times in the boy’s early adoles- 
cence. With a few individuals they may occur several times a week, and 
there are some cases of farm boys who depend upon this source for their 
major outlet. In Western farm areas there are more boys who have animal 
contacts with weekly or bi-weekly regularity through their early and per- 
haps later teens, and occasionally into their early twenties. 

16 



464 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


In summary, it may be emphasized again that there are few material 
differences between the histories of farm boys and the histories of boys 
raised in the city, or between adult males living in the two places. In general 
there are slightly lower frequencies of total sexual activity in the rural 
population, and lower frequencies in most of the particular sources of 
outlet. Nocturnal emissions occur with nearly identical incidences and 
frequencies in rural and in urban groups. The rural population is most 
distinct in having fewer socio-sexual contacts (meaning pre-marital hetero- 
sexual petting, pre-marital and extra-marital intercourse, and homosexual 
relations), and in its much higher frequencies of animal intercourse. But 
the city boy’s interest in animal contacts as soon as they are available 
makes it clear that it is simply a question of opportunity which differen- 
tiates the rural and urban groups on this latter point. 



Chapter 13 


RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 

In the broadest sense, the mores may become systems of morals and 
systems of morals are formalizations of the mores. It is no accident that 
the two words, mores and morals, stem from the same Latin root. Through- 
out history all peoples have defended their mores as stoutly as they have 
defended their religions, and their moral systems have determined the 
custom of the land. Sexual mores and systems of sexual morality are no 
exceptions to this general rule. 

This means that there is nothing in the English- American social structure 
which has had more influence upon present-day patterns of sexual be- 
havior than the religious backgrounds of that culture. It would require 
long research and a complete volume to work out the origins of the present- 
day religious codes which apply to sex, of the present-day sex mores, of 
the coded sex laws, and to trace the subtle ways in which these have in- 
fluenced the behavior of individuals (Northcote 1916, Angus 1925, May 
1931). Our particular systems certainly go back to the Old Testament 
philosophy on which the Talmud is based, and which was the philosophy 
of those Jews who first followed the Christian faith. In many details, the 
proscriptions of the Talmud are nearly identical with those of our present- 
day legal codes governing sexual behavior. Back of the Jewish formula- 
tions were the older codes of such peoples as the Hittites (Barton 1925), 
Babylonians (Harper 1904), Assyrians (Barton 1925), and Egyptians 
(Budge 1 895), all of whom probably had a part in shaping the sexual 
systems of the early Jews. Several Roman ascetic cults had a consid- 
erable influence on the asceticism of the early Christian church, and 
Greek philosophy in a more general way contributed to Christian ethics, 
both in the early days of the church and in the middle ages. 

Ecclesiastic law governing sexual matters set the pattern from which 
the sex law of English common courts was derived between the twelfth 
and fifteenth centuries, and irrespective of the specific statutes which the 
several states have written to control sexual behavior, the decisions of 
American criminal courts today are primarily based upon the precedents 
of those common courts. This is no place to work out the details of the 
historic development, but it is important at this point to reahze that these 
present-day codes are quite ancient, that they are the product of stiU 
older rehgious systems, and that throughout their history they have been 

465 



466 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Total Outlet and Religion 


RELIGIOUS GROUP 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

Mean 

Median 

Incid. 

Mean 

Median 

Frequency 

Freq. 

% 

Frequency 

Freq. 


Single Males: Educational Level 0-8 


Age: Adol -15 
Protestant, active 

89 ■ 

2.65 

±0.40 

1.30 

83.1 

3.18 

±0.46^ 

1.89 

Protestant, inactive 

481 

3.09 

±0.17 

1.78 

93.8 

3.29 

± 0.18: 

2.03 

Catholic, inactive 

106 

3.21 

± 0. 34 

2.04 

90.6 

3.55 

±0,36] 

2.46 

Age: 16-20 
Protestant, active 

91 

2.73 

±0.37 

1.50 

93.4 

2.93 

±0.39 

1.71 

Protestant, inactive 

493 

3.31 

± 0.17 

2.24 

98.2 

3.37 

± 0.17 

2.29 

Catholic, inactive 

105 

3.42 

±0.30 

2.57 

98.1 

3.49 

±0.30 

2.64 

Age: 21-25 

Protestant, inactive 

234 

3.22 

± 0.27 

2.03 

97.9 

3.29 

± 0.27 

2.08 

Catholic, inactive 

60 

3.27 

±0.40 

2.21 

98.3 

3.33 

± 0.40 

2.25 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


Age: Adol.-15 
Protestant, active : 

93 . 

2.61 ± 0.27 

1.71 

i 

1 

92.5 : 

2,83 ± 0.28' 

1.85 

Protestant, inactive 

375 , 

3.38 ±0.18 

2.33 , 

95.2 , 

3.55 ± 0.19 

2.48 

Catholic, inactive 

103 . 

3.85 ±0.43 

2.58 

98.1 

3.93 ± 0.43 

2.67 

Age: 16-20 
Protestant, active 

95 

2.52 ±0.23 

1.69 

100.0 : 

1 

2.52 ± 0.23’ 

1.69 

Protestant, inactive , 

315 

3.53 ± 0.191 

2.67 ' 

99.0 

3.57 ± 0.19 

2.71 

Catholic, inactive 

101 

4.36±0.40| 

3.15 ^ 

100.0 

4.36 ±0.40i 

1 

3.15 


Single Males: Educational Level 13 -f 


Age: Adol.-15 







Protestant, active 

547 

2.48 ±0.10 

1.98 

95.4 

2.60 ±0.10 

2.10 

Protestant, inactive 

1471 

3.04 ±0.07 

2.45 , 

96.0 

3.17 ± 0.08i 

2.58 

Catholic, devout 

132 

2.44 ±0.26 

1.60 

97.7 , 

2.50 ± 0.26 

1.65 

Catholic, inactive 

165 

3.03 ±0.23 

2.16 ' 

95.2 

3.19 ± 0.24' 

2.26 

Jewish, Orthodox 

58 , 

1.96 ±0.27 

1.28 , 

91.4 

2.14 ± 0.28 

1.40 

Jewish, inactive 

601 

2.91 ±0.13 

2.17 

96.0 

3.03 ± 0.13 

2.29 

Age: 16-20 




1 



Protestant, active 

557 

2.31 ±0.08 

1.83 

99.6 

2.32 ± 0.08 

1.83 

Protestant, inactive ' 

1513 

2.87 ±0.06 

2.36 

99.7 

2.88 ± 0.06 

2.37 

Catholic, devout 

136 

2.42 ±0.20 

1.70 

; 100.0 

2.42 ±0.20 

1.70 

Catholic, inactive 

168 

2.79 ±0.19 

2.18 

99.4 

2.81 ± 0.19 

2.19 

Jewish, Orthodox , 

59 

1.97 ±0.21 

1.57 

100.0 

1.97 ± 0.21; 

1.57 

Jewish, inactive 

607 

2.98 ±0.11 

2.30 

100.0 

2.98 ±0.11 

2.30 


{Table continued on next page) 




RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


467 




Total Outlet and Religion 

RELIGIOUS GROUP 

CASES 

TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 



Mean 

Median 

Incid. 

Mean 

Median 



Frequency 

Freq. 

% 

Frequency 

Freq. 

Single Males 

;: Educational Level 13+ (Continued) 


Age: 21-25 
Protestant, active 

384 

2.02 =t 0.10 

1.43 

99.7 

2.02 ± 0.10 

1.43 

Protestant, inactive 

1000 

* 2.62 ± 0.07 

1.99 

99.8 

2.62 ± 0.07 

1.99 

Catholic, devout 

94 

' 1.85 0.16 

1.32 

i 100.0 

1.85 ±0.16 

1.32 

Catholic, inactive 

125 

2.86 ±0.23 

2.09 

100.0 

2.86 ± 0.23 

2.09 

Jewish, inactive 

331 

3.09 ±0.15 

2.44 

100.0 

3.09 ± 0.15 

2.44 

Age: 26-30 







Protestant, active 

100 

1.85 ±0.21 

! 1.25 

100.0 

1.85 ± 0.21 

1.25 

Protestant, inactive 

279 

2.56 ±0.15 

i 1.82 

99.6 

2.57 ± 0.15 

1.83 

Jewish, inactive 

104 

3.23 ±0,35 

2.42 

100.0 

3.23 ± 0.35 

1 2.42 

1 


Married Males: Educational Level 13 + 


Age: 21-25 







Protestant, active 

91 

3.29 ±0.26 

2.71 

100.0 

3.29 ± 0.26 

2.71 

Protestant, inactive 

280 

3.85 ± 0.18 

3.19 

100.0 

3.85 ± 0.18 

3.19 

Jewish, inactive 

86 

4.02 ±0.23 

3.44 

100.0 

4.02 ± 0.23 

3.44 

Age: 26-30 







Protestant, active 

123 

2.67 ± 0.15 

2.50 

100.0 

2.67 ± 0.15 

2.50 

Protestant, inactive 

346 

3.45 ±0.16 

2.76 

100.0 

3.45 ± 0.16 

2.76 

Jewish, inactive 

109 

3.50 ±0.21 

2.86 

100.0 

3.50 ±0.21 

2.86 

Age: 31-35 







Protestant, active 

109 

2.31 ±0.13 

2.17 

100.0 

2.31 ± 0.13 

i 2.17 

Protestant, inactive 

270 

3. 10 ±0.17 

2.37 

100.0 

3.10 ± 0.17 

s 2.37 

Jewish, inactive 

84 

3.05 ±0.26 

2.43 

100.0 

|3.05 ±0.26 

2.43 

Age: 36-40 







Protestant, active 

73 

1.98 ± 0.12 

1.90 

100.0 

1.98 ±0.12 

1.90 

Protestant, inactive 

187 

2,88 ± 0.22 

2.20 

: 99.5 

2.89 ± 0.22 

2.21 

Jewish, inactive 

62 

2.67 ±0.26 

2.13 

100.0 

2.67 ± 0.26 

2.13 


Table 125. Total sexual outlet as related to religious background 




468 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


the bases for the law which has formally expressed society’s interest in 
controlhng human sexual behavior. 

It has already been pointed out (Chapter 6) that average frequencies of 
sexual outlet for the human male are distinctly below those which are 
normal among some other anthropoids and which would probably be 
normal in the human animal if there were no restrictions upon his sexual 
activity. Averages for the single males begin at 3.3 per week and drop with 
old age, and averages for the married males begin at 4.8 per week and drop 
with age ; but the averages in non-restrained human animals would proba- 
bly be nearer 7 per week, and in 10 to 15 per cent of the population would 
probably run higher than that (Chapter 6). It has been stated (Chapter 8) 
that the differences between these higher figures and the actual rates of the 
male population provide some measure of the effectiveness of the social 
pressures, of the specific laws, and of the attitudes, ideals, esthetic values, 
physical interferences, and other restraints which the social organization 
imposes upon the sexual activity of the individual. 

That these social pressures are primarily religious in their origin is 
confirmed by the comparisons which can now be made between the fre- 
quencies of total sexual outlet and the incidences and frequencies of outlet 
from the various sexual sources, among persons who are actively con- 
cerned with religious organizations, and among persons who are less 
closely concerned with the teachings and practices of any church group. 

In attempting to measure these present-day influences of the church on 
the sexual behavior of the American population, three religious groups, 
Protestant, Catholic, and Jewish, are recognized in the present chapter. 
These faiths embrace most of those Americans who recognize any church 
affiliation. Within each group a still more important classification has, 
however, been made. This involves the degree of adherence of the individ- 
ual to the doctrines and to the activities of the religious group in which 
he is at least nominally placed. Of course, every degree of adherence exists 
among various individuals, and as the data have been recorded in our 
original histories, several such degrees have been recognized (Chapter 3). 
However, the limited size of the present sample has made it necessary to 
base the analyses in the present chapter on two groups of Protestants, 
two groups of Catholics, and two groups of those of the Jewish faith. 
These groups have included, on the one hand, the less active (or less devout) 
members of each faith, and on the other hand the more active (or more 
devout) members of those same faiths. Active or devout in this classifica- 
tion has been taken to mean regular attendance and/or active participation 
in organized church activities, and/or frequent attendance at the Catholic 
confessional or the Jewish synagogue. Inactive or non-devout in the imme- 
diate analysis has been apphed to all persons who have not qualified as 
active or devout under the above definitions. 



RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


469 


It is hardly necessary to add that all comparisons made in the present 
chapter are based on a preliminary breakdown of each population for 
sex, race, marital status, age, and educational levels. With the additional 
breakdowns on the religious group and on the degree of adherence of each 
subject to that religious group, all of the calculations given in the present 
chapter are based on 7-way breakdowns. Any analysis short of this would 
have proven inadequate ; but it is only a limited list of religious groups for 
which we have samples that are adequate for such elaborate treatment 
(Chapter 3). All conclusions which are drawn in the present chapter should 
be limited to these particular groups. In the interest of scientific fact and 
of such social and moral applications as others may wish to draw from the 
scientific fact, it is to be hoped that religious groups which are not yet 
sufiiciently represented may be included in the future development of this 
survey. 

TOTAL SEXUAL OUTLET 

Considering the frequencies of total sexual outlet (Table 125), the 
sexually least active individuals in any age and educational group are the 
Orthodox Jews (who are the least active of all), the devout Catholics, and 
the active Protestants (in that order). Conversely, the sexually most active 
individuals are the non-church-going Catholics, with the inactive Protes- 
tants and the inactive Jewish males intermediate in the system. At various 
educational levels and in different age groups there is some variation in 
this ordering, but there is only one group, namely the single males of the 
college level, where the data are abundant enough to compare all six 
religious classifications. 

Between adolescence and 15 years of age, the boys who will ultimately 
go to college are arranged in the following religious groups, beginning 
with those who are sexually least active and building the list in the direc- 
tion of those who are most active: 

Jewish Orthodox 
Catholic devout 
Protestant active 
Jewish inactive 
Catholic inactive 
Protestant inactive 

This order remains constant whether the calculations are made as means or 
as medians, and whether for total populations or for active populations. 
The order is somewhat different when it is based on the number of persons 
involved in sexual activity (the active incidence figures). In this latter case, 
the smallest number of sexually active individuals (91%) is to be found 
among the Orthodox Jews, while the largest number of active individuals 
(98%) is to be found among the devout Catholics. 

Between 16 and 20 years of age the boys who will go to college, or who 
are already in college, are arranged in the following order, beginning with 



470 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Masturbation and Religion 


RELIGIOUS GROUP 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 


Single Males; Educational Level 0-8 


Age: Adol.-15 
Protestant, active 

89 

1.30±0.22 

0.50 

52.9 

70.8 

1.83 

1.13 

Protestant, inactive 

481 

1.58 =t0.10 

0.89 

51.1 

88.1 

1.79 

1.08 

Catholic, inactive 

106 

1.66 =<=0.20 

0.93 

52.6 

85.8 

1.94 

1.21 

Age: 16-20 

Protestant, active 

91 

0.77±0.11 

0.34 

28.6 

81.3 

0.95 

^ 0.49 

Protestant, inactive 

493 

0.94 ±0.06 

0.46 ; 

28.4 

85.8 

1.10 

0.60 

Catholic, inactive ' 

105 

1.04 ±0.13 

0.49 

30.9 

84.8 

1.22 

0.72 

Age: 21-25 

Protestant, inactive 

234 

1 

0.60 ± 0.07 i 

0.18 

18.7 

62.4 

0.96 

0.50 

Catholic, inactive 

60 

0.61 ±0.15 

1 

0.20 

18.9 

66.7 

0.91 

0.42 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


Age:Adol.-15 
Protestant, active 

93 

1.75 ±0.19 

1.32 

68.2 

87.1 

2.01 

1.60 

Protestant, inactive 

375 

1.88 ±0.11 

1.13 

55.8 

90.4 

2.08 

1.35 

Catholic, inactive 

103 

2.22 ±0.23 

1.46 

58.8 

95.1 

2.33 

1.57 

Age: 16-20 

Protestant, active 

95 

1.15±0.14 

0.58 

45.9 

88.4 

1.30 

0.72 

Protestant, inactive 

315 

1,20 ±0.09 

0.66 

34.4 

88.9 

1.35 

0.83 

Catholic, inactive 

101 

1.56±0.18 

0.89 

36.1 

91.1 

1.71 

1.03 


Single Males: Educational Level 13+ 


Age: Adol.-15 





1 





Protestant, active 

547 

1.88 

d= 

0.08 

1.42 

75.6 

80.8 

2.33 

1.88 

Protestant, inactive 

1471 

2.40 

dz 

0.06 

1.82 

78.8 

84.7 

2.83 

2.30 

Catholic, devout 

132 

1.96 

d= 

0.25 

0.96 

81.9 

78.8 

2.49 

1.61 

Catholic, inactive 

165 

2.33 

=b 

0.21 

1.65 

78.4 

80.6 

2.89 

1.98 

Jewish, Orthodox 

58 

1.56 

=fc 

0.26 j 

1.03 

79.3 

72.4 

2.15 

1.47 

Jewish, inactive i 

601 

2.34 

=b 

0 . 12 ! 

1 

1.58 

80.6 

80.0 

2.93 

2.16 

Age: 16-20 










Protestant, active 

557 

1.50 

=b 

0.07 

1.00 

65.0 

85.6 

1.75 

1.33 

Protestant, inactive 

1513 

1.95 

dz 

0.05 

1.41 

67.8 

91.5 

2.13 

1.62 

Catholic, devout 

136 

1.53 

rfc: 

0.19 

0.75 

63.3 

87.8 

1.82 

0.95 

Catholic, inactive 

168 

1.65 

=b 

0.15 

1.06 

58.7 

86.3 ! 

1.91 

1.50 

Jewish, Orthodox 

59 

1.18 

=t= 

0.19 

0.64 

59.4 

78.0 

1.52 

0.93 

Jewish, inactive 

607 

2.09 

d= 

0.11 

1.41 

70.2 

88.1 

2.38 

1.68 


{Table continued on next page) 



RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


471 



Masturbation and Religion 


RELIGIOUS GROUP 

TOTAL POPULATION 

CASES 

ACTIVE POPULATION 


Mean 

Median 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

Mean 

Median 


Frequency 

Freq. 

°/ 

/o 

Freq. ; 

Freq. 


Single Males: Educational Level 13+ (Continued) 


Age; 21-25 

Protestant, active 

384 

1.10 0.07 

0.60 

54.5 

83.6 

1.32 

0.81 

Protestant, inactive 

1000 

1.46 ±0.06 

0.80 

55.6 

90.3 

1.61 

0.93 

Catholic, devout 

94 

0 82 ±0.12 

0.36 

45.1 

78.7 

1.04 

0.52 

Catholic, inactive 

125 

1.10 ±0.15 

0.50 

39.4 

81.6 

1.35 

0.75 

Jewish, inactive 

331 

1.58 ±0.12 

0.73 

51.4 

86.4 

1.83 

1.03 

Age: 26-30 

Protestant, active 

100 

1.00 ±0.14 

0.49 

54.3 

82.0 

1.22 

0.72 

Protestant, inactive 

279 

1.32±0.10 

0.63 

51.8 

88.2 

1.50 

0.81 

Jewish, inactive 

104 

1.02 ±0.21 

0.38 

31.2 

79.8 

1.27 

0.57 


Married Males: Educational Level 13+ 


Age: 21-25 








Protestant, active 

91 

0.22 ±0.04 

0.04 

6.9 

60.4 

0.37 

0,21 

Protestant, inactive 

280 

0.34 ±0.04 

0.08 

8.9 

69.6 

0.49 

0.24 

Jewish, inactive 

86 

0.45 ±0.10 

0.07 

11.4 

64.0 

0.71 

0.28 

Age: 26-30 








Protestant, active 

123 

0.27 ±0.06 

0.05 

10.1 

64.2 

0.41 

0.10 

Protestant, inactive 

346 

0.33 ±0.03 

0.08 

9.6 

71.1 

0.46 

0.24 

Jewish, inactive 

109 

0.25 ±0.05 

0.04 

7.0 

57.8 

0.43 

0.21 

Age: 31-35 

1 







Protestant, active 

109 

0.22 ±0.06 

0.03 

9.7 

59.6 

0.37 

0.09 

Protestant, inactive 

270 

0,29 ±0,04 

0.08 

9.5 

71.1 

0.41 

0.21 

Jewish, inactive 

84 

0.20 ±0.05 

0.01 

6.5 

52.4 

0.37 

0.14 

Age: 36-40 








Protestant, active 

73 

0.22 ±0.07 

0.02 

11,2 

54.8 

0.41 

0.09 

Protestant, inactive 

187 

0,27 ±0.04 

0.07 

9.6 

71.1 

0.38 

0.17 

Jewish, inactive 

62 

0.14 ±0.05 

0.00 

5.1 

41.9 

0.34 

0.10 


Table 126. Masturbation as related to religious background 


472 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


the sexually least active group and building the list in the direction of the 
most active group : 

Jewish Orthodox Catholic inactive 

Catholic devout Protestant inactive 

Protestant active Jewish inactive 

This order varies somewhat with the calculations as means or as medians, 
and for total populations and for active populations. 

Between 21 and 25 years of age, the single males who have gone to 
college are arranged in the following order, beginning with the sexually 
least active group and ending with the most active group : 

Catholic devout Catholic inactive 

Protestant active Jewish inactive 

Protestant inactive 

This order is more or less constant, whatever the method of calculating. 

For the less adequate series of religious groups among males of other 
educational levels the story appears to be much the same. In both grade 
school and high school groups, the persons most actively connected with 
church activities are, again, the least active sexually, and the males who are 
sexually most active are those Catholics who have least to do with their 
church. 

The differences between the frequencies of these several religious groups 
are, in most instances, not large but rather constant. There is a 25 per cent 
difference between groups at certain ages in certain educational levels, and 
in some instances the most extreme groups have rates of total outlet which 
are 75 per cent higher than the rates of the least active groups of the same 
age and educational levels. To put it another way, devout acceptance of 
the church’s teaching is correlated with sexual frequencies which are two- 
thirds or less than two-thirds of the frequencies which are found among 
males of corresponding age and educational levels who are not actively 
connected with the church. Either this is the direct effect of church teach- 
ings, or else those individuals who become most actively associated with 
the church are a select group which would not have had high frequencies 
of sexual outlet if they had never belonged to a church. It will take more 
elaborate analyses of a much larger series of histories to determine which 
explanation is correct, but there is some evidence now at hand (Chapter 6) 
that some portion of the devoutly rehgious individuals have repressed 
rather than sublimated sex histories. 

MASTURBATION 

At various places in the foregoing chapters it has been pointed out that 
masturbation and intercourse are the chief sources of pre-marital outlet. 
It is, therefore, to be expected that there should be certain correlations 
between the frequencies of masturbation and the frequencies of inter 



RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


473 


course, and thus indirectly of total sexual outlet. In upper and lower social 
levels (Chapter 10) the frequencies of masturbation do bear an inverse 
relation to the frequencies of pre-marital intercourse, and it might be 
anticipated that the suppression of one of these activities by the rules of 
any religious group would provide some direct impetus to the development 
of the other activity. 

This, however, proves not to be so. The least frequent experience in 
masturbation is found among the more devout members of each and every 
one of the religious groups (Table 126), and these are the very groups which 
have the lowest rates of total outlet. The incidence figures for masturbation 
are not constantly different, but the mean frequencies are always lower for 
the more devout groups. This is true for both single and married males of 
every age group on which there are sufficient data and, strikingly enough, 
it is true of every educational level in each religious group. In some age and 
educational groups the masturbatory rates of the active Protestants, the 
devout Catholics, and the Orthodox Jews do not average more than two- 
thirds or three-fourths as high as the rates of the inactive members of those 
same churches. Even in those segments of the population where the differ- 
ences between devout and inactive groups are more minor, it is significant 
that they always stand in the same order: the religiously active persons 
masturbate less frequently than the persons who are less concerned with 
their religion. At the other end of the picture, the males who most often 
masturbate are the religiously inactive Protestants, sometimes the non- 
church-going members of the Catholic faith, and in some cases the inactive 
Jewish males. 

The objections to masturbation have originated from reUgious creeds 
which go back to the most remote beginnings of our Western European- 
American civilization. Elsewhere in the world masturbation may be looked 
upon as a childish performance, or as evidence of the incapacity of an 
individual to make socio-sexual adjustments (Chapter 14); but few other 
peoples have condemned masturbation as severely as the Jews have. The 
Talmudic references and discussions make masturbation a greater sin than 
non-marital intercourse. There were excuses for pre-marital intercourse 
and for extra-marital intercourse with certain persons under the Jewish 
code, but no extenuation for masturbation (Bible, Talmud passim). The 
logic of this proscription depended, of course, upon the reproductive 
motive in the sexual philosophy of the Jews. This made any act which 
offered no possibility of a resulting conception unnatural, a perversion, 
and a sin. Whatever other sources may have contributed to the Christian 
church’s objections to masturbation, certainly the Jewish traditions must 
have provided a considerable impetus to the perpetuation of this taboo in 
the Christian religion. In the Orthodox church today, the Jewish boy is 
definitely affected by the old-time Hebraic laws on this point, and the often 



474 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Nocturnal Emissions and Religion 


RELIGIOUS GROUP 

CASES 

TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 



Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Indd. 

% 

1 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 


Single Males: Educational Level 0-8 


Age: Adoi.-15 1 








Protestant, active i 

89 

0.022=1=0.007 

0.00 

0.9 

21.3 

0.10 

0.067 

Protestant, inactive 

481 

0.057=1=0.010 

0.00 , 

1.8 

26.0 , 

0.22 

0.078 

Catholic, inactive . 

106 

0.035=1=0.010 

0.00 : 

1.1 

23.6 

0.15 

0.076 

Age : 16-20 








Protestant, active 

91 

0.14 =b0.04 

0.01 

5.0 

52.7 : 

0.26 

0.077 

Protestant, inactive 

493 

0.14 =i=0.02 

0.02 

4.4 

56.6 

0.26 

0.087 

Catholic, inactive 

105 

0.17 ±0.03 < 

0.03 

5.0 

57.1 

0.30 

0.16 

Age: 21-25 








Protestant, inactive 

234 

0.15 ±0.02 

0.03 

4.6 

59.0 

0.25 

0.087 

Catholic, inactive 

60 

0.21 ±0.04 

0.07 

6.6 

65.0 

0.33 

0.23 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


Age: Adol.-15 
Protestant, active 

93 

0.16 ±0.05 

0,00 

6.3 

40,9 

0.39 

0.15 

Protestant, inactive 

375 

0.14 ±0.03 

0.00 

4.1 

40.0 

0-35 . 

0.10 

Catholic, inactive 

103 

0.13 ±0.04 

0.00 

3.4 

35.0 

0.36 

0.15 

Age: 16-20 

Protestant, active 

95 

0.18 ±0,03 

0.06 

7.3 

71.6 

0.26 

0.10 

Protestant, inactive 

315 

0.22 ±0.02 

0.06 

6.4 

72.7 

0.31 

0.10 

Catholic, inactive 

101 

0.18 ±0.03 

0.04 

4,1 

60.4 

0.29 

0.17 


Single Males: Educational Level 13+ 


Age: Adol.-15 









Protestant, active 

547 

0.34 

±0.03 

0.12 

13.6 

70.7 

0.48 

0.29 

Protestant, inactive 

1471 

0.33 

±0.02 

0.02 

10.8 

68.0 

0.48 

0.27 

Catholic, devout 

132 

0.30 

±0.04 

0.15 

12.4 

75.0 

0.40 

0.27 

CathoHc, inactive 

165 

0.32 

±0.04 

0.10 

10.8 

65.5 

0.49 

0.31 

Jewish, Orthodox 

58 

0.25 

±0.06 

0.09 

12.5 

77.6 

0.32 

0.18 

Jewish, inactive 

601 

0.41 

±0.03 

0.15 

14.1 

75.4 

0.54 

0.29 

Age: 16-20 









Protestant, active 

557 

0.41 

±0.02 

0.27 

17.9 ^ 

92.5 

0.45 

0.30 

Protestant, inactive 

1513 

0.42 

±0.02 

0.24 

14.5 

91.7 i 

0.45 

0.28 

Catholic, devout 

136 

0.48 

±0.06 

0.30 

19.9 

94.1 

0.51 

0.32 

Catholic, inactive 

168 

0.43 

±0.04 

0.26 

15.3 

91.1 

0.47 , 

0.30 

Jewish, Orthodox 

59 

0.37 

±0.06 

0.23 

18.6 

89.8 

0.41 

0.28 

Jewish, inactive 

607 

0.44 

±0.03 

0.24 

14.9 

86.2 

0.51 

0.31 


(Table continued on next page) 


RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


475 


Nocturnal Emissions and Religion 


RELIGIOUS GROUP 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTP/E POPULATION 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 


Single Males: Educational Level 13+ {Continued) 


Age: 21-25 

Protestant, active 

384 

0.42 

=fc0.03 

0.26 

20.8 

90.1 

0.47 

0.30 

Protestant, inactive 

1000 

0.36 

=4=0.02 

0.20 

13.8 

86.9 

0.42 

0.26 

Catholic, devout 

94 

0.48 

=t0.07 

0.31 

26.6 

90.4 

0.53 

, 0.34 

Catholic, inactive 

125 

0.40 

=4=0.05 

0.24 

14.3 

88.0 

0.45 

, 0.29 

Jewish, inactive 

331 

0.40 

=fc:0.04 

0.19 

13.1 

81.0 

0.50 

0.29 

Age: 26-30 

Protestant, active 

100 

0.41 

=4=0.05 

0.27 

21.9 

95.0 

0.43 

0.29 

Protestant, inactive 

279 

0.29 

=4=0.02 

0.17 

11.2 

85.3 

0.34 

0.24 

Jewish, inactive 

104 

0.31 

=4=0.06 

0.12 

9.6 

75.0 

! 0.42 

0.24 


Mamed Males: Educational Level 13 + 


Age: 21-25 








Protestant, active 

91 

0.17 =4=0.03 

0.053 

5.2 

67.0 

0.25 

0,12 

Protestant, inactive 

280 

0.19 =4=0.03 

0.051 

5.0 

67.9 

0.28 

0.10 

Jewish, inactive 

86 

0.13 =4=0.03 

0.033 

3.3 

60.5 

0.22 

0.09 

Age: 26-30 








Protestant, active 

123 

0.16 =4:0.02 

0.067 

6.1 

78,9 

0.20 

0.09 

Protestant, inactive 

346 

0.16 =t0.02 

0.056 

4.7 

71.7 

0.22 

0.09 

Jewish, inactive 

109 

0.17 =±=0.03 

0.043 

4.7 

66.1 

0.25 

0.09 

Age: 31-35 








Protestant, active 

109 

0.17 =4=0.02 

0.073 

7.6 

78.9 

0.22 

0.10 

Protestant, inactive 

270 

0.14 =t0.02 

0.050 

4.6 

72.2 

0.19 

0.08 

Jewish, inactive 

84 

0.12 =4=0.04 

0.017 

4.0 

56.0 

0.21 

0.08 

Age: 36-40 








Protestant, active 

73 

0.16 =fc:0.02 

0.079 

7.9 

75.3 

’ 0.21 

0.16 

Protestant, inactive 

187 

0.12 =4=0.02 

0.042 

4.4 

66.8 

i 0.19 

1 0.08 

Jewish, inactive 

62 

0.11 =fc0.03 

0.028 

3.9 

61.3 

0.18 

0.07 


Table 127. Nocturnal emissions as related to religious background 




476 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


lower rates of the religiously inactive Jewish boys indicate that even they 
are not entirely free of the ancestral codes; 

The Catholic boy is told that masturbation is a carnal sin, particularly 
because it is so often accompanied by erotic fantasies which represent an 
improper use of functions which should be reserved for contacts which 
might lead to reproduction (Davis 1946). Some priests go to considerable 
lengths to impress a confessant with the idea that masturbation is one of 
the more serious sins, and earlier church writers sometimes specifically 
declared it more sinful than fornication (Northcote 1916). 

Through most of its past history the Protestant church has been as 
severe in its condemnation of “self-abuse” as either the Jewish or the 
Catholic groups, and some Protestant clergymen maintain such attitudes 
today. However, many Protestant clergymen now accept medical, psy- 
chologic, and biologic opinion that masturbation does no physical harm. 
Perhaps as a result of this, we find the religiously active Protestants of the 
better educated groups more often involved in masturbation than the 
devout Catholics or Orthodox Jews. Some Protestant groups now lay less 
emphasis upon the physical harm supposed to result from masturbation, 
and attach more importance to the undesirability of allowing oneself to 
become subject to such a habit. Whatever the issues, however, the record 
is clear that religious influences do succeed in reducing both the incidence 
and the frequency of masturbation among the more devout members of 
each church group. 

NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 

Nocturnal emissions represent the one type of sexual outlet to which 
there is a minimum of religious objection. One might, therefore, have 
anticipated that there would be higher frequencies of nocturnal emissions 
among males who are more closely allied to the church; but this is not 
consistently so. What differences there are between the frequencies and the 
incidences of emissions among religiously more active and religiously less 
active groups, are quite minor (Table 127). The means differ from the 
medians calculated for the same groups, nearly as often as the averages for 
devout groups differ from the averages for inactive groups. 

This result is significant. Persons who are interested in moral interpreta- 
tions in sex education insist that the frequencies of nocturnal emissions 
rise to suflicient heights to provide all of the necessary, sexual outlet for a 
boy who abstains from other sexual activities. But although the record 
indicates that those who have been most interested in the church have 
actually reduced their total outlets, the frequencies of nocturnal emissions 
in these groups have not been raised (Chapter 15). The relative importance 
of the emissions is raised by the reduction of the rates of total outlet, but 
the absolute frequencies of the emissions are not altered. 



RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


477 


PRE MARITAL PETTING TO CLIMAX 

There are some slight differences in the incidences and frequencies of 
pre-marital petting in the several religious groups (Table 128) but, as we 
have already seen, there are much greater differences between the several 
social levels (Tables 84, 110, Figure 100). The deliberate elaboration of 
pre-marital petting first emerged as an activity of a particular educational 
level (the college group), and religious backgrounds seem to have had 
little to do with the individual’s acceptance or rejection of such activity. 
Many individuals in lower level groups, whatever their religious connec- 
tions, consider that petting is a perversion because it is a substitute for 
actual coitus. At the college level, however, petting is much more often 
accepted, with little distinction on account of the religious background of 
the group. There may be some tendency for the more devout Catholics of 
the college level to avoid petting during the younger adolescent years, but 
there is no consistent trend in the arrangement of rehgious groups at any 
older age level. 

In all religious groups, older persons have been disturbed over this 
petting behavior. In Catholic philosophy, the general principles concerning 
venial and carnal sins certainly apply to many of the techniques that are 
utilized in petting (Davis 1946). It is surprising, therefore, that there has 
not been more specific religious objection to petting, and that young 
people of all faiths have so uniformly ignored what objections they have 
heard. That petting is not still more abundant must, however, depend upon 
the more general inhibitions on sex which religious teachings have built 
into the very body of our culture. 

PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

As already pointed out (Chapter 10), there are material differences 
between the attitudes on pre-marital intercourse in lower social levels and 
the attitudes in better educated segments of the population. There may be 
7 times (700 per cent) as much pre-marital intercourse among lower social 
levels as there is in the group that goes to college (Tables 85-87, Figures 
101-103). But within any particular social level, the differences between 
Protestants, Catholics, and Jews, and the differences between the most 
active and least devout members of each of these religious groups, are very 
much less (Table 129). Rarely do they amount to more than 50 or 100 
per cent. Lower level, religiously active Protestants average only two-thirds 
as much pre-marital intercourse as religiously inactive Protestants of the 
same level; but the same lower level Protestants average 6 or 8 times as 
much pre-marital intercourse as the religiously inactive Protestants of the 
college level. It is true that within each and every educational level it is the 
religiously devout group which has the least pre-marital intercourse; and 
the differences in incidence and frequency figures are enough to appear 





Petting to Climax, and Religion 

RELIGIOUS GROUP 

CASES 

total population 

active population 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 


Single Males: Educational Level 0-8 


Age: Adol.-15 
Protestant, active 

89 

0.022 ±0.013 

0.00 

0.9 

11.2 

0.20 

0.069 

Protestant, inactive 

422 

0.027 ±0.010 

0.00 

0.9 

13.5 

0.20 

0.060 

Catholic, inactive 

106 

0.025 ±0.011 

0.00 

0.8 

16.0 

0.15 

0.064 

Age: 16-20 
Protestant, active 

91 

0.026 ±0.013 

0.00 

1.0 

18.7 

0.14 

0.060 

Protestant, inactive 

431 

0.043 ±0.012 

0.00 

1.3 

21.6 

0.20 

0.063 

Catholic, inactive 

105 

0.052 ±0.020 

0.00 

1.5 

22.9 

0.23 

0.074 

Age: 21-25 
Protestant, inactive 

234 

0.040 ±0.018 

0.00 

1.2 

15.4 

0.26 

0.066 

Catholic, inactive 

60 

0.053 ±0.031 

0.00 

1.7 

23.3 

0.23 

0.075 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


Age: Adol.-15 
Protestant, active , 

93 

0.013 ±0.005 

0.00 

0.5 

16.1 

0.08 

0.062 

Protestant, inactive 

311 

0.038 ±0.009 

0.00 

1.1 

20.6 

0.19 

0.069 

Catholic, inactive 

103 

0.010 ±0.041 

0.00 

2.7 

21.4 

0.47 

0.088 

Age: 16-20 
Protestant, active 

95 

0.058 ±0.018 

0.00 

2.3 

i 

31.6 

0.18 

0.070 

Protestant, inactive 

315 

0.072 ±0.014 

0.00 

2.1 

33.3 

0.22 

0.071 

Catholic, inactive 

101 

0.13 ±0.040 

0.00 

3.0 

35.6 

0.36 

0.080 


Single Males: Educational Level 134- 


Age: Adol.-15 










Protestant, active 

484 

0.06 

d= 

0.14 

0.00 

2.3 

13.6 

0.43 

0.086 

Protestant, inactive 

1178 

0.05 

db 

0.007 

0.00 

1.7 

17.0 

0.29 

0.082 

Catholic, devout 

132 

0.02 

=b 

0.008 

0.00 

0.8 

8.3 

0.23 

0.180 

Catholic, inactive 

165 

0,03 

=i= 

0.009 

0.00 

1.1 

13.3 

0.24 

0.130 

Jewish, Orthodox 

58 

0.07 

db 

0.034 

0.00 

3.6 

20.7 

0.34 

0.081 

Jewish, inactive 

377 

0.02 

=±= 

0.007 

0.00 

0.9 

10.3 

0.24 

0.091 

Age: 16-20 










Protestant, active 

492 

0.14 


0.020 

0.00 

6.1 

39.6 

0.35 

0.082 

Protestant, inactive 

1210 

0.14 

d= 

0.011 

0.00 

5.2 

46.9 

0.31 

0.084 

Catholic, devout 

136 

0.15 

=b 

0.032 

0.00 

6.2 

43.4 

0.35 

0.097 

Catholic, inactive 

168 

0.11 

db 

0.019 

0.001 

4.0 

50.0 

0.23 

0.087 

Jewish, Orthodox 

59 

0.14 

=b 

0.046 

0.00 

6.9 

45.8 

0.30 

0.088 

Jewish, inactive 

379 

0.13 

d= 

0.019 

0.00 

5.0 

47.8 

0.28 

0.093 

Age: 21-25 







1 



Protestant, active 

339 

0.20 


0.036 

0.000 

10.0 

48.1 

0.42 

0.090 

Protestant, inactive 

841 

0.18 

=b 

0.018 

0.004 

7.6 

50.9 

0.36 

0.093 

Catholic, devout 

94 

0.15 

=fc 

0.032 

0.013 

8.2 

53.2 

0.28 

0.098 

Catholic, inactive 

125 

0.16 

d= 

0.039 

0.008 

5.9 

52.0 

0.32 

0.087 

Jewish, inactive 

331 

0.23 

d= 

0.023 

0.060 

7.5 

68.6 

0.34 

0.150 


Table 128 . Heterosexual petting to climax as related to religious background 

478 


RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OLFTLET 479 

significant if they were not dwarfed by the differences which are effected 
by the mores of the social groups which are involved. 

Heterosexual coitus is, in many ways, the most important aspect of 
human sexual behavior. Its occurrence in non-marital histories has been 
condemned by every religious group, practically without exception, in our 
Western European- American culture. But this is one more type of sexual 
activity where religious restraints have had less direct effect than the mores 
of the several groups which constitute our society. 

On the other hand, the origins of these sexual mores must be credited to 
the long-time effects which religious teachings have had through the many 
hundreds of years in which our cultural patterns have been developing. 
Religiously active members of the upper social level know that they are 
rejecting pre-marital intercourse on purely moral grounds (Chapter 10). 
Religiously inactive persons at this same level reject pre-marital intercourse 
almost as often, but they insist that they do so as a matter of plain decency. 
There is, however, little difference in meaning between these two verbal- 
izations of what are basically the same religious philosophies. 

Among males of the lower social levels, those who are least active in the 
church rather freely accept pre-marital intercourse without conflicts of 
conscience. The religiously more devout individuals at the same level may 
accept coitus almost as often, although they “know that it is a sin,” and 
they may occasionally be disturbed by their recognition of its moral 
significance. Nevertheless, both church-going and non-church-going males 
of the lower social levels continue to have pre-marital coitus with frequen- 
cies that average far above those of any upper level group, because all 
lower level groups, whatever the degree of their religious affiliation, beheve 
that it is human nature to have intercourse, and an inevitable activity for 
man as he is made. 

The church may even have more influence on the masturbatory behavior 
of a male than it does upon his pre-marital coital behavior. The acceptance 
or rejection of masturbation is not so difficult an issue to so many persons; 
but the very fact that coitus is a more significant activity socially makes its 
acceptance or rejection a matter of greater importance in the mores of a 
group. It would appear that in order to go further in changing overt 
behavior in regard to pre-marital intercourse, the church will have to 
affect the thinking of whole social levels; and that is a long-time process. 

MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

The available data are inadequate for analyzing the frequencies of 
marital intercourse in more than a few of the religious groups which have 
contributed to this study. It is interesting to note, however, that in these 
particular groups marital intercourse is consistently affected by the degree 



Total Non-marital Intercourse and Religion 


RELIGIOUS GROUP 


CASES 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

7oOf 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 


Single Males: Educational Level 0 -8 


Age: Adol.-15 










Protestant, active 

83 

0 68 

=fc 

0.21 

0.00 

28.7 

33.7 

2.01 

0.79 

Protestant, inactive 

431 

1 18 

=b 

0.13 

0.07 

40.1 

52.4 

2.24 

1.25 

Catholic, inactive 

89 

1.22 

=fc 

0.25 

0.00 

40.1 

44.9 

2.72 

2.06 

Age: 16-20 










Protestant, active 

81 

1.24 

=b 

0.30 

0.24 

48.6 

70.4 

1.76 

0.56 

Protestant, inactive 

442 

1.88 

=1= 

0.14 

0.90 

60.0 

90.5 

2.08 

1.12 

Catholic, inactive 

88 

1.81 

d= 

0.28 

0.53 

57.0 

80.7 

2.25 

1.25 

Age: 21-25 










Protestant, inactive 

200 

2.23 

=fc 

0.25 

0.94 

69.5 

92.0 

2.42 

1.18 

Catholic, inactive 

53 

1.97 

=b 

0.38 

0.93 

61.3 

84.9 

2.32 

1.38 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


Age: Adol.-15 











Protestant, active 

86 

0.45 

dz 

0.13 

0.00 

18.6 

31.4 

L 

.42 

0.75 

Protestant, inactive 

318 

0.98 

=fc 

0.13 

0.00 

29.3 

47.8 

2. 

.04 

0.97 

Catholic, inactive 

72 

0.59 

d= 

0.34 

0.00 

16.8 

37.5 

1 

.58 

0.45 

Age: 16-20 











Protestant, active 

89 

0.88 

=b 

0.16 

0.21 

34.8 

67.4 

1, 

.31 

0.67 

Protestant, inactive 

322 

1.60 

dz 

0.15 

0.55 i 

46.9 

80.4 

1, 

.99 

0.94 

Catholic, inactive 

69 

1.34 

dz 

0.40 

0.50 

31.3 

68.1 

1, 

.97 

1,00 


Single Males: Educational Level IS-f 


Age: Adol.-15 








Protestant, active 

493 

0.06 0.02 

0.00 

2.5 

7.1 

0.84 

0.32 

Protestant, inactive 

1205 

0.09 ±0.02 

0.00 

3.1 

11.5 

0.77 

0.29 

Catholic, devout 

103 

0.04 ±0.02 

0.00 

1.8 

4.8 

0.85 

1.00 

Catholic, inactive 

117 

0.24 ±0.09 

0.00 

6.3 

16.2 

1.48 

0.50 

Jewish, inactive 

412 

0.03 ±0.02 

0.00 

1 

1.2 

7.0 

0.49 

0.13 

Age: 16-20 








Protestant, active 

502 

0.15 ±0.03 

0.00 

6.8 

27.3 

0.56 

0.09 

Protestant, inactive 

1235 

0.27 ±0.03 

0.00 

10.4 

45.0 

0.61 

0.20 

Catholic, devout 

107 

0.18 ±0.06 

0.00 

6.0 

39.3 

0.46 

0.12 

Catholic, inactive 

120 

0 54 ±0.13 

0.04 

19.5 

59.2 

0.91 

0.27 

Jewish, Orthodox 

54 

0.24 ±0.08 

0.00 

13.4 

38.9 

0.62 

0.30 

Jewish, inactive 

416 

0.21 ±0.03 

0.00 

7.2 

45.9 

0.46 

0.10 


480 


{Table continued on next page) 


RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


481 


Total Non-marital Intercourse and Religion 


RELIGIOUS GROUP 


TOTAL POPULATION 

ACTIVE POPULATION 

Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 


Single Males: Educational Level 13-1- {Continued) 


Age: 21-25 
Protestant, active 

348 

0.20 ±0.04 

0.00 

10.4 

32.5 

0.63 

0.16 

Protestant, inactive 

867 

0.41 ±0.04 

0.04 

17.8 

59.1 

0.70 

0.28 

Catholic, devout 

71 

0.27 ±0.09 

0.00 

11.0 

38.0 

0.71 

0.30 

Catholic, inactive 

180 

0.98 ±0.20 

0.25 

31.7 

75.0 

1.31 

0.45 

Age: 26-30 
Protestant, active 

88 

0.19 ±0.05 

0.00 

9.2 

35.2 

0.54 

0.30 

Protestant, inactive 

187 

0.39 ±0.06 

0.05 

16.2 

59.9 

0.65 

0.34 

Jewish, inactive 

77 

1.69 ±0.32 

0.65 

50.9 

84.4 

2.00 

0.95 


Married Males: Educational Level 13+ 


Age: 21-25 

i 









Protestant, active 

91 

0.04 

=fc 

0.03 

0.00 1 

1.2 

7.7 

0.51 

0.10 

Protestant, inactive 

280 

0.09 


0.03 

0.00 

2.2 

17.9 

0.48 

0.18 

Jewish, inactive 

86 

0.04 

=fc 

0.02 

0.00 

0.9 

16.3 

0.23 

0.08 

Age: 26-30 










Protestant, active 

123 

0.04 

d= 

0.02 

0.00 

1.3 

8.1 

0.43 

0.08 

Protestant, inactive 

346 

0.11 

db 

0.03 

0.00 

3.1 

26.3 

0.40 

0.10 

Jewish, inactive 

109 

0.15 

d= 

0.05 

0.00 

4.3 

27.5 

0.55 

0.23 

Age: 31-35 










Protestant, active 

109 

0.03 

=1= 

0.03 

0.00 

1.4 

12.8 

0.25 

0.06 

Protestant, inactive 

270 

0.20 

d= 

0.04 

0.00 

6.4 

34.8 

0.56 

0.27 

Jewish, inactive 

84 

0.18 

=b 

0.07 

0.00 

5.8 

40.5 

0.43 

0.09 

Age: 36-40 








1 


Protestant, active 

73 

0.02 

d= 

0.01 

0.00 

1.1 

16.4 

0.13 

0.08 

Protestant, inactive 

187 

0.34 

=fc 

0.12 

0.00 

12.1 

40.6 

0.84 

0.27 

Jewish, inactive 

62 

0.23 

dr 

0.08 

0.00 

8.2 

41.9 

0.54 

0.13 


Table 129. Total pre-marital and extra-marital intercourse as related to religious 

background 


482 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


of church affiliation. In practically every instance the religiously active 
groups engage in marital intercourse less frequently than the religiously 
inactive groups (Table 130). There are frequencies in the inactive groups 
which are between 20 and 30 per cent higher than the frequencies in the 
religiously active groups of the same age and educational level. What effect 




Marital Intercourse and Religion 

RELIGIOUS GROUP 

CASES 

TOTAL population 

ACTIVE population 



Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 


Educational Level 13+ 


Age: 21-25 
Protestant, active 

91 

2.80 ±0.25 

2.19 

86.6 

98.9 

2.83 

2.22 

Protestant, inactive 

280 

3.19 ±0.17 

2.51 

83.3 

99.6 

3.20 

2.52 

Jewish, inactive 

86 

3.34 ±0.22 

2.88 

84.3 

100.0 

3.34 

2.88 

Age: 26-30 
Protestant, active 

123 

2.15 ±0.14 

1.85 

82.1 

99.2 

2.17 

1.86 

Protestant, inactive 

346 

2.80 ±0.14 

2.13 

82.1 

99.7 

2.81 

2.14 

Jewish, inactive 

109 

2.92 ±0.20 

2.47 

83.2 

99.1 

2.95 

2.49 

Age: 31-35 
Protestant, active 

109 

1.84±0.11 

1.75 

80.8 

98.2 

1.87 

1.78 

Protestant, inactive 

270 

2.43 ±0.14 

1.86 

78.7 

100.0 

2.43 

1.86 

Jewish, inactive 

84 

2.54 ±0.24 

2.12 

83.8 

100.0 

2.54 

2.12 

Age: 36^0 
Protestant, active 

73 

1.58±0.11 

1.56 

79.8 

98.6 

1.61 

1.57 

Protestant, inactive 

187 

2,07 ±0.16 

1.59 

73.1 

98.4 

2.11 

1.61 

Jewish, inactive 

62 

2.28 ±0.22 

1.90 

82.8 

100.0 

2.28 

1.90 


Table 130. Marital intercourse as related to religious background 


this may have upon the quality of marital adjustments among reUgiously 
active persons is a matter that will merit further investigation. 

HOMOSEXUAL OUTLETS 

There is much more homosexual activity among males of lower educa- 
tional levels than there is among males of the college level (Table 90, 
Figure 105). Within any particular educational level the differences between 
rehgious groups are not so great (Table 131). Between grade school and 
college males of the same religious group there may be a 200 to 500 per 



RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


483 


cent difference. Between religiously active and religiously inactive groups 
of the same denomination in the same educational level, the differences are 
ordinarily not more than 50 to 150 per cent, and sometimes they are not 
even 10 per cent. Such differences as do occur lie in the direction of less 
homosexual activity among devout groups, whether they be Protestant, 
Catholic, or Jewish, and more homosexual activity among religiously less 
active groups. 

The Hebrews, in contrast to some of their neighbors, attached a severe 
reUgious condemnation to homosexual activity (May 1931, Westermarck 
1936, Genesis 19, Leviticus 18: 7, 22, Leviticus 20: 13, Judges 19, 1 Kings 
22:46, II Kings 23 : 7, Romans 1 : 27, 1 Corinthians 6: 9, 1 Timothy 1 : 9-10, 
Talmud passim). There has, in consequence, been a continuous history of 
condemnation of the homosexual in the Christian church from its very 
beginning. Nevertheless, there has not been so frequent or so free dis- 
cussion of the sinfulness of the homosexual in reUgious literature as there 
has been of the sinfulness of masturbation and of pre-marital intercourse. 
Consequently, it is not unusual to find even devoutly religious persons 
who become involved in the homosexual without any clear understanding 
of the church’s attitude on the subject. 

In general, however, the highest incidences of the homosexual are among 
the non-devout groups, and the lowest incidences are to be found among 
the more devout groups. In upper educational levels, among religiously 
inactive groups, something between 10 and 50 per cent more individuals 
may be involved than in the active groups ; and in lower educational levels 
the dijSerences in incidences are even greater. The differences in frequen- 
cies between the devout and non-devout groups are ordinarily much less, 
except that the homosexual among Orthodox Jewish groups appears to be 
phenomenally low. 


RELIGIOUS BASES OF THE MORES 

There are only minor differences in the emphases which the several 
religious groups have placed upon sexual morality. The strictly Orthodox 
Jewish code and the strict Catholic interpretations differ somewhat, but 
both of them accept the reproductive philosophy of sex, and both of them 
consider sexual activities which do not offer the possibility of fruition in 
reproduction as morally wrong. Consequently both of them vigorously 
condemn masturbation, and both of them attach a tremendous importance 
to the value of virginity at the time of marriage. The Jewish church main- 
tains its stand on the basis of Biblical and Talmudic interpretations. The 
Catholic church more often bases its interpretations on a natural philoso- 
phy which may be re-interpreted from time to time but which has always 
emphasized the abnormahty or the perverseness of sexual behavior which 
occurs outside of marriage. 



484 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Homosexual Outlet and Religion 


religious group 

cases 

TOTAL POPULATION 

active POPULATION 



Mean 

Frequency 

Median 

Freq. 

%of 

Total 

Outlet 

Incid. 

% 

Mean 

Freq. 

Median 

Freq. 


Single Males: Educational Level 0-8 


Age: Adol.-15 
Protestant, active 

89 

0.38 =«=0.09 

0.00 

15.3 

22.5 

1.68 

1.69 

Protestant, inactive 

481 

0.21 ±0.03 

0 00 

6.9 

22.5 

0.95 

0.36 

Catholic, inactive 

106 

0.31 ±0.11 

0.00 

9.9 

30.2 

1.03 

0.35 

Age: 16-20 
Protestant, active 

91 

0.33 ±0.12 

0.00 

12.2 

23.1 

1.43 

0.81 

Protestant, inactive 

493 

0.20 ±0.03 

0.00 

6.1 

24.5 

0.83 

0.30 

Catholic, inactive 

105 

0.34 ±0.08 

0.00 

10.2 

35.2 

0.97 

0.37 

Age: 21-25 
Protestant, inactive 

234 

0.21 ±0.05 

0.00 

6.4 

18.8 

1.10 

0.46 

Catholic, inactive 

60 

0.36 ±0.13 

0.00 

11.1 

40.0 

0.89 

0.30 


Single Males: Educational Level 9-12 


Age: Adol.-15 
Protestant, active 

93 

0.11 ±0.05 

0.00 

4.3 

15.1 

0.73 

0.09 

Protestant, inactive 

375 

0.28 ±0.04 

0.00 

8.1 

34.1 

0.81 

0.29 

Catholic, inactive 

103 

0.52 ±0.15 

0.00 

13.7 

46.6 

1.11 

0.41 

Age: 16-20 
Protestant, active 

95 

0.20 ±0.06 

0.00 

8.0 

30.5 

0.65 

0.10 

Protestant, inactive 

315 

0.30 ±0.05 

0.00 

8.7 

37.1 

0.81 

0.21 

Catholic, inactive 

101 

0.85 ±0.19 

0.06 

19.7 

59.4 

1.43 

0.63 


Single Males: Educational Level 13 + 


Age: Adol.-15 








Protestant, active 

547 

0.09 ±0.019 

0 00 

3.7 

23.8 

0.38 

0.09 

Protestant, inactive 

1471 

0.10 ±0.011 

0.00 

3.4 

25.6 

0.41 

0.09 

Catholic, devout 

132 

0.08 ±0.041 

0.00 

3.4 

16.7 

0.48 

0.08 

Catholic, inactive 

165 

0.13 ±0.040 

0.00 

4.3 

20.0 

0.64 

0.21 

Jewish, inactive 

601 

0.07 ±0.015 

0.00 

2.2 

14.6 

0.44 

0.10 

Age: 16-20 








Protestant, active 

557 

0.07 ±0.017 

0.00 

2.8 

17.4 

0.37 

0.09 

Protestant, inactive 

1513 

0.07 ±0.010 

0.00 

2.4 

18.0 

0.39 

0 08 

Catholic, devout 

136 

0.04 ±0.015 

0.00 

1.6 

14.6 

0.26 

0.10 

Catholic, inactive 

168 

0.13 ±0.037 

0.00 

4.8 

19.6 

0.69 

0.28 

Jewish, Orthodox 

59 

0.02 ±0.014 

0.00 

1.1 

11.9 

0.19 

0.08 

Jewish, inactive 

607 

0.06 ±0.015 

0.00 

2.0 

10.7 

0.55 

0.09 

Age: 21-25 








Protestant, active 

384 

0.04 ±0.015 

0.00 

2.0 

6.2 

0.63 

0.23 

Protestant, inactive 

1000 

0.09 ±0.016 

0.00 

3.5 

11.5 

0.79 

0.24 

Catholic, devout 

94 

0.08 ±0.037 

0.00 

4.3 

10.6 

0.73 

G.30 

Catholic, inactive 

125 

0.26 ±0.081 

0.00 

9.2 

15.2 

1.68 

1.50 

Jewish, inactive 

331 

0.07 ±0.025 

0.00 

2.3 

7.9 

0.89 

0.13 


Table 131. Homosexual outlet as related to religious background 




RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


485 


These restraints on sexual activities are well recognized among devout 
Catholics, and often have major effects on the personalities of these 
individuals. Devout Catholics are restrained in regard to the frequencies 
of their total outlet, and in regard to their acceptance of any variety of 
sexual outlets. Non-devout Catholics are much more active sexually. As 
the church might well contend, the Catholics who are most active sexually 
are those who are not good Catholics. There will be persons who will 
suggest that the higher rates of sexual activity among non-devout Catholics 
depend upon the fact that many poorly educated and immigrant groups 
belong to that church; but it should be pointed out again that all com- 
parisons between religious groups have been made for populations that 
are homogeneous in regard to five other biologic and social items, and 
that Catholic groups of particular educational levels have been compared 
only with other groups of exactly the same levels. We have no sufficient 
data for explaining these high rates of sexual activity among the non-devout 
Catholic groups. 

The intermediate positions of the Protestant groups are again in line 
with our understanding of the intermediate effectiveness of the control 
which the Protestant church attempts to exercise on sexual behavior. There 
are, of course, considerable differences between Protestant sects on this 
matter and often greater differences in the interpretations of sexual morali- 
ties among clergymen of the same sect. In general, the more literal groups 
of the Protestant church make sexual appraisals which are close to those 
of the Talmud and of the Catholic natural law; but a more liberal portion 
of the Protestant clergy is inclined to re-interpret all types of sexual be- 
havior in terms of the total social adjustment of the individual. 

Even though the differences between the sexual philosophies of these 
three religious groups are not great, one might have expected greater 
differences than those which actually exist between the histories of the 
adherents of the three groups. With one exception, there are surprisingly 
few differences between the behavior of equally devout or non-devout 
members of the three religious faiths. The one exception lies among the 
Orthodox Jewish males. Of all religious groups they are the sexually least 
active, both in regard to the frequencies of their total sexual outlet, and in 
regard to the incidences and frequencies of masturbation, nocturnal 
emissions, and the homosexual. They are closer to the males of other 
groups in regard to pre-marital petting and pre-marital intercourse. 

This relative inactivity of the Orthodox Jewish males is especially 
interesting, in view of the diametrically opposite opinion which recently 
stirred a considerable portion of Europe against the Jews as a race. It is 
further significant to note here that while the non-Orthodox are much 
more active than the Orthodox Jewish males, the sexually most active 
groups are religiously non-active Protestants or Catholics as often as they 
are Jewish. 



486 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


These data on the lower frequencies of sexual activity among the Ortho- 
dox Jews will occasion no surprise to those who understand the pervading 
asceticism of Hebrew philosophy. Non-devout Jewish groups, even includ- 
ing those who observe none of the Orthodox customs and who may be 
removed by several generations from ancestors who ever attended the 
synagogue, may still be controlled to a considerable degree by the Talmudic 
interpretations of sexual morality. There is a general opinion that Jewish 
groups discuss sexual matters publicly with less restraint than most other 
groups, and this opinion may provide some basis for the general im- 
pression that Jews are sexually more active. It has been notable, 
however, in a high proportion of our Jewish histories, that the freedom 
with which they record the details of their own sexual activities and the 
freedom with which they discuss those details, not only with us but with 
many of their fellows and with utter strangers, has surprisingly little 
relation to the extent of the overt activity in their individual sexual his- 
tories. The influence of the several thousand years of Jewish sexual philos- 
ophy is not to be ignored in the search for any final explanation of these 
data. 

The differences between religiously devout persons and religiously in- 
active persons of the same faith are much greater than the differences 
between two equally devout groups of different faiths. In regard to total 
sexual outlet the religiously inactive groups may have frequencies that are 
25 to 75 per cent higher than the frequencies of the religiously devout 
groups. Among religiously inactive males there are definitely higher 
frequencies of masturbation, pre-marital intercourse, marital intercourse, 
and the homosexual. 

The church, however, exerts a wider influence on even non-devout 
individuals, by way of the influence which it has had throughout the 
centuries upon the development of the sexual mores of our Western 
European-American culture. The religious codes have always and every- 
where been the prime source of those social attitudes which, in their 
aggregate, represent the sexual mores of all groups, devout or non-devout, 
church-going or non-church-going, rational, faithful to a creed, or merely 
following the custom of the land. It is, of course, often contended that 
social attitudes are the product of experience and that the wisdom thus 
acquired becomes the basis of the formalized systems of ethics which are 
recognized by various religious bodies. In theological terms, such systems 
are ascribed to divine revelation. Whether the religious, social, and legal 
systems came before the social experience, or the social experience before 
the formulations of the rules of behavior, is, however, a matter that needs 
careful historical investigation before any final conclusions are reached. 

In an older day, when church courts had authority over the life and 
death of each and every individual, departures from the expressed sexual 



RELIGIOUS BACKGROUND AND SEXUAL OUTLET 


487 


codes made the culprit painfully aware of the source of the sexual mores. 
In the present day, when most of the population refuses to recognize the 
jurisdiction of religious courts, the influence of the church is more indirect; 
but the ancient religious codes are still the prime sources of the attitudes, 
the ideas, the ideals, and the rationalizations by which most individuals 
pattern their sexual hves. 

No social level accepts the whole of the original Judaeo-Christian code, 
but each level derives its taboos from some part of the same basic religious 
philosophy. Whether sexual acts are evaluated in terms of what is right or 
wrong (as the upper social level puts it), or of what is natural or unnatural 
(as the lower social level considers it), the Hebraic and Christian concept 
of the reproductive function of sex lies back of both interpretations. The 
lower social level’s taboo on nudity (Chapter 10) has a long history in 
Jewish codes and in Catholic church rulings, and the upper level’s freer 
acceptance of nudity is in direct violation of church opinion. On the other 
hand, the upper level accepts the church’s restrictions on pre-marital and 
extra-marital intercourse (Chapter 10), while the lower level largely ignores 
the religious objections on those items. Particular individuals may come 
nearer to accepting the whole of the sexual code of the particular religious 
group to which they belong, but the patterns of every social level depart at 
some point from every church code. 

These apparent conflicts between the religious codes and the patterns of 
sexual behavior may lead one to overlook the religious origins of the social 
patterns. Nevertheless, the individual who denies that he is in any way 
influenced by church rulings still stoutly defends the church’s system of 
natural law, recognizes certain behavior as normal and other activities as 
unnatural, abnormal, and perverse, or considers that certain things (but 
only certain things) are fine, esthetically satisfactory, socially expedient, 
and decent for a mature and inteUigent male to engage in. In so contending, 
he perpetuates the tradition of the Judaic law and the Christian precept. 



488 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


I ] ANIMAL CONTACTS [[....L 


1^^ HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET [ j NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 



ADOL'15 16-ZO 21-25 26-30 31-35 36-40 

AGE GROUPS 

Figure 126, Sources of orgasm for total U. S. population, by age groups 


Summary data, corrected for distribution of age, marital status, and educational level 
shown in U. S. Census of 1940. Figures 126-133 on same scale, so percents of total out- 
let derived from each source may be seen by direct comparisons of all these figures. 



CHARTS SHOWING SOURCES OF SEXUAL OUTLET 


489 



POST- MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS 
EXTRA-MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS 
INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 
MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS 



Figure 127. Types of heterosexual coitus contributing to outlet of total U. S. 
population in each age period 


Summary data, corrected for distribution of age, marital status, and educational level 
shown in U. S. Census for 1940. Identical with Fi^re 126, except that the area of the 
total heterosexual intercourse is subdivided into its several sources. Figures 126 and 
127 on same scale, so direct comparisons can be made between figures. 



490 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


h- 

Ui 


O 



u. 

O 

H 



A6E GROUPS 


Figure 128. Sources of orgasm for single males of the grade school level, by 

age groups 


Figures 126-133 on same scale, so percents of total outlet derived from each source 
may be seen by direct comparisons of all these figures. 



AGE GROUPS 

Figure 129. Sources of orgasm for single males of the high school level, by age 

groups 

Figures 126-133 on same scale, so percents of total outlet derived from each source 
may be seen by direct comparisons of all these figures. 





CHARTS SHOWING SOURCES OF SEXUAL OUTLET 


491 



AD0W5 16-20 21-25 26-30 31-35 

AGE GROUPS 


Figure 130. Sources of orgasm for single males of the college level, by age groups 

Figures 126-133 on same scale, so percents of total outlet derived from each source 
may be seen by direct comparisons of ail these figures. 



AHimi CONTACTS 
HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 
INTERCOURSE WHH PROSTITUTES 
INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS 



PETTING TO CLIMAX 
NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 
MASTURBATION 


Key to Figures 128-130 



492 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 



16-ZO 21-25 26-50 SI-SS 36-40 41-45 46-50 51-55 

AGE GROUPS 


Figure 131. Sources of orgasm for married males of the grade school level, by 

age groups 

Figures 126-133 on same scale, so percents of total outlet derived from each source 
may be seen by direct comparisons of all these figures. 



16-20 21-25 26-30 '31-35 36-40 41-45 

AGE GROUPS 


Figure 132. Sources of orgasm for married males of the high school level, by 

age groups 

Figures 126-133 on same scale, so percents of total outlet derived from each source 
may be seen by direct comparisons of all these figures. 




PERCEI 


CHARTS SHOWING SOURCES OF SEXUAL OUTLET 


493 



16-ZO 21-25 26-30 31-35 36-40 41-45 46-50 51-55 

AGE GROUPS 


Figure 133. Sources of orgasm for married males of the college level, by age 

groups 

Figures 126-133 on same scale, so percents of total outlet derived from each source 
may be seen by direct comparisons of all these figures. 


HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


EXTRA-MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH COMPANIONS 


□ 


MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 


EXTRA' MARITAL INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES | 
Key to Figxires 131-133 


MASTURBATION 



Part III 


SOURCES OF SEXUAL OUTLET 

CHAPTER 14 . MASTURBATION 
CHAPTER 15 . NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 
CHAPTER 16 . HETEROSEXUAL PETTING 
CHAPTER 17 . PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 
CHAPTER 18 . MARITAL INTERCOURSE 
CHAPTER 19 . EXTRA-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 
CHAPTER 20 . INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 
CHAPTER 21 . HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 
CHAPTER 22 . ANIMAL CONTACTS 




Chapter 14 


MASTURBATION 

The previous section of this volume has been occupied with an examina- 
tion of the factors which affect human sexual behavior. Such biologic 
items as age and the age at onset of adolescence, and such social factors 
as educational level, occupational class of the subject and of the subject’s 
parents, the rural-urban backgrounds of the individual, and the religious 
backgrounds have been analyzed as factors affecting the total sexual outlet 
and each of the particular types of sexual outlet. The remainder of this 
volume will summarize the record for each source of outlet: masturbation 
(in the present chapter), and nocturnal emissions, pre-marital intercourse, 
homosexual contacts, and other sources of outlet (in the subsequent chap- 
ters). Although many of the specific data in this section will be drawn from 
material presented elsewhere in the book, these chapters will be especially 
concerned with interpretations of the data, and will summarize the nature 
of each type of behavior, emphasize the individual variation that occurs, 
discuss the correlations of each type of activity with each other source of 
outlet, and show something of the significance of these factors to the indi- 
vidual and to the society of which he is a part. 

DEFINITION 

The term masturbation may be applied to any sort of self stimulation 
which brings erotic arousal. Since, as we have already seen (Chapter 5), all 
tactile responses and still others of the sensory responses are basic to sex- 
ual activity, there is considerable justice in extending the concept of mas- 
turbation to all situations in which there is tactile stimulation. Freud (1938) 
and many of the analysts and other clinicians (Meagher 1924, Meagher and 
Jelhffe 1936, Mowrer and Kluckhohn in Hunt 1944, Lorand 1944, Car- 
michael 1946, Landis and Bolles 1946) use the word in this way, especially 
in connection with the behavior of younger children. When so defined, the 
phenomenon of masturbation is recognizable as universal among both 
males and females, from the youngest child to the oldest adult; but this is 
not the concept of masturbation held by the pubhc in general, nor by most 
clinicians who inquire about it or report it in the histories of their patients. 
As more usually employed, the word “masturbation” refers to any self 
stimulation which is deliberate and designed to effect erotic arousal. By 
such a definition, the accidental touching of oneself is not masturbation 
because it is not deliberate. As so defined, not only tactile stimulation, but 
all other sorts of sensory and psychic stimulation, if deliberate and de- 

497 



498 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


signed to bring satisfaction, fall under this head. Rubbing or scratching 
one’s body, even one’s genitalia, is not masturbation when it serves some 
other function than that of effecting erotic arousal. Throughout this vol- 
ume the word has not been applied to anything except deliberate self 
stimulation. 

When so strictly defined, masturbation cannot be taken to be as uni- 
versal as some of the psychiatrists and psychologists would have it. The 
extension of the meaning of the word has, unfortunately, distorted the in- 
terpretation of the actual data on the phenomenon; and it is to be sug- 
gested that the analysts would do better to describe a good deal of what 
they observe, especially among younger children, as tactile experience, 
which is exactly what it is, and not call it masturbation until there is evi- 
dence that the child is reaping an erotic reward for his activity, and that the 
behavior has been inspired by some anticipation of such a reward. 

REFERENCES 

Data on the occurrence of masturbation, its incidences and frequencies 
in various segments of the male population, have already been detailed in 
this volume in the following tables and figures : 


PAGE 

TABLE 

HGURE 

190, 191 

38 

30 

234 

49 

136-137 

238-243 

51 

53-58 

273-277 

61 

53-58 

300-303 

68 

89 

310-313 

72 , 73 

91-92 

339-343 

82 , 108 , 115 

98 , 124 

375-380 

96-97 

106-107 

396 - 399 , 

407-413 

98 , 103-105 

108 , 119 , 122,123 

450 - 453 , ' 

117 

125 

462 

470-476 

488-493 

126 

126 , 128-133 


499-502 4 , 17 , 22 , 132 8 , 16 , 24 , 134-135 
504-506 7 , 136-137 


NATURE OF DATA 

Sources of first ejaculation 
Range of variation and age 
Age and masturbation 
Marital status and masturbation 
Sources of first ejaculation versus age at 
onset of adolescence 

Age at onset of adolescence as related to 
masturbation 

Social level and masturbation 
Masturbation in patterns of behavior at 
different educational levels 
Older and younger generations and mas- 
turbation 

Rural and urban groups and masturba- 
tion 

Religious backgrounds and masturbation 
Significance of masturbation as one source 
of total outlet 

Accumulative incidence of masturbation 
Individual variation in masturbation 



MASTURBATION 


499 


INCIDENCES AND FREQUENCIES 

Incidences. By even the stricter definition, masturbation may be identified 
in the histories of a very high proportion of the human males. Ultimately 
about 92 per cent of the total population is involved in masturbation which 
leads to orgasm (Table 132, Figures 134, 135). More individuals (96%) of 
the college level and 95 per cent of the high school group, are ultimately 
included, fewer (89%) of the males who never go beyond grade school. The 
general opinion that all males masturbate at some time in their lives, and 
the easy acceptance of this opinion among many clinicians and educators, 
are not warranted by the actual record. There are some individuals who do 
not masturbate for the simple reason that they do not have sufficient sex 
drive to cause them to go out of their way to find any sort of outlet, and 
who depend on nocturnal emissions for most of their orgasms. There are 
some boys, particularly at lower social levels, who do not masturbate be- 
cause they become involved in heterosexual coitus at such an early age that 
they have little need for other sources of outlet. There are some duller and 
slower reacting individuals who find it impossible to effect orgasm in mas- 
turbation, and who in consequence make no attempt to masturbate after 
their first experiments. Thus there is a group of males who definitely do not 
have masturbatory histories, although the percentage is as small as is in- 
dicated above. 

In several of the previous studies on human male sexual behavior (Mer- 
rill 1918, Peck and Wells 1923, 1925, Hughes 1926, Hamilton 1929, Dick- 
inson and Beam 1931, Peterson 1938, Wile 1941, Ramsey 1943, Finger 
1947, Hohman and Schaffner 1947), similar incidence figures have been 
obtained. It is interesting to find that a number of the European studies 
report comparable incidences (85% to 96%) among European males (Roh- 
leder 1902, 1921; also summary in Haire 1937). There is every reason to 
believe that the lower figures obtained in some of the other American 
studies (Brockman 1902, Exner 1915, Achilles 1923, Taylor 1933, Bromley 
and Britten 1938) represent failures to obtain the fact. It must be realized 
that masturbation is taboo and even strongly condemned among certain 
groups (Chapter 10); and while college men more often admit their ex- 
perience, there are males in some other groups who would admit almost 
any other kind of sexual activity before they would give a record of mas- 
turbatory experience. On the other hand, the high incidence of masturba- 
tion in the male should not be taken as warrant for believing that there is a 
similarly high incidence in the female. The data on the female will be pre- 
sented in a later volume. 

Pre-adolescent Activity. For two-thirds (68.4%) of the boys, self mastur- 
bation provides the first ejaculation. For most of the other boys, nocturnal 
emissions and heterosexual coitus provide the first ejaculation. There is 
little variation in these data for different social levels. Masturbation is more 



500 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Masturbation: Accumulative Incidence Data 


age 

total 

population 

u. s. 

CORRECTIONS 

EDUC. LEVEL 

0-8 

EDUC. 

9- 

LEVEL 

12 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13 + 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

8 

3960 

0.1 

661 

0.0 

484 

0.2 

2815 

0.0 

9 

3960 

0.3 

661 

0.0 

484 

0.6 

2815 

0.3 

10 

3960 

2.0 

661 

0.8 

484 

2.5 

2815 

2.3 

11 

3959 

6.1 

660 

3.2 

484 

7.0 

2815 

8.9 

12 

3959 

21.2 

660 

15.5 

484 

22.7 

2815 

27.9 

13 

3959 

44.9 

660 

34.4 

484 

48.6 

2815 

52.9 

14 

3956 

71.7 

657 

60.3 

484 

77.9 

2815 

72.2 

15 

3950 

82.2 

651 

77.3 

484 

85.5 

2815 

80.2 

16 

3929 

87.6 

634 

84.5 

481 

90.2 

2814 

84.3 

17 

3870 

90.2 

597 

86.6 

461 

93.1 

2812 

87.0 

18 

3735 

91.8 

573 

89.4 

426 

93.9 

2736 

88.9 

19 

3504 

92.1 

543 

89.5 

389 

94.1 

2572 

90.0 

20 

3200 

92.1 

515 

89.7 

348 

93.7 

2337 

91.1 

21 

2827 

92.6 

491 

89.6 

305 

94.4 

2031 

92.0 

22 

2425 

92.7 

472 

89.6 

283 

94.3 

1670 

92.8 

23 

2110 

93.5 

456 

89.7 

258 

95.7 

1396 

93.3 

24 

1819 

93.8 

436 

89.9 

232 

96.1 

1151 

93:1 

25 

1634 

93.4 

416 

89.7 

216 

96.3 

1002 

93.9 

26 

1491 

93.3 

405 

89.4 

202 

96.0 

884 

94.9 

27 

1356 

93.1 

391 

89.0 

191 

95.8 

774 

95.3 

28 

1250 

92.8 

377 

88.6 

174 

95.4 

699 

95.3 

29 

1141 

92.3 

353 

88.1 

154 

94.8 

634 

95.0 

30 

1047 

91.5 

337 

87.8 

137 

94.2 

573 

95.6 

31 i 

971 

90.9 

317 

87.1 

125 

93.6 

529 

95.3 

32 

913 

91.2 

305 

87.2 

116 

94.0 

492 

95.5 

33 ! 

854 

90.8 

293 

86.7 

113 

93.8 

448 

95.3 

34 

802 

90.9 

285 

86.3 

105 

94.3 

412 

95.9 

35 

745 

90.2 

271 

86.3 

92 

94.6 

382 

95.8 

36 

701 

90.6 

258 

87.2 

87 

94.3 

356 

95.8 

37 

639 

90.0 

240 

86.7 

76 

93.4 

323 

95.7 

38 

609 

89.6 

232 

86.2 

70 

92.9 

307 

95.8 

39 

554 

89.1 

210 

85.7 

64 

92.2 

280 

95.4 

40 

507 

88.3 

192 

85.4 

58 

91.4 

257 

96.1 

41 

472 

87.5 

181 

84.5 

53 

90.6 

238 

95.8 

42 

444 

87.3 

173 

84.4 

50 

90.0 

221 

95.5 


Table 132. Acciimulative incidence data on masturbation 

^ Covering the life span, including both single and married histories. In three educa- 
tional levels, and in the total population corrected for the U. S. Census of 1940. 



MASTURBATION 


501 


likely to provide the first experience (72%) for the boys who become ado- 
lescent at an early age, less likely (52%) for the boys who are slowest in 
development (Table 68, Figure 89). 

The inspiration for the first experimentation in masturbation is a matter 
which will need more extensive consideration in a later study on sex educa- 
tion. It may be stated now that nearly all boys have heard about masturba- 
tion before they attempt it themselves, and a high proportion has observed 
companions masturbating. This is particularly true in the grade school and 
high school levels of society; but some males of the college level will be 
surprised that there are so few boys who discover masturbation on their 
own initiative, because it is chiefly at that level that masturbation is inde- 
pendently discovered by some boys. After early adolescence, there are 
many males who never have an opportunity to observe another male in 
sexual performance, and consequently it is notable that so many boys do 
observe masturbation in connection with their initial experiences. The fe- 
male more often discovers masturbation independently and without any 
previous knowledge that any other person has ever been involved in similar 
activity. 

However extensive the incidental touching of genitalia may be, specific 
masturbation is quite rare among younger boys. Of course, there are cases 
of infants under a year of age who have learned the advantage of specific 
manipulation, sometimes as a result of being so manipulated by older per- 
sons ; and there are some boys who masturbate quite specifically and with 
some frequency from the age of two or three. But most young boys, in at- 
tempting masturbation, engage in such desultory motions and so quickly 
cease their efforts that no satisfaction is obtained and they are, therefore, 
not interested in trying again. When an older person provides the more 
specific sort of manipulation which is usual among adults, the same child 
may be much aroused, and in a high proportion of the cases may be brought 
to actual orgasm (Chapter 5). 

As far as the available data indicate it is, then, a relatively small number 
of the younger pre-adolescents who, in any strict sense, masturbate. Not 
more than 10 per cent seems to have done so before the age of nine, and 13 
per cent before the age of ten. Most boys are ten, eleven or twelve years old 
before they become involved. These are minimum data, derived chiefly 
from the memories of adults, and adults sometimes forget their childhood 
experiences. Comparisons of records from children and from adults 
(Chapter 5) indicate that the actual figures may be somewhat higher, but 
not more than 20 per cent higher, z.e., instead of 13 per cent it may be as 
many as 16 per cent of the boys who masturbate before age ten. 

There appear to be some differences between social levels in the inci- 
dences of pre-adolescent masturbation, but such differences are fewer than 
are ordinarily found between social levels in adolescence. Actually, few 



502 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


boys begin masturbation until they are near the age when regular erotic 
responses are recognizable, which means not more than a year or two be- 
fore adolescence for most of them, even though a larger proportion would 
be capable of definite response at a much earlier age if sufficient contacts 



Figure 134. Masturbation: accumulative incidence, in total U. S. population 


Showing percent of total population that has ever had masturbatory experience by 
each of the indicated ages. All data based on total population, irrespective of marital 
status, and corrected for the U. S. Census distribution. 



Figure 135. Masturbation: accumulative incidence in three educational levels 

Showing percent of each population that has ever had masturbatory experience by each 
of the indicated ages. All data based on total population, irrespective of marital status. 

were had. It is, of course, quite understandable that the boy should not be 
interested until there is a sufficient return for his efforts. 

Some of the pre-adolescents carry their masturbation through to a defi- 
nite and satisfactory orgasm, although in some instances the boy may not 


MASTURBATION 


503 


recognize what is happening, and does not identify the experience by either 
name or description as something that other boys have had. Nevertheless, 
masturbation in the younger boy is usually a definite sort of performance 
which is often limited to a few minutes in time, and which ceases abruptly 
when, as he may report, he has had enough of it. This means either that 
he has reached orgasm, or that he has found at least some sort of release 
from the tension which initiated the activity and which may have been aug- 
mented in the course of the performance. Some adolescent boys and many 
adults recall specific orgasm with all of its adult characteristics occurring 
before they had acquired the ability to ejaculate; and there are definite 
records (Chapter 5) on several hundred boys who have been observed in 
pre-adolescent orgasm which was achieved either through self masturba- 
tion or through socio-sexual contacts. Among the older psychiatrists there 
are some who go so far as to state, dogmatically, that no pre-adolescent 
ever experienced orgasm unless he was neurotic. Such a statement smacks 
of something other than scientific objectivity and is, of course, unaccept- 
able in view of the observations now at hand. 

Adults are often disturbed when they discover young children masturbat- 
ing, and many a clinician supports the parents’ fears and lends little com- 
fort to the child who is taken to the doctor to be cured of his biologically 
normal capacities. Inasmuch as nearly all boys arrive at masturbation 
sooner or later, it may be asked why one should worry over pre-adolescent 
or even infantile masturbation. If it is a moral issue, the answer must come 
from someone else than the scientists, and be treated as a question of mor- 
als (as it is in Kirsch 1930, Ruland and Rattler 1934, Fleege 1945, Davis 
1946 vol. 2). If it is a question of physical outcome, the issue is for the 
biologist; and it should be made clear that there is no evidence, among the 
thousands of histories now at hand, that the boy who begins masturbating 
at an early age suffers any more harm than the boy who delays the begin- 
ning of his experience until some time in adolescence or later. And most 
scientists and clinicians are now agreed that masturbation does no harm 
at the later ages. 

If the question is one of social values, it may be stated that there is no 
record of early masturbation disturbing the child’s adjustments except in 
some of the cases where adults discovered the activity, reprimanded or 
punished the youngster, made a public exhibition of the offense, or upset 
the child’s peace of mind in some other way. Even the parents who try to 
avoid reprimands may cause some disturbance in the child because they, 
the parents themselves, are inhibited, or because they are not accustomed 
to observing sexual behavior of any sort. It takes no more than a show of 
surprise on the part of the parent, a supercilious smile, or even a studied 
avoidance of the issue to make it apparent to the child that the parent is 
emotionally upset, and that sexual activity is in a different category from 



504 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 



Figure 136. Masturbation: individual variation in frequencies, at ages adolescent- 
15 and 16“20, for three educational levels 

Showing percent of each population (vertical line) which masturbates with each 
type of frequency (horizontal line)^ 







Masturbation 


505 



Figure 137. Masturbation: individual variation in frequencies, at ages 21-25 and 
26-30, for three educational levels 

Showing percent of each population (vertical line) which masturbates with each type 
of frequency (horizontal line). 





506 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


other everyday affairs. Children, even infants and very young children, are 
especially sensitive to the reactions of other persons. If the child is seriously 
disturbed over his behavior, the disturbance may color his personality 
throughout life, as the psychiatrist and psychologist well know. 

For the parent who intends that the child, or even the older adolescent, 
shall not be upset over masturbation, it is a matter of accepting the be- 
havior without allowing it to appear important (Weiss and English 1943), 
while still making it clear that such activity in front of other individuals 
may bring social difficulties. The careful adjustments which are worked out 
in the home may be completely upset by the violent reactions of other chil- 
dren or adults who become aware of the child’s masturbation. Neverthe- 
less, there are cases of parents who have succeeded in accomplishing this 
delicate adjustment between things that are acceptable in the home and 
things that other people outside the home “just don’t understand.” 

Adolescent Activity. When specific masturbation does occur in pre- 
adolescence, it is almost invariably continued into adolescence. 

For most males, of every social level, masturbation provides the chief 
source of sexual outlet in early adolescence. It is at that period that the 
activity reaches its highest frequencies. For those males who subsequently 
turn to socio-sexual contacts for their pre-marital outlet, masturbatory 
frequencies will never again be so high. 

Masturbation and pre-marital intercourse are the only types of sexual 
activity which show something of the same range of individual variation 
(Table 49, Figures 136, 137) as is shown by total outlet (Chapter 6). There 
are males who never masturbate. There are a few males who masturbate 
only once or twice in their lives ; and there are others who have frequencies 
that may average seven to fourteen or twenty or more per week for long 
periods of years. There are males whose high frequencies extend from pre- 
adolescence through all of the pre-marital years, and males who may main- 
tain average frequencies of three or four a week through the marital years 
into old age. There are a few males who are still masturbating at seventy- 
five years of age, but there are no older ones in the available record whose 
masturbation results in orgasm. Some males may masturbate several thou- 
sand times as often as some others in the population. The statement about 
lack of harmful outcome still applies to these most active cases. In the 
present records, the highest-rating males were masturbating with average 
frequencies of 23 per week in early adolescence. These maximum average 
frequencies drop to 15 per week by twenty years of age, to 6 per week by 
fifty years of age, and to once in two weeks at sixty years of age. It is about 
then that the older males are most inclined to warn the adolescent boy that 
masturbation will certainly harm him if he does it to excess. 

For the active population, average frequencies of masturbation in early 
adolescence are nearly two and a half (2.4) per week, but a goodly number 



508 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


(Table 82). At the college level, masturbation involves most of the males 
(96%) and continues to be the chief source (about 60%) of the outlet up 
until the time of marriage. More than two-thirds (69%) of the college-bred 
males have some masturbation in their histories after marriage, and this 
provides no small part (about 9%) of the total outlet of the group after 
marriage. On the other hand, masturbation in the more poorly educated 
groups may begin to drop out almost immediately after it is begun. There 
are lower level males who have masturbated only a single time or two, or a 
few times in their lives. Some of them may masturbate for a year or two, 
but then stop. By sixteen years of age 16 per cent of them has stopped mas- 
turbating, and nearly 40 per cent has stopped by the time age twenty has 
been passed. In these late teens masturbation supplies only about a fourth 
(29.2%) of the outlet for the lower level males. Most males of this level find 
it difficult to understand how a grown man could think of masturbating, 
particularly if he is married and living with his wife. 

The average frequencies of masturbation differ between social levels, 
more than the incidences. Average frequencies (active population) for the 
boys of the grade school level, between adolescence and fifteen years of 
age, are 1 .8 per week ; for the corresponding high school level, 2.2 per week ; 
for the boys who will ultimately go to college, 2.7 per week (Table 82). The 
differences in frequencies become greater in the later age groups, and be- 
tween adolescence and marriage the males of the college level masturbate 
more than twice as frequently as the males of the grade school level. 

At lower levels there are definite taboos against masturbation. These 
may be fortified with the explanation that masturbation will drive one 
crazy, give one pimples, make one weak, or do some other sort of physical 
harm. More often masturbation is simply rejected because it is considered 
unnatural. The entire sexual philosophy at this level is turned around the 
acceptance of what is natural and the rejection of activities that are un- 
natural. The upper level’s wider acceptance of masturbation is rationalized 
on the ground that scientific investigation shows that it does no physical 
harm. Actually, however, the acceptance of masturbation in the upper 
level is probably the result of the very strong taboos which that group has 
against pre-marital intercourse. It is not a case of liking masturbation more, 
so much as it is a case of liking non-marital heterosexual relations less. 

The masturbatory records of older and younger generations of males 
from the college level are practically identical, both as to the percentage of 
persons involved and the frequencies of the activity (Tables 98, 104). The 
present-day masturbatory pattern of the college level male goes back at 
least twenty-two years; but there is a more conscious, more generally 
verbalized acceptance of the reality among college-bred males today. 
Moreover, the upper level attitude and its scientific acceptance of mas- 
turbation seems to have extended to at least some of the more poorly edu- 



MASTURBATION 


509 


cated males in the population. For the younger generation of the grade 
school level, for instance, masturbation begins earlier, includes more per- 
sons, and is had nearly twice as frequently as it was had in the older gen- 
eration. 

Boys in rural areas masturbate less often than boys raised in cities or 
towns, especially during adolescence (Table 117). But since the farm boy 
has a somewhat lower total outlet than the city boy, and since the farm boy 
has definitely lower rates of socio-sexual contacts, masturbation provides 
a higher percentage of his total outlet than it does for the city boy. 

Masturbation occurs with the lowest incidences and frequencies among 
Jewish males who are Orthodox and among devout Catholics, and it occurs 
with the highest frequencies among religiously inactive Protestants (Table 
126). The religious codes, both Jewish and Christian, have been the prime 
source of the taboos on masturbation (Chapter 13). 

TECHNIQUES 

In the human male, masturbatory techniques are largely manual. They 
usually constitute a deliberate attempt to provide genital stimulation which 
will result in the satisfaction of orgasm. There are very few males who de- 
liberately avoid orgasm as the conclusion of the activity, although a few 
of them may deliberately prolong the act into a matter of several minutes 
or more — sometimes to half an hour or an hour or more, in order to ex- 
tend the sensory satisfaction. Most males carry the activity through to 
climax as rapidly as is possible, which means it does not ordinarily continue 
for more than a minute or two. Some males, indeed, are able to achieve 
orgasm quite regularly in a half minute or so, sometimes in ten or twenty 
seconds. 

There are some boys who attempt to masturbate by moving the penis 
against a bed or against some other object; but for most males this tech- 
nique is rare and confined to an incidental experience or two. Surprisingly 
enough, this method seems to be common only among the males of a par- 
ticular group; but the specific data are insutficient to present at this time, 
and it has been impossible to get any clue as to the origin or significance 
of this pattern. Many of the persons who depend upon this mode of mas- 
turbation think of the act as a substitute for heterosexual coitus, and there 
are some clinicians who specifically recommend such a technique, in con- 
junction with fantasy, on the ground that it may provide some carry-over 
into later heterosexual activity. But however good the theory may appear 
to be, it finds no substantiation in the specific record, for the great major- 
ity of the boys use simpler manual techniques in masturbation, and make 
perfectly satisfactory heterosexual adjustments anyway. It is also to be 
noted that males have been known to masturbate against a bed while 
fantasying frictation or anal relations in the homosexual. 



510 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Self fellation is an anatomic impossibility for most human males, but it is 
a common means of masturbation among rhesus monkeys, the macaque, 
mandrille, chimpanzees and other primates (Carpenter 1942, National Re- 
search Council Conference on Mammalian Sex Behavior, 1943), and oc- 
curs quite widely among mammals of many other groups (Beach 1947). 
Throughout vertebrate sexual behavior there is such a close tie-up between 
oral eroticism and genital stimulation that oral activity of any sort must be 
accepted by the scientist as a biologically normal aspect of sexuality. Its 
tremendous suppression in the human animal must be taken to be the out- 
come of cultural developments. Consequently, it is not surprising to find 
that the human male, with his animal background, does sometimes at- 
tempt self fellation. It has taken special interviewing techniques (Chapter 2) 
to get adults to admit such experience, but a considerable portion of the 
population does record attempts at self fellation, at least in early adoles- 
cence. Only two or three males in a thousand are able to achieve the ob- 
jective, but there are three or four histories of males who had depended 
upon self fellation as a masturbatory technique for some appreciable period 
of time — in the case of one thirty-year old male, for most of his life. In his 
psychic drive, the human animal is more mammalian than even his anat- 
omy allows him to be. 

Only a limited number of individuals extend their masturbatory tech- 
niques to involve any variety of other procedures. Such experimentation is 
most often found among better educated individuals who have well de- 
veloped imaginative capacities and who are, of course, the ones most likely 
to have a minimum of overt socio-sexual contacts. The manual techniques 
of genital manipulation are elaborated by a few individuals. The use of 
literature and erotic pictures for stimulation during masturbation is not 
really common, and it is largely confined to better educated individuals. 
Urethral insertions and other masochistic techniques, and anal stimulation 
and anal insertions occur only very occasionally. Sometimes devices which 
simulate the female genitalia may be used for masturbation, but they are 
rarely employed. Most males restrict themselves to a limited series of par- 
ticular techniques to which they have been erotically conditioned. 

Nearly, but not quite, all males experience sexual fantasies during mas- 
turbation. The female fantasies much less often while masturbating. Mas- 
turbatory fantasies accord with the general psychiatric and psychologic 
understanding of the matter. The fantasies are heterosexual when the pri- 
mary interests of the individual are heterosexual, homosexual when the in- 
dividual’s overt experience or psychic reactions are homosexual. They may 
be alternately heterosexual and homosexual in the case of the individual 
who reacts definitely in both directions. The fantasies may include animal 
contacts for boys who have had such animal experience as some farm boys 
have. There are occasional sadistic or masochistic fantasies. Just as with 



MASTURBATION 


511 


nocturnal dreams (Chapter 15), there may be some striking disparities be- 
tween the nature of the fantasies accompanying masturbation and the overt 
experience of the male, and one cannot discover the history of an individual 
merely by finding out what he thinks about when he masturbates. 

Where the masturbatory techniques are manual, many individuals find 
some additional stimulation in observing their own genitalia ; and this may 
have some homosexual significance, although most persons with such his- 
tories may deny any other homosexual interests. A considerable portion of 
the pre-eminently homosexual males whose homosexual activities involve 
mutual masturbation or oral techniques do observe their own genitalia 
during self masturbation; but not even all of them do so. Some of the most 
vigorously heterosexual males, however, carefully avoid any observation 
of their genitalia during masturbation, and their performances depend pri- 
marily upon involved heterosexual fantasies. Many of these persons mas- 
turbate in the dark, in order to concentrate the better upon the imagery. 
It is an important question whether masturbation should be interpreted as 
a narcissistic performance or a socio-sexual activity, and one is not war- 
ranted in considering that all masturbation is a matter of self interest. There 
are some individuals for whom masturbation is a distinctly heterosexual or 
a distinctly homosexual experience, depending upon the strength of the 
fantasy and the abundance of the associations which complement the ac- 
tivity. 

CORRELATIONS WITH OTHER OUTLETS 

It has not been possible to make precise correlations between the fre- 
quencies of the various types of sexual outlet for the present study; and at 
this time the relations between masturbation and the other outlets may only 
be suggested in the most general terms. 

There may be some correlation between the frequencies of masturbation 
and the frequencies of nocturnal dreams. In general, the males who have the 
highest frequencies of nocturnal emissions may have somewhat lower rates 
of masturbation. Some of these males credit the frequent emissions to the 
fact that they do not masturbate; but it is just as likely that the reverse re- 
lationship is true, namely, that they do not masturbate because they have 
frequent emissions. 

On the other hand, there is little evidence that high frequencies of mas- 
turbation reduce the frequencies of nocturnal emissions (Chapter 15). Even 
where there are high masturbatory frequencies and low frequencies of noc- 
turnal emissions, the possibihty should not be overlooked that those par- 
ticular males nevdr would have had frequent emissions, even if they had 
stopped masturbating. 

There may be some relationship between masturbation and pre-marital 
petting with females. Both of these activities are most frequent at upper 
levels, but it is probable that both are the products of the upper level sexual 



512 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


philosophy, rather than the products of each other. Nevertheless, the gen- 
ital manipulations which are employed in masturbation may provide some 
introduction to the techniques of heterosexual petting. 

Masturbation sometimes shows a complementary relationship with pre- 
marital coitus. Where the one is high, the other is likely to be lower. Where 
there is sufficient coitus, it may be that there is not much need for masturba- 
tion. On the other hand, it remains to be demonstrated that a sufficiency of 
masturbation reduces the incentive to find a socio-sexual outlet. There are 
those who believe so, and recommend masturbation as a means of con- 
trolling what they consider the more immoral pre-marital activity. There 
are also those, including not a few psychiatrists, who feel that it would be 
unfortunate if pre-marital masturbation reduced the urge to make a hetero- 
sexual adjustment. It will take a carefully objective study to show what the 
real relationships may be. 

Any relation which may exist between masturbation and the homosexual 
similarly needs to be studied in detail (Taylor 1933). It has already been 
suggested that an interest in one’s own genitalia may be transferred to an 
interest in the genitalia of another individual of the same sex. Certainly 
there are some records of the sort in the histories now at hand ; but the 
number of clear-cut cases is not large. Careful analyses of a considerable 
series of individual histories should be made before any conclusion is 
reached on these matters. 

Even some of the animal contacts which the farm boy has may have been 
inspired by his own masturbatory experience, for the masturbation of farm 
animals and of household pets is about as frequent as coitus or oral rela- 
tions with the animals. In a considerable number of cases the boy’s relation 
is had with a male animal which he masturbates. If the boy is erotically 
aroused in such a case, the relationship may involve some homosexual ele- 
ment, as well as the zodphilic. 

SIGNIFICANCE OF MASTURBATION 

There are no other sexual activities which involve anywhere near so 
many individuals as are involved in heterosexual coitus and masturbation ; 
and there are no other activities which provide so large a proportion of the 
total orgasms which the average male experiences in his lifetime. For most 
males, coitus is primary, masturbation secondary in importance; but for 
males of the college level masturbation is, as we have seen, the chief source 
of outlet up to the time of marriage. It is a question whether activities which 
are as important as these can be altogether ignored, easily regulated, or 
completely ruled out of the lives of any large number of people. Unmarried 
youths who had neither masturbation nor coitus (nor the homosexual) in 
their histories would be left with essentially no outlet except nocturnal 
emissions. That would nearly amount to abstinence, for such emissions do 



MASTURBATION 


513 


not ordinarily account for more than 10 or 12 per cent of the orgasms of 
any group of males, and there is no evidence that the frequencies of such 
emissions can be materially increased by avoiding other sexual activities 
(Chapter 15). The moral desirability of eliminating masturbation is, of 
course, an issue whose merits scientists are not qualified to judge. Whether 
such a program is psychologically or socially desirable or physically pos- 
sible for any large number of males is a question that can be submitted to 
scientific examination (Chapter 6). 

Throughout history, both the Jewish and Christian churches have con- 
demned masturbation as either immoral or unnatural (Chapter 13). In 
more recent years, with an increase in public respect for science, the moral 
arguments have been supported with statements concerning the physical 
and mental harm supposed to come from the continuance of such a habit. 
The older males who have contributed to the present study were adolescent 
in a day in which there was widespread teaching against the sin of self- 
abuse (e.g„ Vecki 1901, 1920, G. S. Hall 1904, W. S. Hall 1907, 1909, 1920, 
Boy Scout Manual, all editions 1911-1945, Jefferis and Nichols 1912, 
Wulffen 1913, Lieber 1920, U. S. Public Health Service 1921, 1934, 
Coppens and Spalding 1921, Forel 1922, Meyer 1927, 1929a, 1929b, 
Weatherhead 1932, Bloch 1933, Crisp 1939, T. V. Moore 1945). Every 
conceivable ill from pimples to insanity, including stooped shoulders, loss 
of weight, fatigue, insomnia, general weakness, neurasthenia, loss of manly 
vigor, weak eyes, digestive upsets, stomach ulcers, impotence, feeble- 
mindedness, genital cancer, and the rest, was ascribed to masturbation. 
Feeble-minded and insane individuals in the neighborhood were held 
up as horrid examples of the result of masturbation, and the authorities in 
mental institutions maintained separate wards for those whose insanity 
was supposed to have originated from such practices. Patients in such in- 
stitutions were observed to engage in frequent masturbation, and this 
seemed sufficient proof that the insanity was a product of the sexual be- 
havior. Since the lives of university scholars were not so easily observed, it 
was not so generally known that masturbation occurred quite as frequently 
among them. Thousands of patients in mental institutions were put into 
strait jackets or other restraints, on the assumption that they had no chance 
of recovery unless the masturbation was controlled and cured. There are 
mental institutions which are operated on the same theory today. In many 
penal institutions inmates may still be punished severely if found mastur- 
bating, and in some homes for children and in some other institutions the 
older attitudes are still enforced. The United States Naval Academy at An- 
napolis rules that a candidate “shall be rejected by the examining surgeon 
for . . . evidence of . . . masturbation” (U. S. Navy Dpt. 1940). 

Millions of boys have lived in continual mental conflict over this prob- 
lem. For that matter, many a boy still does. Many boys pass through a pe- 



514 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


riodic succession of attempts to stop the habit, inevitable failures in those 
attempts, consequent periods of remorse, the making of new resolutions — 
and a new start on the whole cycle. It is diflScult to imagine anything better 
calculated to do permanent damage to the personality of an individual. 

For several decades now, educators, clinical psychologists and psychia- 
trists, and some of the general medical practitioners have come to agree 
that the physical effects of masturbation are not fundamentally different 
from the physical effects of any other sexual activity, and that any mental 
harm resulting from masturbation is an outcome of the conflicts introduced 
by the condemnation of the boy’s activity (e.g., Tenenbaum in Robinson 
1936, Willoughby 1937, Haire 1937, Butterfield 1939, Himes 1940, Kirk- 
endall 1940, Allen 1940, Weiss and English 1943, Sadler and Sadler 1944, 
English and Pearson 1945, Frank 1946, Seward 1946). 

In the present study we have examined the histories of 5300 males, of 
which about 5100 record experience in masturbation. It would be difficult 
to show that the masturbatory activities have done measurable damage to 
any of these individuals, with the very rare exception of the psychotic who 
is compulsive in his behavior. On the other hand, the record does include 
thousands of cases of boys living in continual conflict, fearful of social dis- 
grace, oftentimes disturbed over the effect of such behavior on their ulti- 
mate sexual capacities, occasionally attempting suicide — as a result of the 
teachings concerning masturbation. For the boys who have not been too 
disturbed psychically, masturbation has, however, provided a regular 
sexual outlet which has alleviated nervous tensions; and the record is clear 
in many cases that these boys have on the whole lived more balanced lives 
than the boys who have been more restrained in their sexual activities 
(Chapter 6). The resolution adopted at an American Medical Association 
convention in 1917 asserting that there is no evidence that abstinence from 
sex activity is “inconsistent with the highest physical, mental and moral 
efficiency” would be questioned by most clinical psychologists and psychia- 
trists today, and is definitely contrary to the findings in the present study 
(Chapter 6). 

The scientific judgments are, however, not fully accepted by the persons 
who have been most interested in sex education (Bigelow 1916, Eddy 1928a, 
1928b, Elliott and Bone 1929, Amer. Soc. Hyg. Assoc. 1930, Dickerson 
1930, 1933, Rice 1933, Strain 1934, Ellis 1936, Snow 1937, Henry 1938, 
Rosanoff 1938, Stone and Stone 1937, Eaton and Bafiey 1940, Lovell 1940, 
Gruenberg and Kaukonen 1940, Corner and Landis 1941, Boys Club 
Amer. 1946, Hyman 1946, Landis and BoUes 1946, Thornton 1946, 
Popenoe 1946). 

In this literature it has become customary to admit that the earlier teach- 
ings greatly exaggerated the possible harms of masturbation; but the con- 



MASTURBATION 


515 


elusion is nevertheless reached that no manly youth will want to accept 
such a habit as part of his lifelong pattern. The boy is advised that a lim- 
ited amount of masturbation may do him no harm, but that in excess it is 
something which needs the attention of a physician. Since the point at 
which excess begins is never defined, the conscientious boy is left uncertain 
whether his own rate is going to harm him; and psychiatrists will quickly 
recognize that such subtle and indirect condemnation can do as much 
damage to the boy’s personality as the more extreme teaching of the older 
day. One needs to be reminded again (as in Chapter 6) that there is tre- 
mendous individual variation in the human male’s capacity to engage in 
sexual activity without undue fatigue or other physical harm. Some in- 
dividuals reach their limits when they experience orgasm once in a week 
or two. The average adolescent boy is quite capable of three or four ejacu- 
lations per week, and there are boys who are capable of seven to fourteen or 
more per week without incurring any greater disturbance than that which 
accompanies the infrequent activities of less capable males. Like many 
other physiologic functions, erotic response depends upon a remarkably 
foolproof mechanism. When one reaches the limit of physiologic endurance 
he no longer responds erotically. He is no longer capable of erection and 
finds little incentive to force the situation. Once or twice in a lifetime a few 
of the males may try to establish a record of repeated orgasms, and ex- 
treme fatigue and even some local pain may -result; but, except by a few 
psychotics, this type of performance is not likely to be repeated. 

Many of the persons who are responsible for the compromised attitudes 
found in the sex literature cited above are physicians. Even psychiatrists 
are divided on this question. In general, those who were raised in Europe, 
as were many of the psychiatrists who are now in this country, consider 
masturbation an infantile substitute for heterosexual coitus, which latter 
activity they take to be synonymous with a good sexual adjustment. Often 
these clinicians are amazed to find masturbation persisting into the adult 
Uves of American males, and look upon masturbation in the history of the 
married male as nothing short 'of pathologic. This is, of course, merely a 
rationalization of their own European mores. American psychiatrists, on 
the contrary, with their American backgrounds, are much more acceptant 
of the same activity. 

Although masturbation may do no physical harm, and although it may 
do no mental harm unless psychic conflicts are involved, it still remains to 
be determined what relations there may be between masturbation and 
socio-sexual adjustments (Henry 1938). It is now clear that masturbation 
is relied upon by the upper level primarily because it has an insufficient 
outlet through heterosexual coitus. Thi$ is, to a degree, an escape from re- 
ality, and the effect upon the ultimate personality of the individual is some- 
thing that needs consideration. It is to be noted again (Chapter 1 1) that at 



516 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


age fifty-five the college-bred males derive only 62 per cent of their total 
outlet from marital intercourse, and that 19 per cent of the outlet at that 
age is derived from the dream world which accompanies masturbation or 
nocturnal emissions. Any final assay of the significance of masturbation 
should take these and still other specific data into account. 



Chapter 15 

NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 

It is possible that the first sexual responses of an infant or younger pre- 
adolescent could be evoked by physical stimulation alone; but the human 
animal is always conditioned by its experiences, and its reactions may come 
to depend as much upon the previous experience as upon any immediate 
stimuli. The evidence accumulates that the physical is usually a minor ele- 
ment in evoking sexual responses among older males, and there are few of 
the responses of an experienced adult which would be possible without a 
sufficient psychologic accompaniment. 

Time and again a male may fail to respond to particular physical con- 
tacts, while responding almost instantly to more minor stimulation which 
comes under other circumstances (Vecki 1920, Haire 1937, Lovell 1940, 
Weiss and English 1943). His responses in the heterosexual may be imme- 
diate, while he experiences a minimum of arousal, or none at all, when sub- 
jected to identical techniques in contact with another male. The next male’s 
responses, on the contrary, may be immediate in the homosexual, and com- 
pletely fail in the heterosexual. Some males are impotent when they attempt 
extra-marital intercourse, although they may be perfectly potent with their 
own wives. Other males may become impotent with their wives and capable 
of performing only with extra-marital partners. There are a few males who 
are impotent when they attempt to masturbate, although they are potent 
enough under other circumstances. There are males who are potent and re- 
spond to the point of orgasm in petting, although they block and become 
incapable of performing when they attempt actual coitus. Such differential 
impotency emphasizes the importance of the psychic element in sexual ac- 
tivities. 

Except for inexperienced children, most males come to erection if there 
is any considerable arousal, even before they have made physical contacts. 
The exceptions include those males who have had such an abundance of 
sexual activity (as among certain lower level groups) that they are psycho- 
logically satisfied or even fatigued; and among upper level groups, where 
psychic stimulation means most to the individual, there are some males who 
do not erect in anticipation of a sexual situation because they are inhibited 
by moral or social training. This further emphasizes the importance of the 
psychic factor in a sexual relation. 

Three or four adult males, out of the more than 5000 in the present study, 
have been able to ejaculate by deliberately concentrating on sexual fan- 

517 



518 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


tasies, without any genital manipulation. In such a case the psychic stimu- 
lation is entirely responsible for the result. Spontaneous ejaculation occurs 
most often among young adolescent boys. A list of situations which bring 
spontaneous ejaculation in the younger male has already been given 
(Chapter 5). Some of the climaces reached in heterosexual petting may 
amount to spontaneous ejaculation without genital stimulation. Orgasm 
from purely psychic sources may occur more often in the female. 

Psychic stimulation during sleep is a more familiar phenomenon. It re- 
sults in orgasm much more often than does psychic stimulation during 
waking hours. This is probably due to the fact that one is not so inhibited 
during sleep. Orgasm as the product of nocturnal dreams is well known in 
the male, but it is not so generally understood that similar orgasm during 
sleep is not uncommon in the female, especially in the older and sexually 
more experienced female (Ellis 1936). In the male, nocturnal emissions or 
wet dreams are generally accepted as a usual part of the sexual picture. 

There are many nocturnal sex dreams which do not result in orgasm for 
the male. There are some males who may have sex dreams with considerable 
frequency, even every night, without ever experiencing orgasm, unless it be 
in their early years. On the other hand, there are some who regularly have 
nocturnal emissions but are unable to recall that such experiences were ever 
accompanied by dreams. If the absence of the dreams could be absolutely 
established in these cases, they would be perfect instances of orgasm from 
physical or physiologic stimulation alone. Most psychologists and psychi- 
atrists, however, hesitate to believe that emissions ever result from internal 
forces which do not have psychosexual backgrounds. Generally such emis- 
sions are considered to be products of dreams that are forgotten by their 
author, and we incline to this interpretation. Proving that an emission 
could occur without a psychic accompaniment would, however, be of such 
great importance that a series of experiments should be devised for testing 
these unusual individuals. 

Throughout the present volume, nocturnal dreams have been recorded 
as outlets only when they have led to actual ejaculation; but all ejaculation 
during sleep has been recorded, whether reported with or without dreams. 

REFERENCES 

Specific data on the incidences and frequencies of nocturnal emissions, 
in various age groups and in various social divisions of the population, 
have already been given in tables and charts, and in earlier discussions in 
this volume, as follows : 


PAGE 

TABLE 

HGURE 

NATURE OF DATA 

190-191 

38 

30 

Sources of first ejaculation 

234 

49 

139-140 

Range of variation and age 

243-245 

52 

59-64 

Age and nocturnal emissions 



NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 


519 


PAGE 

TABLE 

FIGURE 

NATURE OF DATA 

277 

62 

59-64 

Marital status and nocturnal emissions 

300-303 

68 

89 

Sources of first ejaculation and age at on- 
set of adolescence 

315,319 

79 


Lack of correlation between nocturnal 
emissions and age at onset of adoles- 
cence 

342-345 

83, 109, 115 

99, 139 

Social level and nocturnal emissions 

376-382 

96-97 

106-107 

Nocturnal emissions in patterns of sexual 
behavior 

396-399 

407-413 

98, 103-105 

109, 120 

Older and younger generations and noc- 
turnal emissions 

452-453, 

462 

118 

125 

Rural and urban groups and nocturnal 
emissions 

474-477 

127 


Religious backgrounds and nocturnal 
emissions 

488-493 


126, 128-133 

Significance of nocturnal emissions as one 
source of total outlet 

519-525 

4, 18, 22, 133 

9,17, 22,138-139 

Accumulative incidence of nocturnal 
emissions 

519-525 


140 

Individual variation in nocturnal emis- 
sions 


INCIDENCES AND FREQUENCIES 

A high percentage of all males experience nocturnal emissions at some 
time in their lives. Ultimately, about 83 per cent is involved (Table 133, 
Figure 138). There is 17 per cent of the male population that never seems 
to have nocturnal emissions. Somewhat similar data have been reported by 
some other investigators (Achilles 1923, Peck and Wells 1923, 1925, 
Hughes 1926, Hamilton 1929, Willoughby 1937). The figures differ con- 
siderably for different social levels, the highest incidence being among those 
males who go to college and the lowest among males of the grade school 
level. 

Over 99 per cent of the males who go to college have nocturnal emis- 
sions at some time in their lives, but only 85 per cent of the high school 
males, and only 75 per cent of the males who never go beyond grade school 
(Table 133, Figure 139). The high incidence figures given in some of the 
previous studies (e.g., Peterson 1938) apply, obviously, only to the college 
populations on which the data were based. 

Nocturnal emissions never account for any large portion of the total 
number of orgasms experienced by the male population (Table 52, Fig- 
ures 60, 63). For instance, the single males of the college level who are in 
their twenties, derive about one-sixth (12% to 16%) of their total outlet 
from this source. For the high school level it is about 8 per cent, but at the 
grade school level less than 6 per cent of the outlet is so derived. Among 
married males, emissions account for something between 2 per cent and 6 
per cent of the total outlet. 



520 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Nocturnal Emissions: Accumulative Incidence 


AGE 

1 

TOTAL 

POPULATION 

U. S. 

CORRECTIONS 

EDUC. LEVEL 

0-8 

i 

EDUC. 

9- 

LEVEL 

•12 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13+ 


Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

8 

3986 

0.1 

681 

0.0 

494 

0.2 

2811 

0.0 

9 

3986 

0.1 

681 

0.0 

494 

0.2 i 

2811 

0.0 

10 

3986 

0.2 

681 

0.0 

494 

0.2 

2811 

0.5 

11 

3985 

0.8 

680 

0.3 i 

494 

0.4 

2811 

3.2 

12 

3985 

3.3 

680 

1.0 

494 

2.4 

2811 

11.6 

13 

3985 

9.8 

680 

3.7 

494 

8.1 

2811 

29.1 

14 

3982 

25.3 

677 

11.8 

494 

25.7 

2811 

52.2 

15 

3976 

39.6 

671 

25.3 

494 

39.9 

2811 

68.9 

16 

.1955 

54.1 

654 

39.1 

491 

55.6 

2810 

80.7 

17 

3896 

63.1 

617 

47.5 

471 

65.6 

2808 

87.0 

18 

3758 

71.4 

590 

56.9 

436 

74.3 

2732 

91.0 

19 

3527 

73.6 

560 

60.2 

399 

76.2 

2568 

92.6 

20 

3222 

77.0 

532 

65.8 

357 

79.0 

2333 

93.6 

21 

2848 

78.7 

508 

67.5 

313 

80.8 

2027 

94.5 

22 

2446 

80.0 

488 

70.1 

290 

81.7 

1668 

94.7 

23 

2131 

81.8 

472 

70.8 

264 

84.5 

1395 

94.8 

24 

1839 

81.5 

452 

71.9 

237 

83.1 

1150 

95.7 

25 

1654 

81.5 

432 

73.1 

221 

83.7 

1001 

96.2 

26 

1510 

81.6 

420 

73.8 

207 

83.1 

883 

96.5 

27 

1374 

81.9 

405 

73.6 

196 

84.2 

773 

96.2 

28 

1268 

82.9 

391 

74.2 

179 

85.5 

698 

96.6 

29 

1159 

83.2 

367 

74.9 

159 

85.5 

633 

96.8 

30 

1064 

82.8 

351 

76.1 ‘ 

141 

85.1 

572 i 

97.2 

31 

988 

82.7 

331 

75.5 

129 

85.3 

528 

97.3 

32 

929 

82.9 

319 

76.2 

119 

84.9 

491 

97.6 

33 

868 

82.5 

305 

75.7 

116 

84.5 

447 

97.5 

34 

814 

82.6 

296 

75.3 

107 

85.0 

411 

98.1 

35 

757 

81.2 

282 

74.8 

94 

85.1 

381 

98.4 

36 

713 

81.0 

269 

74.3 

89 

85.4 

355 

98.6 

37 

651 

80.6 

251 

74.1 

78 

84.6 

1 322 

98.4 

38 

620 

81.1 

242 

74.8 

72 

84.7 

306 

98.7 

39 

565 

80.3 

220 

74.1 

66 

83.3 

, 279 

98.6 

40 

516 

78.8 

200 

73.5 

i 60 

81.7 ' 

! 256 

1 99.2 

41 

480 

78.0 

188 

72.9 

55 

80.0 

237 

99.6 

42 

452 

77.2 

180 

72.2 

; 52 

i 78.8 

220 

99.5 

43 

405 

77.0 

! 164 

71.3 

I 50 

80.0 

191 

99.5 


Table 133. Accumulative incidence data on nocturnal emissions 

_ Covering the life span, including both single and married histories. In three educa- 
tional levels, and in the total population corrected for the U, S. Census of 1940. 



NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 


521 


As might be expected, there is considerable variation among individuals 
in the frequencies with which they have nocturnal emissions (Figure 140). 
There are males who never ejaculate in their sleep, and more males who 
have only a few wet dreams in their lives. There are also some who experi- 
ence orgasm practically every time they awaken from sleep, even though 
that may be two or three times in a single night. Sometimes emissions may 
accompany daytime naps. Among men who have been suddenly deprived 
of some drug to which they have been addicted, emissions may occur sev- 
eral times in each twenty-four hours, for two or three weeks or more. For 
most males during their earlier years, nocturnal emissions are usually 
monthly or bi-monthly, rarely weekly or more than weekly events. 

The frequencies of nocturnal emissions are, as might have been antici- 
pated, highest among males of the college level (Table 83, Figure 99). They 
occur less frequently among the males who never go beyond high school, 
and even less often among the males who never go beyond grade school 
(Chapter 10). In their younger adolescent years, the boys who will go to 
college have 7 times as frequent dreams as the boys who never go beyond 
grade school. In the later teens the differences are nearly 3 to 1 in favor of 
the college males. They are almost 2 to 1 among the males who are still 
single at 30 years of age. The frequencies of nocturnal dreams of any sort, 
sexual or otherwise, appear to have some correlation with the imaginative 
capacities of an individual. The sexual life of a male of a lower educational 
level is primarily dependent upon actual physical contacts. He is aroused 
during his waking hours by relatively few psychic stimuli, and he rarely 
utilizes such secondhand sources of stimulation as art, literature, nude pic- 
tures, stories, or specifically pornographic materials to accompany or sub- 
stitute for overt sex acts. At night he probably does less dreaming, of any 
sort, than the better educated male, and his sex dreams are certainly not 
frequent. 

At all social levels, nocturnal emissions occur most abundantly before 
marriage. Ultimately about 85 per cent of the unmarried males are involved. 
After marriage a number of the males have their nocturnal emissions re- 
duced or altogether stopped, and the accumulative incidence figure for the 
married population is something under 60 per cent. At most ages in mar- 
riage the frequencies are only two-thirds as high as they are among un- 
married males. Since the total outlets of married males are much higher 
than those of the single males, nocturnal emissions provide only a small 
part, 2 to 6 per cent, of the outlet after marriage (Chapter 8). Among those 
males who have been previously married but who have been divorced or 
otherwise separated from their wives, nocturnal emissions may begin again, 
even though they were absent during the married years ; but the average 
frequencies in the post-marital group remain at the same low levels as 
among married males. 



522 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


100 



AGE 


Figure 138. Nocturnal emissions: accumulative incidence in total U. S. 

population 


Showing percent of total population that has ever experience^! nocturnal emissions by 
each of the indicated ages. All data based on total population^ irrespective of marital 
status, and corrected for the U. S. Census distribution. 



Figure 139. Nocturnal emissions: accumulative incidence in three educational 

levels 


Showing percent of each population that has ever had nocturnal emissions by 
each of the indicated ages. All data based on total population, irrespective of marital 
status. 


NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 


523 


In all social groups, nocturnal emissions are primarily an outlet of 
younger adolescent and older teen-age boys. It is interesting to find that 
they ordinarily do not begin with the first signs of adolescence (Chapters 
5, 9). Most boys obtain their first orgasms through self stimulation or from 
physical contacts with other individuals, either in late pre-adolescence or 
immediately with the onset of adolescence. It is probable that all boys are 
capable of such orgasm as soon as they turn adolescent, and most of them 
would be capable at least a year or two before the onset of adolescence. 
However, the first orgasms resulting from dreams ordinarily do not come 
until a year or more after the onset of adolescence (Chapter 10). Even in 
those cases where nocturnal emissions provide the very first experiences in 
orgasm, they almost invariably begin a year or more after the other ado- 
lescent developments (pubic hair, voice change, growth in height, etc.) 
are under way. 

These data provide some measure of the relative positions of the physical 
and the psychic in the sex life of the human animal. It is for a similar reason 
that fantasies often do not begin to accompany masturbation until a year 
or more after such self stimulation has begun. Although the psychic may 
play a considerable part in even the earliest sexual experiences of pre- 
adolescent males, and becomes important soon after the onset of ado- 
lescence, it ordinarily does not become very significant until there has been 
a certain amount of physical experience. 

The highest incidence and frequency figures of nocturnal emissions (for 
the population taken as a whole) come in the late teens, during which pe- 
riod fully 70 per cent of the males experience orgasm in sleep (Table 52). 
The frequencies then average about once in four weeks. If the experienced 
males alone are used in the calculations, the highest frequencies of noc- 
turnal emissions occur between adolescence and fifteen, at rates of about 
once in three weeks. From that point, both the incidence and frequency 
figures go down, and the dreams become few after age thirty and are largely 
out of the picture after age forty. Beyond fifty years of age nocturnal emis- 
sions rarely occur more often than four or five times a year, if they occur 
at all. There are, however, a few cases of nocturnal emissions occurring 
among still older males, even between the ages of seventy-six and eighty. 

Among boys of the high school and college levels, the emissions be- 
tween adolescence and age fifteen occur most frequently among those who 
became adolescent before eleven or twelve. On the other hand, they provide 
a higher percentage of the total outlet for those boys who reach adoles- 
cence last, and some of these males depend largely or exclusively on 
nocturnal emissions for their total sexual outlet. The early-adolescent boys 
more frequently depend on masturbation, heterosexual coitus, and the 
homosexual. 



524 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


%rl 0=40 4% 

AGE GROUP • ADOL- 15 50 

EDUC LEVEL 0-5 - \ 



-t.O -L5 -^.0 -Z5 -30 0 -0.5 -LO 

% ^ 

AGE GROUP -AOOL- 15 r \ 

EOUC LEVEL 9-12 iqL \ 


•1.0 H.5 -2 0 -2.5 -3,0 0 -0.5 -1.0 

A 

A 

age GROUP >AD0L -IS rrx / \ 

EOUC LEVEL 15+ / * \ 


-1.0 -1.S -2.0 -2.5 -3 0 0 -0.5 -1.0 

AGE GROUP- 16-20 30 h X 

EDUC LEVEL 0-8 r 


-1.0 “1,5 -20 -L5 -3.0 0 -0.5 -1.0 




AGE GROUP- 16-20 
EDUC LEVEL S-12 


AGE GROUP 21-25 
EOUC LEVEL 0-8 


-1.5 -2.0 -2.5 -3.0 


age GROUP 21-25 
EDUC LEVEL 9-12 


“1.5 “2.0 -2.5 


AGE GROUP- 21-25 
EDUC LEVEL 13 + 


“1.5 “2.0 -25 -• 


AGE GROUP -26-30 
EDUC LEVEL 0-8 


-15 “2 0 -2.5 


AGE GROUP- 26-30 
EDUC LEVEL 9-12 



AGE GROUP. 26-30 
EDUC LEVEL 13 + 


Figure 140. Nocturnal emissions: individual variation in frequencies, in four 
age groups, at three educational levels 

Showing percent of each population (vertical line) which has nocturnal emissions with 
each type of frequency (horizontal line). 






NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 


525 


In younger generations of the social level that goes to college, the num- 
ber of males having nocturnal emissions is almost identical with the number 
involved (in the corresponding social level) twenty-two years ago (Chapter 
11). On the other hand, in the grade school groups, the younger genera- 
tions appear to have emissions at an earlier age, and more of them are 
ultimately involved than was true in older generations. At all levels, how- 
ever, the frequencies today are almost precisely what they were twenty- 
two years ago. These data are important to note, even if their explanation 
is not immediately apparent. 

Nocturnal emissions occur with about the same incidences and fre- 
quencies among rural and urban males (Chapter 12). 

The incidences and frequencies of nocturnal emissions are almost ex- 
actly the same among the active and inactive members of each religious 
group (Chapter 13). 

Among the several sources of sexual outlet, there is none that is less 
significant than nocturnal emissions, unless it be intercourse with animals 
of other species. Intercourse with prostitutes and the homosexual both 
contribute more to the total sexual picture. There are some males who draw 
the whole of their sexual outlet from nocturnal emissions, and this is true 
of 8 per cent of the younger, adolescent boys, but it is not true of more than 
5 per cent of the males at any later age. It is, in consequence, interesting to 
find so much attention given to the significance of nocturnal emissions in 
certain literature. This undoubtedly reflects a wish that involuntary emis- 
sions were a more important part of the pre-marital outlet. 

On the whole, the males who are most dependent upon nocturnal emis- 
sions are those who are slow in developing physically, those who are slow 
in their nervous reactions or unresponsive to the usual sexual stimuli, or 
those who are timid and awkward in making social contacts. They are the 
males who are most often restrained for moral reasons. There are some out- 
standing exceptions to this, proving that a multiplicity of factors may be in- 
volved in determining the frequencies of nocturnal emissions; but, by and 
large, emissions are most often depended upon by the male who has not 
made what the psychiatrist would call a good socio-sexual adjustment. 

CONTENT OF NOCTURNAL SEX DREAMS 

Considering the importance which Freud (1938, 1945) and others have 
attached to the interpretation of dreams, and considering the considerable 
literature which has in consequence developed on this subject (e.g., MoU 
1899, Ellis 1936, Weiss and English 1943, Meyer in Lorand 1944), we will 
attempt to add nothing at this time except certain factual data that have 
been accumulated in the course of the present investigation. 



526 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


The parallel between the content of the nocturnal dream and one’s overt 
daytime experience has been recognized by all peoples, primitive and civ- 
ilized, since the dawn of history. The present study confirms the usual in- 
terpretations, although it has nothing to contribute on the question of 
symbolism in dreams. The dream is usually a reflection of the individual’s 
overt experience or of his desire for experience. It often involves other per- 
sons, usually persons who are obscure and unidentifiable. Sometimes the 
actors are engaged in non-sexual daytime activities, more often they are 
about to make sexual contacts, or to engage in actual coitus or other re- 
lations. Sometimes the dreamer is a participant in the activities, and some- 
times the dreamer does not participate but merely observes. The dream 
situations are most often heterosexual when the overt experience or day- 
time reactions of the individual are heterosexual, and the dreams are most 
often homosexual when the overt experience of the individual is homo- 
sexual. Persons who have both things in their histories have dreams that 
are sometimes homosexual, sometimes heterosexual, and sometimes both 
homosexual and heterosexual in the same event. In such cases, the pre- 
dominance of heterosexual or homosexual dreams may reflect the indi- 
vidual’s preference for one or the other sort of experience, but this is not 
always so. 

A number of males dream of females who have male genitalia, and this is 
particularly interesting in the light of the fact that most of these males have 
not heard of the classic Greek concept of the hermaphrodite (Licht 1925- 
1928), nor are they acquainted with the psychoanalytic treatments of such 
combinations of male and female characters. Sometimes dreams of her- 
maphrodites occur among males who have had neither heterosexual nor 
homosexual experience, and we are inclined to interpret them as primarily 
heterosexual (as also in Nacke 1908, Ellis 1936). The maleness of the gen- 
italia in the dream may depend upon the fact that an individual who has 
not actually seen female genitalia may have some difficulty in imagining an 
anatomy or a genital performance which is different from that which he 
has experienced in his own person. 

There are a few males who dream of masturbation, but this is not com- 
mon. Boys who have had animal contacts, or thought about having them, 
quite regularly dream of such experiences. There are occasional sadistic or 
masochistic dreams which may reflect some phase of the thinking of the 
individual, or of his actual experience. 

Finally, it must be emphasized that there are some dreams which simply 
do not correlate with any overt experience. Such dreams are not frequent, 
but they do seem to occur, for they are sometimes reported under circum- 
stances which make one feel that the record is thoroughly reliable. For in- 
stance, a male who reports an extensive homosexual history would appear 
to have little reason for distorting the fact when he says that all of his 



NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 


527 


dreams are heterosexual. He may emphasize that he wishes they were not 
so, because he has no use for the heterosexual, and would enjoy the ex- 
perience of having homosexual dreams. Explanations of such contra- 
dictory dreams should be based on more detail than anyone seems yet to 
have obtained. It is difficult to believe that suppressed desires are always 
involved. It is not at all impossible that familiarity with the experiences of 
other persons would be sufficient to generate a dream, even though it in- 
cluded events which were totally distasteful to the individual and in which 
he had no desire to participate. 

Often the actual experience of orgasm is not realized in a dream. Even 
when the subject wakes to find himself ejaculating, he may not have reached 
the fulfillment of his activity in the dream itself. Most individuals wake up 
when there is an orgasm, but there are some who continue to sleep through 
it. Even in those cases, however, the dream may not include any realiza- 
tion of the activity which produced the orgasm. 

RELATION TO OTHER OUTLETS 

By nearly all moral philosophies, nocturnal emissions provide the one 
form of sexual outlet for which the individual is least responsible. Mas- 
turbation, heterosexual petting or coitus, and homosexual and animal con- 
tacts may all be penalized as the product of the individuaFs deliberate in- 
tent and conscious performance. There is some contention that one should 
control the frequencies of his emissions by controlhng his thoughts before 
going to sleep (e.g,, W. S. Hall 1920, Lieber 1920, Amer. Soc. Hyg. Assoc. 
1930, Kirsch 1930. See Ellis 1936, Vol. 1 (1):188 ff. for a history of the 
Church’s attitudes). It is suggested that dreams may be controlled by regu- 
lating the position in which one sleeps, or by choosing one’s night clothing 
(e.g,, W. S. Hall 1907, Jefferis and Nichols 1921, Exner 1932, Kirkendall 
1940). But the condemnation of nocturnal emissions has not been great. 
They have been looked upon as involuntary and spontaneous releases of 
pressure, and as a means of avoiding other sexual activities. Since there are 
several biologic problems involved in these interpretations, a scientist is 
justified in asking for objective data on the physiology of the several proc- 
esses concerned, and on the relation between nocturnal emissions and the 
frequencies of other sexual outlets. 

On the physiologic origin of these emissions, the information is ex- 
ceedingly scant, and investigations are much needed. Certainly no inter- 
pretation is tenable which depends upon the idea that the testes are the 
sources of the semen, and that they or other glands become so engorged 
with accumulating secretions that involuntary ejaculation is the result. The 
semen is chiefly composed of secretions from the seminal vesicles and the 
prostate gland, and the testes contribute nothing but sperm which, of 
course, constitute only the mo^ minute part of the semen (e.g,, Hotchkiss 


18 



528 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


1944). There is no evidence that the testes have any part in effecting erotic 
arousal or ejaculation, except indirectly as they supply hormones which in- 
fluence the male metabolism. If there are any pressures involved, they must 
arise in the seminal vesicles or in the prostate gland; but data on this point 
are lacking. It is more tenable to think of nervous tensions which are built 
up until, periodically, they precipitate an orgasm; but again the physiology 
is not understood. We are, in consequence, almost completely in the dark 
as to the possibility of a biologic mechanism which could force nocturnal 
emissions when other sexual outlets were insufflcient. 

Neither are there objective data on the mechanical factors that may effect 
nocturnal emissions. It has been known for some time that the nerve centers 
that control ejaculation are in the lower spinal cord, and this has lent some 
substantiation to the theory that sleeping on one’s back, especially if there 
are pressures on the lower spine, may increase the frequencies of emissions; 
but there seem to be no sound data to substantiate this. Genital stimulation 
from tight clothing, more general stimulation from too warm a bed, or still 
other conditions may have some effect; but such relationships are not yet 
scientifically established. 

It has frequently been said that persons who are celibate, or at least ab- 
stinent before marriage, will find an increased outlet through involuntary 
emissions, and that the emissions will provide a sufiicient release to keep 
an individual physically and mentally balanced (e.g., W. S. Hall 1907, 1909, 
Eddy 1928a, 1928b, Elliott and Bone 1929, Ruland and Rattler 1934, 
Frank 1946). It would, in consequence, be of exceeding scientific impor- 
tance to have histories from a sufficient sample of highly restrained indi- 
viduals, particularly of those who are celibate. Without such data it is, of 
course, impossible to depend upon general statements which have been 
made on this point, especially when they come from persons who are in- 
terested in defending moral or social philosophies. 

It can, however, be noted again that the frequencies of emissions among 
the religiously more devout males are neither higher nor lower than the 
frequencies among the non-church-going males in the population (Chapter 
13). The rates of masturbation, pre-marital intercourse, and the homosexual 
are much reduced in the devout group and, in consequence, the total sexual 
outlets are reduced; but this merely increases the percentage of the total 
outlet which is derived from nocturnal emissions. Among these histories of 
the religiously devout, the absolute frequencies of such emissions are not 
altered by the abstinence. 

Lacking a sufficient series of histories of celibates, the best data now 
available on persons largely deprived of other outlets come from men who 
have been confined to penal institutions and who are cut off from their out- 
side sexual activities. Among such men there is a slightly higher frequency 
of nocturnal emissions, but the increases are not great (Plattner 1930). Even 



NOCTURNAL EMISSIONS 


529 


though there are some men who have their first emissions after entering an 
institution, their subsequent experiences rarely occur more often than a few 
times a year or, at most, once in a month or two. 

While it is commonly believed that males in a prison find an abundant 
release through the homosexual, and while it is in actuality a fact that a 
high percentage of them do become involved in such activity after they 
have been in a penal institution for some length of time, neither the homo- 
sexual nor masturbation ever provides any frequent outlet for more than 
a small proportion of a prison population. Many males do not begin their 
homosexual activity for some years after entering an institution. Perhaps 
half of the men in a short-time institution never do arrive at such activity 
during the period of their stay. Consequently for a fair number of the in- 
mates either nocturnal emissions provide the total outlet, or these men have 
none at all. Considering that most prison inmates come from social levels 
where the frequencies of marital intercourse often average six or seven 
times a week, nocturnal emissions at the rate of three to six per year do not 
provide much compensation. 

There are some records of persons who report an increase in the fre- 
quencies of nocturnal emissions when there has been a minimum of other 
activity; but there are some who never have emissions unless they have en- 
gaged in heterosexual petting or unless they have experienced orgasm in 
heterosexual coitus or in homosexual relations during the preceding eve- 
ning. In the latter instance, it would appear that the psychic stimulation re- 
sulting from the overt contacts had been carried over into sleep. If there 
are, in some cases, physiologic mechanisms which produce emissions when 
there is an insufficiency of other outlets, there may be psychologic mech- 
anisms which work in exactly the opposite way. In some individuals the 
physiologic factors may predominate; in others, the psychologic factors 
may be more significant. It is quite probable that in still other cases, still 
other factors are involved. 

There are individuals who have high rates of total outlet and who have 
infrequent nocturnal emissions, or none at all; and there are individuals 
with low rates of total outlet who similarly have infrequent emissions. On 
the other hand, there are individuals of both high and low rates who have 
an abundance of nocturnal emissions. The situation cannot be simply 
summarized. When precise correlations are run they must not only relate 
the emissions with the several other sources of outlet, but must also take in- 
to account the nature of the socio-sexual contacts which each individual is 
making, the significance of his personality, and those psychic capacities 
which might influence his daytime imaginations and his nighttime dreaming. 

Most boys have learned from current opinion and from printed Utera- 
ture that nocturnal emissions are usual and normal sources of sexual out- 
let (as in G. S. Hall 1904, Bigelow 1916, Liederman 1926, Exner 1932, 



530 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Weatherhead 1932, Rice 1933, Dickerson 1933, Ellis 1936, Bruckner 1937, 
U. S. Public Health Service 1937, Crisp 1939, Klemer 1939, Gruenberg and 
Kaukonen 1940, Lovell 1940, Corner and Landis 1941, Kelly 1941, Sadler 
and Sadler 1944, Boys Club Amer. 1946). Males of an older generation 
more often worried over the emissions, and an occasional boy may still 
become disturbed over the question of the frequency of his experience. 
Writers who advise one who has “frequent” emissions to consult his phys- 
ician or his confessor, do not help in allaying the boy’s fears. Authorities in 
some schools, especially in some religious schools and in some penal insti- 
tutions, may reprimand the individual who “allows” emissions to occur, 
and there are records of at least two disciplinary schools where the boys 
were punished for having nocturnal emissions. 

There are some individuals who report some feeling of lassitude after 
experiencing a nocturnal emission, but this is a possible outcome of any type 
of sexual activity. It is the more usual experience that such emissions do not 
have even this after-effect. As a matter of fact, nocturnal emissions are ac- 
cepted by most of the boys of the present generation as a usual part of male 
experience. 



Chapter 16 


HETEROSEXUAL PETTING 

During the past few decades, particularly at upper social levels, pre- 
marital physical contacts between males and females have been consider- 
ably elaborated without any increase in the frequency of actual intercourse 
(Chapter 11). These contacts may go far beyond the hugging and kissing 
which occurred in older generations. In their maximum extensions they 
may involve all of the techniques of the pre-coital play in which sophisti- 
cated married partners engage. 

In general this behavior is known to the younger generation as petting, 
although other terms are applied to certain types of contacts. Those which 
are confined to latitudes not lower than the neck are sometimes known as 
necking, and petting is distinguished from the heavy petting which involves 
a deliberate stimulation of the female breast, or of the male or female gen- 
itaha. While most of the younger generation of high school and college- 
bred males and females more or less accepts petting as usual and proper in 
pre-marital behavior, some of those who have doubts about the morahty 
of their activities ease their consciences by avoiding the term petting for 
anything except the more extended forms of contact. 

In the present volume the term “petting” has been apphed to any sort of 
physical contact which does not involve a union of genitalia but in which 
there is a dehberate attempt to effect erotic arousal. Accidental touching is 
not petting, even though it may bring an erotic response. Simple hp kissing 
may or may not be petting, depending on the intent and earnestness of the 
procedure. Petting is not always effective in achieving an arousal, but if 
there has been a dehberate attempt it satisfies the definition. Soul kissing, 
smooching, necking, mild petting, and heavy petting are basically one 
thing, even though there may be differences in the limits to which the tech- 
niques are carried. The extent of the petting is not necessarily related to the 
degree of arousal. Relatively simple contacts — which in some cases may not 
involve more than a touch or a kiss — may be as effective for certain indi- 
viduals, under certain circumstances, as the most extreme genital manipula- 
tions. If the erotic significance is being considered, a classification of petting 
should be based on the degree of arousal and the success of the activity in 
effecting orgasm, rather than upon the nature of the mechanics employed. 
Obviously, the psychic components are, again (Chapter 15), more im- 
portant than the physical in this sort of sexual activity. 

531 



532 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Until quite recently the deliberate elaboration of petting techniques has 
been confined largely to pre-marital and marital relationships, and all of 
the data which are given in the present chapter apply to the pre-marital 
petting activities of single males. Within much more recent years there has 
been an increasing tendency to accept petting as an extra-marital relation- 
ship among persons who would not think of having extra-marital inter- 
course, and who more or less persuade themselves that they are still faith- 
ful to their spouses if they engage in nothing but petting with other part- 
ners. At many an upper level social affair, at cocktail parties, at dances, 
during automobile rides, after dinner parties, and on other occasions, 
married males may engage in such flirtations and physical contacts with 
other men’s wives, sometimes quite openly and often without being re- 
strained by the presence of the other spouses. Unfortunately, the extent of 
this extra-marital petting was not comprehended in the earlier years of the 
present study, and we have not yet accumulated sufficient material for re- 
porting on this aspect of human sexual behavior. 

REFERENCES 

Specific data on the incidences and frequencies of pre-marital hetero- 
sexual petting have already been given in the following tables and charts, 
and in earlier discussions in this volume, as follows : 


PAGE 

TABLE 

HGURE 

NATURE OF DATA 

174 

29 


Continuity of pre-adolescent sex play 
with adolescent activity 

190-191 

38 

30 

Sources of first ejaculation 

234 

49 

144 

Range of variation and age 

244-249 

277 

53 

38-43 

Age and petting to climax 

Petting as an activity of single males 

300 

68 


Age at onset of adolescence and petting 

345-347 

84,96,110,115 

100 

Social level and petting 

376-381 

93-95 


Attitudes toward petting in various social 
levels 

399,406- 

412 

102, 103, 105 

117-118, 121 

Older and younger generations and pet- 
ting 

453-454, 

462 

119 

125 

Rural and urban populations and petting 

477-478 

128 


Religious backgrounds and petting 

488-493 

511-512 


126, 128-130 

Significance of petting as one source of 
total outlet 

Masturbation and pre-marital petting 

533-535 

134 

141, 142 

Accumulative incidence of total petting 
experience 

533-539 

5, 135 

10, 143 

Accumulative incidence of petting to 
climax 

533-539 


144 

Individual variation in petting to climax 



HETEROSEXUAL PETTING 


533 


INCIDENCES AND FREQUENCIES 

In pre-adolescence there is, in actuality, very little behavior which can 
properly be classified as petting; but with the onset of adolescence the boy 
increasingly realizes the significance of sexual arousal and, with the ex- 
ample set by his older companions, he is likely to begin more specific 
manipulation of the girls with whom he has social contact. Petting pro- 
vides the first ejaculation for only one-third of one per cent of the boys at 
the turn of adolescence, but by the age of 15 a number (8.4%) find a 
specific sexual outlet in such activity. There is a steady increase in the 
amount of petting that is done in the later teens. 

On the exact incidences and frequencies of petting, few data have been 
available (Hamilton 1929, Willoughby 1937, Bromley and Britten 1938, 
Wile 1941, Ramsey 1943, Seward 1946). The histories in the present study 
show that about 88 per cent of the total male population has engaged in 
some sort of petting, or (in the case of the younger males) will engage in at 
least some petting prior to marriage (Table 134, Figure 141). The activity 
is extensive enough to result in orgasm for the males in over a quarter 
(28%) of this population prior to the time of marriage. However, since 
petting is more extensive in the present generation than it was in the older 
generation, the more significant accumulative incidence calculations on 
males of the college level show over 85 per cent of the younger groups with 
some petting experience, and something over 50 per cent of the younger 
males petting to climax prior to marriage (Table 135, Figure 143). 

Something between 18 and 32 per cent of the younger population may be 
involved in petting which leads to climax in each age period prior to mar- 
riage (Table 53, Figure 40). The highest active incidence occurs between 
the ages of 16 and 20. At that time, about a third of the males are securing 
a portion of their outlet through petting. 

Among those who engage in petting, the frequencies vary considerably 
(Figure 144), There are males who have petting experiences practically 
every night in the week and on other occasions in the day. The most ac- 
tive male in the series averaged orgasm about 7 times per week throughout 
the five-year period between 21 and 25. There are, of course, males who go 
for weeks and months without dating girls with whom they may engage in 
petting, and there are some males who may have lapses of a year or two or 
more between such dates. There are some males who have petting experi- 
ence with dozens or even hundreds of girls prior to marriage, and there are 
a few males who have such experience with only two or three girls, and 
sometimes with no more than the one whom they subsequently marry. 
These are tremendous differences to be found in any sort of behavior; and 
since the social significance of petting is much greater than the social sig- 
nificance of masturbation or of some other types of sexual activity, one 



534 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Total Petting Experience: Accumulative Incidence 


AGE 

TOTAL 

POPULATION 

U. S. 

CORRECTIONS 

EDUC. LEVEL 
0-8 

EDUC. 

9- 

LEVEL 

■12 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13+ 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

3989 

0.0 

682 

0.0 

493 

0.0 

2814 

0.0 

9 

3989 

0.0 

682 

0.0 

493 

0.0 

2814 

0.0 

10 

3989 

' 0.2 

682 

0.1 

493 

0.2 

2814 

0.3 

11 

3988 

1.8 

681 

1.2 

493 

2.2 

2814 

1.4 

12 

3988 

7.7 

681 

6.9 

493 

8.7 

2814 

5.8 

13 

3987 

21.4 

680 

18.7 

493 

24.1 

2814 

17.0 

14 

3984 

43.2 

677 

35.5 

493 

50.0 

2814 

33.7 

15 

3977 

57.0 

670 

51.2 

493 

62.9 

2814 

47.2 

16 

3952 

71.3 

651 

64.5 

488 

77.0 

2813 

64.2 

17 

3882 

79.1 

607 

71.7 

466 

84.3 

2809 

75.3 

18 

3704 

83.7 

559 

77.5 

414 

87.4 

2731 

82.9 

19 

3413 

85.9 

500 

79.6 

351 

88.9 

2562 

87.7 

20 

3026 

87.0 

437 

80.5 

284 

89.8 

2305 

89.9 

21 

2592 

87,9 

384 

81.8 

231 

90.5 i 

1977 

90.6 

22 

2074 

88.4 

309 

81.6 

186 

91.4 ^ 

1579 

91.1 

23 

1658 

89.0 

256 

80.9 

155 

92.9 

1247 

91.2 

24 

1313 

88.6 

223 

82.5 1 

125 

91.2 

965 

91.7 

25 

1057 

88.7 

188 

83.5 1 

110 

91.8 

759 

92.4 

26 

857 

88.5 

164 

82.3 

92 

92.4 

601 

92.5 

27 

659 

88.3 

149 

81.9 

80 

92.5 

430 

91.2 

28 

528 

90.3 

131 

82.4 

69 

97.1 

328 

89.3 

29 

432 

89.8 

113 

81.4 

60 

96.7 

259 

89.6 

30 

351 

88.5 

100 

82.0 

50 

96.0 

201 

89.1 

31 

282 

88.0 

83 

81.9 



155 

87.1 

32 

242 

86.8 

77 

80.5 



127 

85.0 

33 

212 

86.4 

68 

77.9 



110 

84.5 

34 

180 

86.1 

64 

78.1 



87 

82.8 

35 

155 

84.9 

59 

79.7 



73 

80.8 

36 

140 

85.0 

58 

81.0 



63 

77.8 

37 

121 

84.7 

52 

80.8 



53 

79.2 

38 

114 

86.2 

50 

84.0 



51 

80.4 


Table 134. Accumulative incidence data on total petting experience 

Covering pre-marital activity, whether with or without orgasm. In three educational 
levels and in the total population corrected for the U. S. Census of 1940. 


HETEROSEXUAL PETTING 


535 



Figure 141. Pre-marital petting experience (of any kind), and petting to cHmax: 
accumulative incidence among single males 

Showing percent of the single males that has ever had petting experience by each of 
the indicated ages. Based on pre-marital histories of population corrected for U. S. 
Census distribution. 



Figure 142. Pre-marital petting experience of any kind: accumulative incidence 
in three educational levels * 

Showing percent of each population that has had any kind of petting experience by 
each of the indicated ages. All data based on unmarried males. 





536 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Petting to Climax: Accumulative Incidence 


AGE 

TOTAL 

POPULATION 

u. s. 

CORRECTIONS 

EDUC. LEVEL 
0-8 

EDUC. 

9- 

LEVEL 

-12 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13+ 


Cases 

%with 

Cases 

%with 

Cases 

% with 

Cases 

% with 



Exper. 


Exper. 


Exper. 


Exper. 

8 

2304 

0.0 

419 

0,0 

289 

0.0 

1596 

0.0 

9 

2304 

0.0 

419 

0.0 

289 

0.0 

1596 

0.0 

10 

2304 

0.0 

419 

0.0 

289 

0.0 

1596 

0.0 

11 

2304 

0.2 

419 

0.2 

289 

0.3 

1596 

0.0 

12 

2304 

1.0 

419 

1.4 

289 

1.0 

1596 

0.3 

13 

2303 

2.0 

418 

2.6 

289 

1.7 

1596 

1.6 

14 

2301 

5.1 

416 

5.3 

289 

5.2 

1596 

4.1 

15 

2295 

8.4 

410 

8.8 

289 

8.3 

1596 

7.6 

16 

2271 

12.8 

392 

10.5 

283 

13.4 

1596 

15.5 

17 

2213 

17,9 

356 

12.1 

264 

19.7 

1593 

23.0 

18 

2097 

21.3 

322 

14.6 

241 

22.4 

1534 

31.2 

19 

1969 

22.6 

285 

14.7 

210 

22.9 

1474 

38.4 

20 

1816 

23.6 

249 

14.1 

178 

23.0 

1389 

46.0 

21 

1616 

24.0 

220 

12.3 

156 

23.7 

1240 

50.1 

22 

1347 

23.7 

174 

11.5 

126 

23.0 

1047 

52.8 

23 

1123 

23.1 

139 

11.5 

112 

21.4 

872 

54.6 

24 

903 

25.8 

121 

14.0 

94 

24.5 

688 

56.3 

25 

729 

27.9 

103 

16.5 

83 

27.7 

543 

58.2 

26 

595 

28.5 

84 

13.1 

74 

31.1 

437 

60.6 

27 

465 

30.8 

75 

16.0 

69 

34.8 

321 

56.7 

28 

382 

31.1 

66 

15.2 

61 

36.1 

255 

56.5 

29 

315 

29.9 

59 

15.3 

52 

32.7 

204 

59.3 

30 

254 

26.9 

55 

14.5 



161 

61.5 

31 

206 

25.6 





122 

60.7 

32 

178 

23.1 





100 

57.0 

33 

154 

23.4 





89 

56.2 

34 

132 

24.9 





72 

55.6 

35 

115 

20.3 





61 

55.7 

36 

105 

19.4 





54 

51.9 


Table 135. Accumulative incidence data on petting to climax 


Covering pre-marital petting experience which leads to orgasm without coitus. In 
three educational levels, and in the total population corrected for the U. S. Census of 



HETEROSEXUAL PETTING 537 

who attempts to understand human society must allow for wide variations 
between males on this point. 

Petting is pre-eminently an activity of youth of the high school and col- 
lege levels. In both of these groups about 92 per cent of all the males are 
involved before marriage (Table 135, Figure 142), and the figure is still 
higher for the younger generations. It is only a slightly smaller number 
(84%) of the males of the grade school level which has experience of the 
sort, but this level is restricted in the nature of its activity. Such petting as 
does occur in the grade school group is often incidental, confined to a few 
minutes of hugging and kissing prior to actual coitus, and quite without 
the elaborations which are usual among college students. Petting at upper 
social levels may be indefinitely prolonged, even into hours of intensive 



Figure 143. Pre-marital petting to climax: accumulative incidence in three 

educational levels 

Showing percent of each population that has ever engaged in pre-marital petting to 
the point of climax, at each of the indicated ages. All data based on unmarried males. 

erotic play, and usually never arrives at coitus. Orgasm as a product of 
petting occurs among 16 per cent of the males of the grade school level, 32 
per cent of the males of the high school level, and over 61 per cent of the 
coUege-bred males who are not married by the age of 30. The social issues 
involved in petting are, therefore, matters that chiefly concern the high 
school and college groups. 

Thirty years ago, petting involved fewer persons and was a less highly 
elaborated activity than it often is today. In regard to most other types of 
sexual activity, the behavior of the older generations (during their youth) 
was so nearly identical with the behavior of the present-day youth that no 
significant differences are shown in statistical analyses of the data obtained 
from the two groups (Chapter 1 1). The records for petting, however, show 



m 


J5 f,* 0*86.7% 

to 

5 


10 

ts 

to 

5 

0 

%i 

20 j 
ts 
10 
5 

% 

15 

10 

5 

0 


0*80.5% 


0*862% 


-OS 


0 - 0.5 
0 = 54.2% 


AGE GROUP-AOOL-IS 
EOUC LEVEL 0-8 


-1.5 


AGE GROUP -ADOL- 15 
EDUC LEVEL 9>I2 


AGE GROUP* ADOL- 15 
EDUC LEVEL 13 + 


-25 


AGE GROUP- 16-20 
EOUC LEVEL 0-8 


0 -O.S 
0 = 65.7% 


- 1.0 


AGE GROUP ♦ 16-20 
EDUC LEVEL 9-12 


AGE GROUP -16-20 
EDUC LEVEL 13 + 


-05 -to -15 -20 

FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


= 84.5 


-2,5. 



AGE GROUP - 21-25 
EDUC LEVEL 0*8 


-0.5 -1.0 -1.5 -2.0 

= 71.5% 


-2.5 


AGE GROUP - 21-25 
EOUC LEVEL 9-12 


-2.5 


t 

"O 

30 

25 - 

20 - 

15 - 

10- 

5 t— 


-0.5 
= 48.1% 


-2.5 


AGE GROUP- 21-25 
EDUC LEVEL 13 + 


-2.5 


-0.5 -1.0 -l.S -2.0 

= 80.5% 

AGE GROUP- 26-30 
EOUC LEVEL 0-8 



-2.5 


AGE GROUP -26-30 
EDUC LEVEL 9-12 


-2.5 


20 - 
15 ~ 
10 - 
5 * 

qLl 


AGE GROUP - 26-30 
EDUC LEVEL 13 + 


-0.5 -1.0 -1.5 -2.0 

FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


-25 


Figure 144. Pre-maritai petting to climax: individual variation in frequencies, 
in four age groups, at three educational levels 

Showing percent of each population (vertical line) which engages in pre-marital pet- 
ting to the point of climax, with each type of frequency (horizontal line). Based on un- 
married males. 


538 



HETEROSEXUAL PETTING 


539 


actual differences between the generations (Table 102, Figures 117, 118, 
121). Even at the college level petting has increased within these thirty 
years. In the older generation of this group, about 87 per cent of the males 
were involved ; nearly 95 per cent has such experience today. Moreover, the 
younger generation of college males is starting its petting activity at an 
earlier age. Among those males who never go beyond grade school, only 
78 per cent of the older generation had any petting experience, in contrast 
to 94 per cent in the present day. 

The frequencies of petting activities reach their height between the ages 
of 21 and 25 (Table 53, Figures 38, 41). Calculating averages for the 
total population, including both those who do and those who do not 
engage in petting, the mean frequency of orgasm from this source, be- 
tween 21 and 25, is about once in six weeks. For those males who are 
actually reaching orgasm, the average frequencies are something more than 
once in three weeks. It is, of course, only a small part of the petting which 
actually reaches climax, and the frequencies of petting without climax are 
many times higher than the frequencies of petting to climax. Males who 
remain unmarried into still older age periods pet to climax less frequently, 
partly because they carry more of their heterosexual contacts through to 
coitus, partly because some of them are sexually apathetic, and partly be- 
cause some of them have homosexual histories. 

Petting never accounts for more than 3 per cent of the total outlet for 
any segment of the total male population (Table 53, Figures 39, 42). This 
is in the age period between 21 and 25. Considering only those who do 
reach orgasm in petting (the active population), about 6 per cent of the 
total outlet is derived from this source during the late teens. While the 
incidence and frequency figures in the total population drop in later 
years, the frequencies for those who go as far as orgasm gradually increase. 
In this active population, among those males who are still unmarried in 
their thirties, as much as 10 per cent of the outlet is derived from petting. 

Petting provides somewhat fewer orgasms than nocturnal emissions, and 
only animal intercourse is less important as a source of outlet. The real im- 
portance of petting, however, lies in the education it provides in making 
socio-sexual contacts. On this score, pre-marital petting is one of the most 
significant factors in the sexual lives of high school and college males and 
females. 

Among all urban groups, of all educational levels, petting to climax oc- 
curs 2 to 3 times as often as it does among rural boys of the same levels 
(Table 119). This is one of the most marked distinctions between rural and 
urban populations. These differences are more marked than the differences 
between the social levels within each area. 



540 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN tHE HUMAN MALE 


While there has been a fair amount of moral objection to petting, it has 
already been shown (Chapter 13) that religiously devout males are involved 
as often as those who are religiously inactive. The differences in patterns of 
petting in the several social levels outweigh any religious influences within 
any single social level. 

TECHNIQUES IN PETTING 

Petting techniques may include all the conceivable forms of physical 
contact between two individuals of the opposite sex, except that they do 
not include the actual union of genitalia. Petting usually starts with general 
body contacts, and with kissing. While kissing under any circumstances is 
more or less taboo for some individuals of the lower social levels (Chapter 
10), it is the most widely distributed form of contact among males and fe- 
males of the high school and college levels. In these groups it may occur 
among casual acquaintances who are having their first date. So common is 
kissing at this level that it has relatively little sexual significance unless it 
becomes specifically elaborated. Simple hp kissing may be extended into a 
deep kiss (a French kiss or soul kiss, in the college parlance) which may in- 
volve more or less extensive tongue contacts, contacts of the inner lips, and 
a considerable stimulation of the interior of the mouth by the other indi- 
vidual’s tongue. From the reptiles, down through the birds and the mam- 
mals, such tongue and mouth contacts are common concomitants of other 
sexual activities (Beach 1947). For the other vertebrates, tongue contacts 
are definitely erotic, and they are naturally so for the human animal that is 
not too inhibited by its esthetic and cultural backgrounds. Deep kissing 
may effect orgasm, even though no other physical contacts are involved. 

Petting techniques usually expand in a more or less standard sequence, 
as the partners become better acquainted. Beginning with general body 
contact, lip kissing, and the deep kiss, it advances to a deliberate manipula- 
tion of the female breast, to mouth contacts with the female breast, to 
manual stimulation of the female genitalia, less often to the manual stimu- 
lation of the male genitalia, to the apposition of naked genitalia, to oral 
stimulation of the male genitalia, and finally to oral stimulation of the fe- 
male genitalia (Tables 93, 94). Petting techniques at the grade school level 
rarely go beyond incidental breast and genital contacts ; but a goodly por- 
tion of the petting at high school and college levels does arrive at more spe- 
cific genital manipulation. A great many engaged couples go that far before 
they marry. It is a smaller portion of the population which includes mouth- 
genital contacts in its pre-marital history (Table 94). 

Most of the action in a petting relationship originates with the male. 
Most of it is designed to stimulate the female. It is doubtful if a sufficient 
biologic basis could be shown for such a one-sided performance, and it may 
be that this great difference in the activity of the male and the female is, at 
least in part, another outcome of the patterns by which females are raised 



HETEROSEXUAL PETTING 


541 


in our culture. The male in the petting relationship derives his stimulation 
through his own activity in contact with the female, and this is often suffi- 
cient, as already indicated, to lead to spontaneous ejaculation. 

The astonishment of the lower level at the petting behavior of the better 
educated groups has been recorded in Chapter 10. As there noted, petting 
is the particular activity which has led many persons to conclude that col- 
lege students are sexually wild and perverted. On the other hand, the college 
level disapproves of the heterosexual intercourse which the lower level has, 
in some abundance, before marriage. The conflict is obviously one between 
two systems of mores, between two cultural patterns, only one of which 
seems right to a person who accepts the traditions of the group in which 
he has been raised. With the better educated groups, intercourse versus 
petting is a question of morals. For the lower level, it is a problem of un- 
derstandmg how a mentally normal individual can engage in such highly 
erotic activity as petting and still refrain from actual intercourse. 

There is some indication that younger generations have become freer in 
making these contacts. They also seem to be becoming freer in petting in 
public places. On doorsteps and on street corners, and on high school and 
college campuses, general body contacts and more specific hugging and 
kissing may be observed in the daytime as well as in the evening hours. 
Similar contacts may be observed in automobiles, on double dates, at cock- 
tail parties, at parties of other sorts, in taverns and in restaurants, in drug 
stores and inns, in reception rooms in college dormitories, in high school 
corridors, in the homes of many of the students, and wherever else young 
people congregate. More specific contacts may call for more privacy. On 
occasion, some nudity may be involved, and there are a few records of 
males who sleep nude with partners with whom they become involved in 
intensive petting, while never having genital intercourse. Sometimes naked 
genitalia are placed in apposition, again without effecting coitus. 

To some extent, petting is the outcome of the upper level’s attempt to 
avoid pre-marital intercourse. The condemnation of petting on the ground 
that it may lead to something that is worse is quite unfounded, for there is 
no evidence that the frequency of pre-marital intercourse has increased 
during recent generations (Chapter 11), even though petting has increased. 
In a number of cases, the specific record indicates that there would have 
been intercourse if petting had not supplied an outlet. 

The physical outcome of petting has been a matter of some concern to 
educators, to parents, and to high school and college students themselves 
(as in Elliott and Bone 1929, Butterfield 1939, Rice 1946, Frank 1946). 
There is probably no sex question which is asked more often by the younger 
generation than this one concerning the physical outcome of their petting 
behavior. Consequently, it has been important to secure data on this point. 
The evidence is now clear that such arousal as petting provides may seri- 



542 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


ously disturb some individuals, leaving them in a more or less extended 
nervous state unless the activity has proceeded to the point of orgasm. If 
orgasm results, there seem to be no after-effects other than those which fol- 
low any other type of sexual activity. On the other hand, there is a portion 
of the males, perhaps as many as a third of those in the present sample, who 
may become involved in extensive petting which stops short of orgasm, and 
who are able to calm down without the specific release that sexual climax 
would provide. Many males who do not reach orgasm while in contact with 
the female resort to masturbation soon after they leave the girl. Pain which 
is ordinarily said to occur in the testes or in the groin (but which probably 
involves some other structures instead) is not uncommonly experienced by 
the male who fails to reach climax during the petting. It occasionally hap- 
pens that a male who has gone through a prolonged period of arousal, ex- 
tending perhaps for an hour or more, finds difficulty in achieving orgasm 
or, if that point is finally reached, may find that there is an insufficient 
nervous release, or that there is some localized pain following ejaculation. 

SOCIAL SIGNIFICANCE OF PETTING 

Throughout the animal kingdom, and to some extent in the plant king- 
dom as well, it is normal for an organism to respond to physical contact by 
pressing against the stimulating object. Unless high temperatures are in- 
volved, or pain is adduced by some other quality of the situation, pressure 
by an object normally leads the animal or some part of its body to move 
toward that object. The human babe so responds from the time of birth, 
and it soon learns that such responses are rewarded by the warmth of a 
contact with another human body, by additional satisfaction which it may 
receive from the petting and cuddling which its mother or others may give 
it, sometimes by food, and sometimes by protection from unpleasant situ- 
ations. These early contacts bring such arousal as would be called erotic 
in an adult, and which are undoubtedly so in the younger animal (Chap- 
ter 5). 

Throughout the first years of its life, most parents provide a considerable 
amount of stimulation for the child, and aid and abet the development of its 
emotional responses. To love a babe and to teach it to love in return is an 
accepted part of the mores. But as the child grows still older, most parents 
in our English-American culture begin to restrain its physical contacts, 
whether with themselves or with other persons. The small girl is taught 
that she should not allow contacts if they come from persons who are not 
relatives and, in particular, that she should avoid contacts with males. The 
boy learns that he is not supposed to touch girls, at least ‘‘until he gets 
older.” Any show of affection is deliberately controlled, and the growing 
boy is taught not to expect mothering or much sympathy when he faces 
difficult situations. As some of the psychiatrists (e,g., English and Pearson 
1945) have pointed out, the child is brought into a world that is filled with 



HETEROSEXUAL PETTING 


543 


affection and physical love; but as it grows up it is taught to resist its bio- 
logically normal responses and to pull away when it is touched by any 
other person. After fifteen or twenty years of such training, a marriage 
ceremony is supposed to correct all of the negative responses which have 
been drilled into the boy or girl, and in their marital relations they are sup- 
posed to become as natural and unrestrained as when they were babes. 
This, of course, is just too much to expect, and it is not surprising that a 
considerable portion of the best drilled persons in the population (males 
and females of the college level) is awkward and ineffective in developing 
affectional relations after marriage. 

The female is, on the average, slower in developing sexually, and less re- 
sponsive than the male. She is, in consequence, more easily affected by this 
training in the niceties of restraint. It is, therefore, not surprising to find 
sexually unresponsive wives in a startUngly high proportion of the mar- 
riages, especially in the better educated segments of the population. 

Within recent years, younger generations have come to realize something 
of the significance of pre-marital restraint. Although there is, of course, no 
doubt that many of the boys and girls who engage in petting do so for the 
sake of the immediate satisfaction to be obtained, a surprising number of 
them have consciously considered the relation of such experience to their 
subsequent marital adjustments. Their understanding of the situation has 
been helped by the numerous marriage manuals that have been published 
within the last twenty years, and by courses in psychology, home econom- 
ics, marriage, and child development, and in other fields of the social 
sciences. This explains, at least in part, why this younger generation has 
been more or less obhvious to the not inconsiderable criticisms made by 
older persons about its petting behavior (Jefferis and Nichols 1912, For- 
bush 1919, Liederman 1926, Meyer 1927, 1929, 1934, 1935, Eddy 1928a, 
1928b, Elliott and Bone 1929, Kirsch 1930, Weatherhead 1932, Edson 1936, 
Bruckner 1937, Dickerson 1937, Clarke 1938, A Catholic Woman Doctor 
1939, Kirkendall 1940, Kelly 1941, Bowman 1942, Moffett 1942, Morgan 
1943, Moore 1943, Popenoe 1943, Sadler and Sadler 1944, Fleege 1945, 
Griffin 1945, 1946, Davis 1946, Rice 1946, Boys Club Amer. 1946, Tanner 
1946, A Redemptorist Father 1946, H. Frank 1946, R. Frank 1946, 
McGill 1946a, 1946b, Gartland 1946). 

It is amazing to observe the mixture of scientifically supported logic, and 
of qtter illogic, which shapes the petting behavior of most of these youths. 
That some of them are in some psychic conflict over their activities is evi- 
denced by the curious rationalizations which they use to satisfy their con- 
sciences. They are particularly concerned with the avoidance of genital 
union. The fact that petting involves erotic contacts which are as effective 
as genital union, and that it may even involve contacts which have been 
more taboo than genital union, including some that have been considered 



544 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


perversions, does not disturb the youth so much as actual intercourse 
would. By petting, they preserve their virginities, even though they may 
achieve orgasm while doing so. They still value virginity, much as the pre- 
vious generations valued it. Only the list of most other activities has had 
new values placed on it. 

The younger generation considers that its type of behavior is more nat- 
ural than the restrained courting of the Victorian generations. It sees logic 
in the Freudian interpretations of the outcome of such restraint on the total 
personality of an individual. And it is impressed by the evidence which 
marriage counselors and psychiatrists have that the long periods of pre- 
marital restraint are the source of some of the difficulties which many per- 
sons find in making sexual adjustments in marriage. 

While our data on the sexual factor in marital adjustment must be pre- 
sented in a later volume, it may now be stated that there are always many 
factors which are involved in the success or failure of a marriage. It is usu- 
ally difficult to understand which factors came first in the chain of events, 
and the persons immediately concerned in any discord are often the ones 
least capable of understanding the sources of the difficulties in which they 
find themselves. Sexual adjustments are not the only problems involved in 
marriage, and often they are not even the most important factors in marital 
adjustments, A preliminary examination of the six thousand marital his- 
tories in the present study, and of nearly three thousand divorce histories, 
suggests that there may be nothing more important in a marriage than a 
determination that it shall persist. With such a determination, individuals 
force themselves to adjust and to accept situations which would seem suffi- 
cient grounds for a break-up, if the continuation of the marriage were not 
the prime objective. 

Nevertheless, sexual maladjustments contribute in perhaps three- 
quarters of the upper level marriages that end in separation or divorce, and 
in some smaller percentage of the lower level marriages that break up. 
Where the sexual adjustments are poor, marriages are maintained with 
difficulty. It takes a considerable amount of idealism and determination to 
keep a marriage together when the sexual adjustments are not right. Sexual 
factors are, in consequence, very important in a marriage. 

Specifically, the sexual factors which most often cause difficulty in the 
upper level marriages are (1) the failure of the male to show skill in sexual 
ap|)roach and technique, and (2) the failure of the female to participate 
with the abandon which is necessary for the successful consummation of 
any sexual relation. Both of these difficulties stem from the same source, 
namely, the restraints which are developed in pre-marital years, and the 
impossibihty of freely releasing those restraints after marriage. On this 
point Freud, the psychoanalysts, and the psychiatrists in general are largely 
agreed. On this point, our own data provide abundant evidence. The de- 



HETEROSEXUAL RETTING 


545 


tails of the several thousand marital histories that substantiate this con- 
clusion must be given later, but the matter needs to be brought up at this 
time because of its bearing on the significance of pre-marital petting. 

The male’s difficulties in his sexual relations after marriage include a lack 
of facility, of ease, or of suavity in establishing rapport in a sexual situa- 
tion. Marriage manuals are mistaken in considering that the masculine 
failure lies in an insufficient knowledge of techniques. Details of techniques 
come spontaneously enough when the male is at ease in his own mind 
about the propriety of his sexual behavior. But as an educated youth he 
has acquired ideas concerning esthetic acceptability, about the scientific 
interpretations of actions as clean or hygienic, about techniques that should 
be effective, mechanically, when he has intercourse. He has decided that 
there are sexual activities which are right and sexual activities which are 
wrong, or at least indecent — perhaps abnormal and perverted. Even though 
these things may not be consciously considered at the moment of inter- 
course, they are part of the subconscious which controls his performance. 
Few males achieve any real freedom in their sexual relations even with their 
wives. Few males realize how badly inhibited they are on these matters. In 
extreme cases these inhibitions may result in impotence for the male; and 
most instances of impotence (prior to old age, and outside of those few 
cases where there is physical damage to the genitalia) are to be found 
among upper level, educated males. The psychiatrist well understands that 
such impotence is the product of inhibitions. The hesitancy of the inhibited 
male even to try to secure coitus is reflected in the fact that marital coitus 
in the more religiously inclined males (Chapter 13) and among upper level 
males in general (Chapter 10) occurs with significantly lower frequencies 
than marital coitus in the lower educational levels. 

The inhibitions of the upper level female are more extreme than those of 
the average male. There are some of these females who object to all inter- 
course with their newly married husbands, and a larger number of the 
wives who remain uninterested in intercourse through the years of their 
marriage, who object to each new technique which the male tries, who 
charge their husbands with being lewd, lascivious, lacking in consideration, 
and guilty of sex perversion in general. There are numerous divorces which 
turn on the wife’s refusal to accept some item in coital technique which may 
in actuahty be commonplace in human behavior. The female who has hved 
for twenty or more years without learning that any ethically or socially de- 
cent male has ever touched a female breast, and the female who has no 
comprehension of the fact that sexual contacts may involve a great deal 
more than genital union, find it difficult to give up their ideas about the 
right and wrong of these matters and accept sexual relations with any 
abandon after marriage. The girl who, as a result of pre-marital petting re- 
lations, has learned something about the significance of tactile stimulation 



546 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


and response, has less of a problem in resolving her inhibitions after mar- 
riage. 

There is, then, considerable evidence that pre-marital petting experience 
contributes definitely to the elfectiveness of the sexual relations after 
marriage. The correlations will be given in a later volume. Some of those 
who have not had pre-marital petting experience do make satisfactory 
marital adjustments, but in many cases they make poorer adjustments. 
Although this conclusion is contrary to the usual statements in the sex 
education literature {e,g., Dickerson 1930, 1937, 1944, Popenoe 1938), it is 
in line with Terman’s findings (Terman 1938), and there have been some 
others (e.g.. Rice 1933, 1946, Taylor 1933, Himes 1940, Laton and Bailey 
1940, Corner and Landis 1941, English and Pearson 1945, Adams 1946) 
who have arrived at more or less the same conclusion. Whether pre-marital 
petting is right or wrong is, of course, a moral issue which a scientist has 
no capacity to decide. What the relations of pre-marital petting may be to 
a subsequent marital adjustment, is a matter that the scientist can measure. 



Chapter 17 

PRE-MARTTAL INTERCOURSE 


Throughout history, in all cultures, primitive, classic, and modern, the 
matter of non-marital intercourse has been one of social concern; but in 
nearly all cultures extra-marital intercourse has been considered more im- 
portant than pre-marital intercourse. In the ancient Hittite, Assyrian, and 
Babylonian codes (Harper 1904, Barton 1925), the issue was more often 
one of property rights, rather than one of ethics or morals. The married 
male’s ownership of his wife and his rights to all of the privileges that she 
could grant, were the primary concern. In most of the codes, pre-marital 
intercourse was rarely mentioned, unless it occurred after the time of 
betrothal. Then the first property rights emerged, there were laws against 
the infringement of those rights by another male, and considerable atten- 
tion was given to the nature of those rights when an engagement was 
broken. In all history there are few instances of such concern over pre- 
marital intercourse as exists in the Jewish and Anglo-American codes. 

There is an almost universal acceptance of pre-marital intercourse 
among so-called primitive peoples today, throughout the world (e.g,, 
Ratzel 1896, Malinowski 1929, Thurnwald 1931, Wissler 1922, Fortune 
1932, Murdock 1934, Blackwood 1935, Linton 1936, Landes 1938, Mead 
1939, Reichard 1938, Schapera 1941, Chappie and Coon 1942, Bryk 1944, 
Ford 1945, Fehlinger 1945). Sometimes the pre-marital activity has cer- 
tain restrictions put on it, often it is accepted quite without reservation. 
In only a few instances is there any outright condemnation of the in- 
tercourse (Murdock 1934, Mead 1939, Fehlinger 1945, Ford 1945, Morley 
1946). 

Pre-marital relations have also been more or less openly accepted in 
most of the other civilizations of the world, in the Orient, in the Ancient 
World, and among most European groups apart from the Anglo-American 
stocks. 

It would be significant to examine the origins of our current attitudes on 
coitus before marriage. Explanations of the codes as products of experience, 
as instruments designed to protect children born out of wedlock, and as de- 
vices for protecting the institution of marriage cannot represent the whole 
of the history. Part of it must stem from the tremendous importance which 
is attached in Jewish codes to the virginity of the female at the time of 
marriage. 


547 



548 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Even among the Jews, however, the virginity of the male was a matter of 
less concern, and most of the Continental European codes are closer to the 
Talmud than they are to Anglo-American attitudes in this respect. Certain 
it is that in our own culture, today, there is a considerable group of de- 
voutly religious persons who consider the pre-marital loss of virginity as a 
cardinal sin for the male, as well as for the female. 

In the case of the female, the unbroken hymen was depended upon in the 
Jewish code and subsequently among many European peoples as evidence 
of virginity at marriage, and marriage ceremonies in many Eastern Euro- 
pean and other groups still require the demonstration of such virginity for 
the sake of the public record. First generation immigrants in some parts of 
this country today may still send the blood-stained napkin back to relatives 
in Europe, as evidence of the vahd consummation of the marriage. Among 
present-day youth in our own culture, an individual may still be rated as a 
virgin, even though there have been other sorts of sexual relations, such as 
petting and all types of manual and oral contacts, as long as they do not 
involve intercourse which breaks the hymen (Chapter 16). 

Scientifically, popularly, and legally, the term ‘‘sexual intercourse” re- 
fers to genital union, and it is in that sense that the term is used here. 
The present chapter summarizes the data on the occurrence of all types 
of pre-marital intercourse, whether it is had with companions or with pros- 
titutes. A later chapter (Chapter 20) is concerned with the record on that 
part of the intercourse which is had with prostitutes. 

REFERENCES 

Data on the occurrence of pre-marital intercourse, and on the factors 
affecting the incidence and frequencies of pre-marital intercourse, have al- 
ready been presented in this volume in tables and charts, and in discussions 
in the text, as follows : 


PAGE TABLE FIGURE NATURE OF DATA 


116-118 

12 

14 

162-174 

24 , 25 , 27-29 

25 , 26 

190-191 

38 , 68 

30 

234 

49 


248 - 253 , 

54 , 63 - 65 , 

71 - 76 , 65-82 

277-288 

77-82 

312-315 

68 , 74 , 75 

91 

347-355 

85 - 87 , 96 , 
111 , 113 , 115 

101 - 102 , 145, 146 

364 

92 



Accumulative incidence data: intercourse 
with prostitutes 

Pre-adolescent sex play 

Sources of first ejaculation 

Range of variation and pre-marital inter- 
course 

Age and pre-marital intercourse 

Age at onset of adolescence and pre- 
marital intercourse 

Social level and pre-marital intercourse 

Attitudes on pre-marital intercourse at 
different social levels 



PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 549 


PAGE 

TABLE 

FIGURE 

376-383 


106 

396-413 

99-101, 
104, 105 

110,111,116,122 

455-458, 

120, 121 

125 

462 

477^81 

129 


488-^93 


126-130 

512 

549-552 

136, 138 

145-146 

552-555 


147-148, 155 


NATURE OF DATA 

Pre-marital intercourse in patterns of 
sexual behavior 

Comparisons of two generations in pre- 
marital intercourse 

Rural-urban backgrounds and pre-mar- 
ital intercourse 

Religious backgrounds and pre-marital 
intercourse 

Significance of pre-marital intercourse as 
one source of total outlet 

Relation of masturbation to pre-marital 
intercourse 

Accumulative incidence data on pre- 
marital intercourse 

Individual variation in pre-marital inter- 
course 


INCIDENCES AND FREQUENCIES 

In all other anthropoids effective coitus develops out of pre-adolescent 
attempts at heterosexual relations and begins as soon as the animal is 
physically capable and psychically oriented toward socio-sexual contacts. 
While there are families of a sort among anthropoids in the wild, where the 
male’s right to his females may be defended against outsiders, there is, of 
course, nothing to demark pre-marital from marital chapters in the coital 
history. Among some of the most poorly educated groups in our own cul- 
ture the distinctions between pre-marital and marital experience are hardly 
greater than those among the sub-human anthropoids; and there is no 
doubt that all males in an uninhibited society would have pre-adolescent 
and adolescent intercourse before marriage if there were no social re- 
straints to prevent them. The only conceivable exceptions would be found 
among those few individuals who were either physically incapable or 
physically so weak that they could not assert themselves against competing 
males. 

It is, in consequence, not surprising to find that most human males do 
have intercourse prior to marriage. Twenty-two per cent of all the pre- 
adolescents attempt coitus, chiefly between age ten and adolescence 
(Table 27, Chapter 5). Having once begun, the childhood activity carries 
over into adolescence in more than half of all the cases — among three- 
quarters of all the boys of the lower educational levels (Table 29). Hetero- 
sexual coitus provides the first ejaculation for an eighth (12.5%) of all the 
boys (Table 38), for a higher percentage (18.5%) of the boys who will not 
go beyond grade school, and for a much lower percentage (1.4%) of those 
who will ultimately go to college. 

The accumulative incidence figures for pre-marital intercourse vary con- 
siderably for difierent social levels (Figure 146). Among the males who go 



550 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Total Pre-marital Intercourse: Accumulati\t Incidence 


AGE 

1 

total 

POPULATION 

U. S. 

CORRECTIONS 

EDUC. LEVEL 

0-8 

EDUC. 

9- 

LEVEL 

■12 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13-}- 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

%with 1 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

3994 

0.0 

683 

0.0 

494 

0.0 

2817 

0.0 

9 

3994 

0.0 

683 

0.0 

494 

0.0 

2817 

0.0 

10 

3994 

0.0 

683 

0.1 

494 

0.0 

2817 i 

0.0 

11 

3993 

1.0 

682 

1.3 

494 

1.0 

2817 1 

0.2 

12 

3993 

4.9 

682 

6.5 

494 

5.1 

2817 

1.0 

13 

3992 

13.8 

681 

14.5 

494 

16.2 

2817 

3,1 

14 

3989 

27.8 

678 

28.0 

494 

33.4 

2817 

6.0 

15 

3982 

38.8 

671 

42.2 

494 

44.7 

2817 

9.5 

16 

3957 

51.6 

652 

56.9 

489 

58.1 

2816 

15.5 

17 

3887 

61.3 

608 

66.8 

467 

68.3 

2812 ! 

23.1 

18 

3709 

68,2 

560 

76.1 

415 

73.7 

2734 i 

30.8 

19 

3418 

71.5 

501 

80.0 

352 

75.6 

2565 

38.0 

20 

3031 

73.1 

438 

82.9 

285 : 

75.1 

2308 

44.4 

21 

2597 

74.9 

385 

83.6 

232 

76.7 

1980 

49.1 

22 

2078 

76.6 

310 

83.5 

187 

78.6 

1581 

54.1 

23 

1662 

78.7 

257 

85.2 ! 

156 

80.8 

1249 

56.9 

24 

1317 

80.2 

224 

87.1 

126 

81.7 

' 967 

59.3 

25 

1061 

83.3 

189 

89.9 

111 

83.8 

761 

64.4 

26 

861 

83.1 

165 

91.5 

93 

81.7 

603 

65.2 

27 

663 

84.3 

150 

92.0 

81 

84.0 

432 

65.0 

28 

530 

86.1 1 

131 

93.9 

70 

85.7 

329 

66.6 

29 

434 

85.5 1 

113 

92.9 

61 

85.2 

260 

67.3 

30 

353 

86.4 

100 

94.0 

51 

84.3 

202 

67.8 

31 

283 

86.4 

83 

94.0 



155 

67.1 

32 

243 

86.6 

77 

94.8 



127 

65.4 

33 

213 

85.4 

68 

94.1 



no 

64.5 

34 

181 

85.3 

64 

95.3 



87 

67.8 

35 

156 

87.5 

59 

94.9 



73 

65.8 

36 

140 

89.4 

58 

98.3 



63 

63.5 

37 

121 

90.4 

52 

98.1 



53 

64.2 

38 

114 

92.2 

i 50 

98.0 



51 

66,7 

i 


Table 136. Accumulative incidence data on total pre-marital intercourse 

Including pre-marital intercourse with companions and with prostitutes. In three 
educational levels, and in the total population corrected for the U. S. Census of 1940. 



PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


551 



Figure 145. Total pre-marital intercourse: accumulative incidence among single 

males 


Includes pre-marital intercourse with companions and pre-marital intercourse with 
prostitutes. Based on pre-marital histories of population corrected for U. S. Census 
distribution. 



Figure 146. Total pre-marital intercourse: accumulative incidence in three 

educational levels 

Showing percent of each population that has ever had pre-marital experience by each 
ot the indicated ages. Data based on unmarried males. 





552 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


to college, about 67 per cent has coital experience before marriage; among 
those who go into high school but not beyond, about 84 per cent has such 
intercourse; and among the boys who never go beyond grade school the 
accumulative incidence figure is 98 per cent. There are even some groups 
among the lower social levels where it appears to be impossible to find a 
single male who had not had experience by the time he had reached his 
middle teens. These class differences account for the fact that many an 
upper level clinician is amazed (as in Hohman and Schaffner 1947) at the 
coital records of the men with whom he is called upon to deal in a penal 
or mental institution, in the Army or in the Navy, in a factory or on some 
other industrial assignment. Most of the previous studies which have been 
confined to males of a single social level arrive at much the same incidence 
figures which we have obtained: 54 per cent in Hamilton (1929), 55 per 
cent in Peterson (1938), 52 per cent in Bromley and Britten (1938), 60 per 
cent in Wile (1941), 45 per cent in Finger (1947) — most of these for males 
of the college level, while they were still in college and therefore short of 
having their full pre-marital experience. Another group of studies (Exner 
1915, Achilles 1923, Willoughby 1937, Terman 1938, Butterfield 1939) has 
involved populations of mixed social levels, and other sources of error 
which make the data uninterpretable. 

The frequencies of pre-marital intercourse vary between social levels 
even more than do the incidences (Tables 86, 1 1 1). Coitus, either with com- 
panions or with prostitutes, never accounts for more than 21 per cent of the 
total outlet of the unmarried males of the college level, but it may consti- 
tute as much as 68 per cent of the outlet for males of the lower educational 
levels. For the better educated portion of the population, the significance of 
pre-marital intercourse lies not in the number of the orgasms which it 
provides, but in the fact that such orgasms as do come from this source 
represent a break with the mores of the group. An upper level male who is 
not married thinks of sex as masturbation, nocturnal emissions, petting, 
and a continual excitement over girls with whom he would like relations, 
but with whom he rarely effects actual coitus. The lower level male, on the 
other hand, may find it difficult to understand that a sex study should be 
concerned with anything except heterosexual coitus, unless perchance he is 
interested in homosexual relations. 

In the population as a whole, the frequencies of pre-marital intercourse 
reach their maximum (for those males who have any such experience) in 
the earlier adolescent years, where coitus averages about 2.0 per week, and 
where it provides nearly half of the total sexual outlet (Table 57). The fre- 
quencies gradually drop with age, but not so fast as the total outlet drops. 
Consequently the significance of pre-marital intercourse rises, and by the 
middle forties it accounts for two-thirds (66.6%) of the total outlet of the 
unmarried males who are having any coitus at all (Table 57). 



PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


553 


Since masturbation and intercourse are the two chief sources of pre- 
marital orgasm for the population as a whole, they are, as previously 
noted, the only outlets which show the same range of individual variation. 
There are some males, chiefly at the upper level, whose pre-marital inter- 
course is confined to a single experience, and then only with the fiancee 
immediately before marriage. There are some males, chiefly at the lower 
levels, who have much higher frequencies of intercourse, even up to ten or 
more times per week. In every age group between adolescence and 25 
there are males who reach frequencies which may average as much as 
25 times per week continuously for five or more years ; but the rates of the 
extreme individuals are lower in the later years. For the total population 
the frequencies average 1.4 per week in the late teens and early twenties, 
and less than that thereafter. Boys of the lower level are more likely to av- 
erage pre-marital intercourse with frequencies of two to four times a week, 
which is close to the average frequencies in marriage for many of the popu- 
lation. The rates for any particular individual may vary considerably, de- 
pending upon the accessibility of female partners. 

The highest incidences and highest average frequencies of pre-marital 
intercourse occur among those males of the grade school level who became 
adolescent by ten or eleven (Table 74). By the late teens, 86 per cent of this 
lower level and early-adolescent group is having intercourse, as against 
less than 33 per cent of the college-bred males who were late in becoming 
adolescent. The highest frequencies of pre-marital intercourse are reached 
by this same grade school group in their late teens, where the averages are 
over 3.0 per week for the whole group, or 3.6 per week for those individuals 
who are actively involved in intercourse. In the same age period (16-20), 
the frequencies for the high school group average about 1.5 per week, and 
for the college group about 0.3 per week (once in three weeks). 

At all social levels, pre-marital intercourse occurs much less frequently 
among males who are devoutly religious, whether they be Protestant or 
Cathohc (Table 128). Conversely, it occurs most frequently among the 
males who are least concerned with the Church. The frequencies are low 
for all Jewish groups but, interestingly enough, the incidences and fre- 
quencies seem to be higher for Orthodox Jews than for inactive Jews. This 
may be a result of the considerable condemnation which the Jewish faith 
puts upon masturbation as an outlet. The differences between devout and 
inactive Catholics are much greater. There may be three times as much pre- 
marital intercourse among inactive Catholics as there is among those who 
follow the Church teachings more strictly. The differences between active 
and inactive Protestants are not so great. 

Pre-marital intercourse occurs much more often among boys who live in 
cities and towns, less often among farm boys (Table 120). This is true at all 



A<5£ GROUP • ADOL-15 
EDUC LEVEL 0-8 


-t.O -Z.0 -5.0 -40 -5.0 -60 -7.0 


AGE GROUP • ADOL-15 
EDUC LEVEL 9-IE 


5 -1.0 -LO -3.0 -4 0 -5 0 -60 -7.0 


LI 0 *90.7% 


AGE GROUP • ADOL-15 
EDUC LEVEL 13 + 


-1.0 -20 -50 -40 -50 -6.0 


AGE GROUP ‘16-20 
EDUC LEVEL 0-8 


-8.0 -9.0 -10.0 



AGE GROUP -16-20 
EDUC LEVEL 9-12 


0 - 1.0 - 2.0 
%(| 0 *61 2 % 

20 Ll 


-40 -5.0 -6.0 -7.0 -8.0 


AGE GROUP ‘16-20 
EDUC LEVEL 13 + 


-40 -50 -60 -7.0 

FREQUENCY PER WEEK 


Figure 147. Pre-marital intercourse with companions: individual variation in 
frequencies, at ages adolescent~15 and 16-20, for three educational levels 

Showing percent of each population (vertical line) which engages in pre-marital in- 
tercourse with companions with each type of frequency (horizontal line). 

554 


AGE GROUP - 21- as 
€DUC LEVEL 0-8 



Figure 148. Pre-marital intercourse with companions: individual variation in 
frequencies, at ages 21-25 and 26-30, for three educational levels 

Showing percent of each population (vertical line) that has pre-marital intercourse 
with each type of frequency (horizontal line). 

555 





556 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


social levels, and it is particularly true in the earlier adolescent years. This 
may be a product of the generally stricter religious attitudes in rural com- 
munities, and/or of the reduced opportunity which the farm boy has to date 
girls and to meet them in any sort of social relation. 

Finally, the fundamental position of heterosexual coitus in the lives of 
younger males is attested by the fact that the place of intercourse in the 
present day is not materially different from what it was twenty-two years 
ago (Table 99). As regards the incidences and frequencies with which it is 
had in each social level, intercourse in the present generation is about what 
it was a generation or two ago. For the college segment of the population, 
as many males are involved, and with the same frequencies, as in the older 
generations. In view of the considerable efforts that have been made by 
some groups to control pre-marital intercourse, this failure to change the 
pattern is most significant. For the lower educational levels, the accumula- 
tive incidence figures for the younger generation are also the same as those 
for the older generation ; but the lower level boys of the younger generation 
start earlier and have higher frequencies at an earlier age. This earlier ac- 
tivity may be the product of improved nutritional and health conditions in 
the lower levels today. 

In 1938, Terman, in a volume on Psychological Factors in Marital Hap- 
piness, attempted to compare the amount of pre-marital intercourse in 
older and younger generations. He reached the conclusion that there had 
been a steady increase in the incidence of such activity among the persons 
born in four successive decades (before 1890, and through 1910), and that 
these trends were proceediag with such “extraordinary rapidity” that “in- 
tercourse with future spouse before marriage will become universal by 1950 
or 1955” — meaning among persons born in those years. This finding is not 
borne out by data in the present study (Chapter 1 1). Since the Terman fig- 
ures have been extensively quoted, it is important to point out that the 
study, of which those data were a part, involved some basic procedural er- 
rors. There was no sufficient, successive breakdown of the population for 
the series of biologic and social factors which must be kept constant if 
sound analyses are to be made (Chapter 3). The study was based upon 
group-administered questionnaires, which have proved inadequate for sex 
studies in all but a very few special cases. The subjects were mostly chents 
of family relations institutes, and it is not certain that such a sample is rep- 
resentative of the population as a whole. The group represented mixed edu- 
cational levels. Nearly 71 per cent of the male population had had college 
or more advanced training, but 30 per cent were persons who had not gone 
beyond high school, or in some cases not beyond grade school. In view of 
the considerable differences which our present data show to exist between 
these several educational levels (Chapter 10), it is obvious that any mixed 
population is inadequate for analyzing pre-marital intercourse. 



PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


557 


It should also be emphasized that the most strategic population in the 
Terman series, the sample which estabhshed the last point (for 1910) on the 
curve, included only 22 males. Such a sample is, of course, totally insuflB- 
cient for representing any large portion of the American population (Chap- 
ter 3). The data which we now have on pre-marital intercourse would lead 
us to predict that there will always be a segment of the population which 
will, as a moral issue, avoid such activity. While the incidence of pre- 
marital intercourse has remained stable within each social level in the last 
twenty or thirty years, it should be pointed out that the number of persons 
who go to college has materially increased in that period. Since this is the 
group that has the least pre-marital coitus, this means that there is now a 
distinctly larger portion of the population which is going without pre- 
marital coitus than there was when Terman made his prediction ten years 
ago. 

NATURE OF PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

A detailed analysis of the varying situations under which pre-marital 
intercourse is had will have to be made in a later publication. It should, 
however, be emphasized now that the intercourse varies considerably not 
only in frequencies but also in regard to the number of partners involved, 
in regard to the nature of the partners, and in regard to the times and places 
where the activity is had. 

There are males, particularly of the upper social level, who may confine 
their pre-marital intercourse to a single girl, who is often the fiancee. 
There are males who have some dozens or scores of partners before they 
marry. In some cases, lower level males may have intercourse with several 
hundred or even a thousand or more girls in pre-marital relations. There 
are quite a few individuals, especially of the grade school and high school 
levels, who find more interest in the pursuit and conquest, and in a variety 
of partners, than they do in developing long-time relations with a single 
girl. Some males avoid all repetitions of experience with the same girl. 
Sometimes the interest which such a promiscuous male has in heterosexual 
coitus does not involve any interest in the girls themselves. Many a lower 
level male states quite frankly that he does not hke girls, and that he would 
have nothing to do with them if it were not for the fact that they are sources 
of intercourse. There are vernacular phrases which precisely sum up this 
situation. Until such attitudes are comprehended by clinicians, and espe- 
cially by pubhc health officials, and until such professional groups under- 
stand the lower level’s ability to effect frequent contacts with such a va- 
riety of partners, the control of venereal disease is not likely to become more 
effective. 

Unfortunately, data on the social levels of the girls with whom males 
have their pre-marital relations have not been systematically gathered in 
the present study. There is a popular opinion that most pre-marital inter- 



558 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


course is had with girls who are below the social status of the male. Such 
information as we have does not seem to confirm this opinion. Certainly, 
at the college level today, males find a great deal of their pre-marital inter- 
course within their own level. Although there is some reason for believing 
that older generations of college males more often resorted to town girls 
for their sexual contacts, the specific data are not available. Of course, the 
educationally lower level males have most of their pre-marital intercourse 
with lower level girls. 

The upper level male has only a very small portion of his pre-marital 
contacts with professional prostitutes. The lower level male depends to a 
much greater degree upon the commercial source. 

Most males have intercourse with girls of about their own age, or with 
girls who are only a few years younger. Only a few males have intercourse 
with very young girls, except when they themselves are equally young. 
There are not many males who have intercourse with women who are 
much older than themselves, although there are some cases of teen-age and 
even pre-adolescent boys who have intercourse with married women in 
their twenties, their thirties, or older. A few males develop long-time rela- 
tions with older women, either single, married, or divorced women; but 
nearly all of the intercourse which the young, unmarried male has is with 
unmarried females. 

Heterosexual incest occurs more frequently in the thinking of clinicians 
and social workers than it does in actual performance. There may be a 
good many males who have thought of the possibilities of sexual relations 
with sisters or mothers or with other close female relatives, but even this 
is by no means universal, and is usually confined to limited periods in the 
boy’s younger years. There are some psychoanalysts who contend that 
they have never had a patient who has not had incestuous relations; but 
such a statement is totally out of line with the specific records which have 
been obtained in this study or which, for that matter, have been obtained 
in any other survey of the general population. The clinician must beware 
that the select group of persons who come to a clinic does not color his 
thinking concerning the population as a whole. In the present study, such 
incestuous relations as have been recorded represent every social level, in- 
cluding males of the lower levels and males who belong to the socially top 
levels. Because the cases are so few, it would be misleading to suggest 
where the highest incidences lie. The most frequent incestuous contacts are 
between pre-adolescent children, but the number of such cases among ado- 
lescent or older males is very small. 

The circumstances under which pre-marital intercourse is had differ 
again for social levels. Some of the intercourse which the college male has 
before marriage may be had on the college grounds, or in college buildings, 
but more of it occurs during vacation periods, often in the girPs home town. 



PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


559 


and often under the girl’s parental roof. For all levels, intercourse is had 
in cars, some of it outdoors in the open, some in tourist camps and hotels, 
some in the homes of friends or in rented apartments, some in the male’s 
home, but much of it in the home of the girl. Special provisions for pre- 
marital intercourse are almost as commonly accepted in certain seg- 
ments of the population as communal bachelors’ huts are in some primi- 
tive societies (Malinowski 1929, Murdock 1934, Reichard 1938, Mead 
1939, Bryk 1944, Fehlinger 1945, Morley 1946). 

While the upper social level has a high portion (90%) of its marital inter- 
course without clothing, not much more than half (55%) of its pre-marital 
intercourse is had under circumstances where that is possible (Table 95). 
The lower social level, which has less than half (43%) of its marital inter- 
course without clothing, has even less (32%) of its pre-marital intercourse 
in that fashion. 

SIGNIFICANCE OF PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

To have or not to have pre-marital intercourse is a more important issue 
for more males than any other aspect of sex. Heterosexual intercourse is 
the ultimate goal of all sexual thought and of all deliberately planned sexual 
activity for perhaps half or more of the unmarried male population; and it 
is a matter of considerable importance for a high proportion of the re- 
maining males who, nevertheless, may get their actual outlet from other 
sources. Except for the 15 per cent of the population which goes to col- 
lege, most males actually accept pre-marital intercourse, and believe it 
to be a desirable part of a normal human development. Even among those 
who pubhcly uphold the taboos against pre-marital relations, including 
legislators and the law enforcement officers who sporadically impose legal 
penalties upon non-marital activities, there are many who demonstrate 
through their own histories that they consider pre-marital and extra- 
marital intercourse acceptable and desirable. There is a not inconsiderable 
portion of the population which openly defends the value of such inter- 
course. This is particularly true at the lower educational levels, but it is 
sometimes true at top social levels. The general impression which is held 
by many students of social affairs that the middle class is the one which 
most rigorously upholds the social traditions is obviously based on the ex- 
pressed opinions of this group, rather than upon the record of its actual 
behavior. 

In Continental Europe, the acceptance of pre-marital intercourse is more 
general than it is in our American population, and European clinicians have 
contributed materially to an increasing opinion among professional groups 
in this country that there are social values to be obtained by pre-marital 
experience in intercourse. There are some clinicians who advise their pa- 
tients to this effect, and there are histories of individuals who would have 


19 



560 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


found it difiScult to have made socio-sexual adjustments without such ex- 
perience. 

On the other hand, of course, there is no sort of sexual behavior which 
has been more often condemned than pre-marital intercourse. It has usu- 
ally been condemned on strictly moral grounds (as in Jefferis and Nichols 
1912, Armitage 1913, Exner 1914, Gallichan 1916, Bigelow 1916, Forbush 
1919, W. S. Hall 1920, Coppens and Spalding 1921, U. S. Public Health 
Service 1921, 1937, Meyer 1927, 1929, 1934, Eddy 1928a, Clark 1928, El- 
liott and Bone 1929, Kirsch 1930, Gillis 1930, Amer. Soc. Hygiene Associa- 
tion 1930, Ruland and Rattler 1934, Hildebrand 1935, Martindale 1925, 
Bruckner 1937, Lowry 1938, A Catholic Woman Doctor 1939, Kelly 1941, 
H. Frank 1941, Moore 1943, Dickerson 1944, GriflBn 1945, 1946, Davis 
1946, Wood in Chivers 1946, Gartland 1946, McGill 1946, Redemptorist 
Father 1946). 

More scientific issues are raised when pre-marital intercourse is con- 
demned on the ground that it leads to unwanted pregnancies, to the birth 
of offspring outside of wedlock, to the acquirement and spread of venereal 
disease, to psychic upset for the individual, to social and legal diflSculties, 
and to maladjustments with one’s spouse after marriage (W. S. Hall 1907, 
1909, Liederman 1926, Eddy 1928a, 1928b, Amer. Soc. Hygiene Associa- 
tion 1930, Dickerson 1930, 1937, 1944, 1946, Exner 1932, Rice 1933a, 
1933b, 1946, Meagher and Jelliffe 1936, Popenoe 1936, 1940, 1943, 1944, 
Stone and Stone 1937, Snow 1937, Clarke 1938, Butterfield 1939, Crisp 
1939, Kirkendall 1940, Bowman 1942, Sadler and Sadler 1944, Adams 
1946, Boys Club Amer. 1946, R. Frank 1946). The questions involved here 
represent physical situations and measurable social relationships which can 
be subjected to scientific investigation. Unfortunately, the few scientists 
who have written on these matters have treated them in much the same 
subjective fashion as have persons without scientific backgrounds. There 
have been pleas for polygamy and promiscuity and there have been pleas 
for chastity, written by biologists, by physicians, by psychologists, and by 
psychiatrists, quite without benefit from the scientific training on which 
they traded for their reputations. 

In a later volume we shall endeavor to make an objective study of pre- 
marital intercourse in its several social relations, and particularly in regard 
to its effect on subsequent marital adjustments. It may be pointed out now 
that simple correlations (as used in Terman 1938, Burgess and Cottrell 
1939) cannot suflSce to measure the effects of pre-marital experience upon 
marital histories. Simple two-way correlations are never wholly adequate 
for showing cause and effect. At the best they show a relation, but not 
necessarily a causal relationship. They are always inadequate unless the 
items that are correlated are well-defined units, rather than complexes of 
units which have varied effects as their ingredients vary. 



PRE-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


561 


It does not suffice to show that the persons who have had or who have 
not had pre-marital experience are the ones who make the best or do not 
make the best adjustments after marriage. For pre-marital intercourse is 
always a complexity of things. It is, in part, a question of the sort of indi- 
vidual who has the intercourse and the degree to which the pre-marital 
activity is acceptable or unacceptable in the individual’s whole pattern of 
behavior. It depends upon the extent of the psychic conflict which may be 
evoked for an individual who transgresses the ideals and philosophies by 
which he has been raised, and to which he may still subconsciously adhere. 
For a person who believes that pre-marital intercourse is morally wrong 
there may be, as the specific histories show, conflicts which can do damage 
not only to marital adjustments, but to the entire personality of the indi- 
vidual. For a person who really accepts pre-marital intercourse, and who in 
actuahty is not in conflict with himself when he engages in such behavior, 
the outcome may be totally different. 

Again, the effects of pre-marital intercourse depend upon the nature of 
the partners with whom it is had, and the degree to which the activity be- 
comes promiscuous. It is a question of the nature of the female partners, 
whether it is had with girls of the same social level or with girls of lower 
social levels, whether it is had as a social relationship or as a commercial 
relation, whether or nor it is had with the fiancee before marriage. The 
effect of pre-marital intercourse upon the marital adjustment may depend 
upon the extent to which the female partner accepts the intercourse, and 
the extent to which the male accepts the idea of his wife’s having had in- 
tercourse before he married her. Even in those cases where both the spouses 
beheve that they accept the idea, situations of stress after marriage may 
bring the issue up for recriminations. 

The significance of pre-marital intercourse depends upon the situations 
under which it is had. If it is had under conditions which are physically un- 
comfortable and not conducive to a mutually satisfactory relationship, if 
it is had under conditions which leave the individuals disturbed for fear 
that they have been or will be detected, the outcome is one thing. If it is had 
under satisfying circumstances and without fear, the outcome may be very 
different. 

The meaning of the pre-marital intercourse will vary with its relation to 
venereal disease. At the college level, nearly all of the relations are had with 
a condom. Most of the pre-marital intercourse is had with girls of the same 
level. Consequently the incidence of venereal disease acquired by these 
persons is exceedingly low. On the other hand, the incidence of venereal 
disease resulting from pre-marital intercourse at the lower social levels, 
where condoms are not often used, is as high as and probably higher than 
is ordinarily indicated in the social hygiene literature. 



562 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


The significance of pre-marital intercourse depends upon the success or 
failure with which the couple avoids an unwanted pregnancy. It is much 
affected even by the fear of such a pregnancy. At the college level where 
contraceptives are almost universally used, the incidence of pre-marital 
pregnancies is phenomenally low. Those pregnancies that do occur almost 
invariably represent instances where contraceptives were not employed. In 
segments of the population which rarely use contraceptives, the frequencies 
of pre-marital pregnancies are quite high. 

At the other end of the correlation, it is, of course, equally inadequate to 
treat marital happiness as a unit character. There are many factors which 
may affect marital adjustment, and the identification of the part which the 
sexual factor plays must depend on an exceedingly acute understanding of 
the effects of all these other factors. 

It is sometimes asserted that all persons who have pre-marital intercourse 
subsequently regret the experience, and that such regrets may constitute a 
major cloud on their lives. There are a few males whose histories seem to 
indicate that they have so reacted to their pre-marital experience, but a very 
high proportion of the thousands of experienced males whom we have 
questioned on this point indicated that they did not regret having had such 
experience, and that the pre-marital intercourse had not caused any trouble 
in their subsequent marital adjustments. It is notable that most of the males 
who did regret the experience were individuals who had had very little pre- 
marital intercourse, amounting in most cases to not more than one or two 
experiences. It will, of course, be particularly significant at some later time 
to compare the responses of the females who have had pre-marital ex- 
perience. 

For the individual who is particularly concerned with the moral values 
of sexual behavior, none of these scientific issues are, of course, of any 
moment. For such individuals, moral issues are a very real part of life. 
They are as real as the social values of a heterosexual adjustment, and the 
happiness or unhappiness of a marital adjustment. They should not be 
overlooked by the scientist who attempts to make an objective measure of 
the outcome of pre-marital intercourse. 



Chapter 18 

MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

Marital intercourse is the one type of sexual activity which is approved 
by our Anglo-American mores and legal codes. For those males who are 
married and living with their wives, marital intercourse accounts for most 
of the sexual outlet; and to them, a successful sexual adjustment means 
sufiBciently frequent and emotionally effective intercourse with their wives. 
It is, in consequence, inevitable in any study of human sexual behavior that 
especial attention be given to the nature of marital relationships. 

Sociologists and anthropologists generally consider that the family is the 
basis of human society, and at least some students believe that the sexual 
attraction between the anthropoid male and female has been fundamental 
in the development of the human and infra-human family. Supporting data 
for these opinions are adduced from a study of the anthropoid family (e.g,, 
Miller 1928, 1931). But whatever the phylogenetic history of the human 
family, the evidence is clear that the sexual factor contributes materially to 
its maintenance today. We have already emphasized (Chapter 16) that the 
success or failure of a marriage usually depends upon a multiplicity of fac- 
tors, of which the sexual are only a part. Nevertheless, as we have further 
pointed out, where the sexual adjustments are poor, marriages are main- 
tained with difficulty. 

Society is interested in the nature of marital intercourse because it is in- 
terested in the maintenance of the family. Society is interested in maintain- 
ing the family as a way for men and women to live together in partnerships 
that may make for more effective functioning than solitary living may al- 
low. Society is interested in maintaining the family as a means of providing 
homes for children that result from coitus; and in Jewish and many Chris- 
tian philosophies, this is made a prime end of marriage. Society is also in- 
terested in maintaining families as a means of providing a regular sexual 
outlet for adults, and as a means of controlling promiscuous sexual activity. 
While these latter interests are not so often formulated in the thinking of 
our culture, these functions of marriage are more evident in some primitive 
cultures. Whatever other interests are involved, the sexual factor is one 
which is of considerable concern to any group that is interested in the 
maintenance of the family. 

While it is not the function of the present chapter to measure the signifi- 
cance of the sexual factors in the success or failure of a marriage, it will 
summarize the data which have been given in the present volume on the 

563 



564 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


nature of sexual relations in marriage. With this as a starting point, it 
should be possible, in a subsequent volume, to make more understanding 
analyses of the data which we are gathering on the sexual factor in marital 
adjustments. 

That the present volume has been so largely concerned with types of sex- 
ual activity other than marital intercourse is due to the fact that only a por- 
tion of the male population is married at any one time. There are, of course, 
males who never marry; and every male spends a considerable part of his 
life outside of marriage. All males must depend, at times, on some other 
source than marital intercourse for their sexual outlets. Moreover, even 
those males who are married derive a not inconsiderable portion of their 
orgasms from sources other than intercourse with their wives. It is one of 
the functions of the present chapter to show the place of marital intercourse 
among the many sexual activities which contribute to the total sexual life 
of the human male. 


REFERENCES 

The term marital intercourse as used in the present volume applies to in- 
tercourse which is had between spouses. Specific data on the incidences and 
frequencies of marital intercourse, and the factors affecting that intercourse, 
have been presented previously in this volume as follows: 


PAGE 

TABLE 

FIGURE 

NATURE OF DATA 

234 

49 


Range of variation and marital intercourse 

252-257 

56 

44-49 

Age and marital intercourse 

277-281 

63 


Total intercourse in relation to marital status and age 

306-308 

71 


Total sexual outlet in married males, as related to age 
at onset of adolescence 

355-358, 

88,112 

104 

Social level and marital intercourse 

432 

363-369 

93 


Petting techniques at three educational levels 

368-373 

94 


Oral techniques at three educational levels 

365-367, 

95 


Coital techniques and nudity at three educational levels 

372-374 

380, 382 

97 

107 

Patterns of sexual behavior among married males 

410, 414 

104 


Frequencies of marital intercourse in older and younger 
generations 

455, 459 

122 


Marital intercourse and rural-urban backgrounds 

479-482 

130 


Marital intercourse and religious backgrounds 

382, 488- 

97 

126-127, 

Significance of marital intercourse as one source of 

489 


131-133 

total outlet 

564-566 


149-150 

Accumulative incidence for total intercourse 

564-565, 


151 

Individual variation in marital intercourse 

570 


INCIDENCE AND SIGNIFICANCE 

Marital intercourse is the one sort of sexual activity which involves prac- 
tically 100 per cent of the eligible males in the population. There are ex- 
ceedingly few who marry and then fail to have any intercourse with their 



MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


565 


wives. Exceptions occur only among those who never live with their spouses 
after marriage, among those very few who are physically incapable of even 
attempting intercourse, among a few of those who are primarily homo- 
sexual and whose wives may be similarly homosexual, and among the still 
fewer males who are so inhibited by religious, esthetic, or other philoso- 
phies that they are incapable of performance or deliberately choose to 
avoid coitus even with their wives. Under forty years of age, these abstinent 
males are so few that they never account for more than a fraction of 1 per 
cent of the married population. At later ages, there are a few more males 
who do not engage in marital intercourse: as many as 2 per cent in the late 
forties and as many as 6 per cent in the late fifties (Table 56). No other type 
of sexual activity is found in the histories of such a high proportion of an 
eligible population. 



Showing percent of total population that has ever had experience in any kind of inter- 
course by each of the indicated ages. All data based on total population irrespective of 
marital status, and corrected for the U. S. Census distribution. 


But although marital intercourse thus provides the chief source of outlet 
for married males, immediately from the time of onset of marriage, it falls 
considerably short of constituting the total outlet of those individuals. In 
the married population taken as a whole, it does not ordinarily provide 
more than about 85 per cent of the total sexual outlet (Table 56). The re- 
maining orgasms of the married male are derived from masturbation, noc- 
turnal emissions, petting and heterosexual coitus with partners other than 
wives, the homosexual, and, especially in some Western rural areas, from 
intercourse with other animals (Figures 131-133). There is no pre-marital 
sexual activity which may not continue into marriage, although the fre- 
quencies of all these other activities are almost invariably reduced. 



566 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Total Intercourse: Accumulative Incidence 


AGE 

TOTAL POPULATION 
U. S. CORRECTIONS 

EDUC, LEVEL 

0-8 

EDUC. LEVEL 

9-12 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13+ 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
feper. 

Cases ' 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

4148 

0.0 

663 

0.0 

668 

0.0 

2817 ! 

0.0 

9 

4148 

0.0 

663 

0.0 

■ 668 1 

0.0 i 

2817 [ 

0.0 

10 

4148 

0.1 

663 

0.2 1 

1 668 

0.0 

2817 ' 

0.0 

11 

4148 

0.8 

663 

1.4 i 

■ 668 

0.7 

2817 [ 

0.2 

12 

4148 

4.4 

663 

6.8 i 

i 668 

4.0 , 

2817 ' 

1.0 

13 

4147 

12.9 

662 

15.0 

1 668 

14.2 

2817 

3.1 

14 

4143 

25.6 

659 

28.2 

667 

29.2 

2817 

j 6.0 

15 

4115 

36.8 

653 

42.7 i 

645 1 

40.6 

2817 

9.5 

16 

4047 

49.2 

636 f 

57.2 

595 ! 

53.4 

2816 

15.5 

17 

3939 

60.1 

599 1 

67.6 

’ 526 ’ 

65.6 

2814 

23.1 

18 

3769 

68.3 

575 , 

78.1 

456 

72.6 

2738 

30.9 

19 

3516 

73.9 

545 ' 

82.4 

397 ! 

78.3 

2574 

38.3 

20 

3206 

77.1 

517 

86.1 

350 : 

80.3 

2339 

45.7 

21 

2832 

80.5 

493 

88.4 

306 

83.7 

2033 

50.9 

22 

2430 

83.9 

474 

91.4 

284 

86.3 

1672 

58.3 

23 

2114 

86.5 

458 ; 

92.6 

259 

89.2 

1397 1 

63.0 

24 

1823 , 

88.6 

438 

95.0 

233 

90.1 

1152 : 

68.8 

25 

1637 ’ 

90.7 

418 

95.7 

217 ' 

91.7 

1002 

75.0 

26 

1494 , 

91.8 

407 

96.6 

203 

91.6 

884 

80.1 

27 

1359 ! 

93.6 

393 

97.5 

192 

93.8 

774 

82.6 

28 

1253 ' 

94.2 

379 i 

97.6 ' 

175 

94.3 

699 

85.1 

29 

1144 

94.7 

355 j 

97.5 

155 

94.8 

634 

87.1 

30 

1050 

95.2 

339 1 

97.9 

138 

94.2 

! 573 i 

89.5 

31 

973 

95,9 

319 ' 

98.1 

125 

95.2 

529 1 

90.9 

32 

915 

96.0 

307 

98.7 

116 

94.8 

492 i 

91.3 

33 ; 

856 

95.9 

295 

98.6 

113 

94.7 

: 448 j 

91.1 

34 

804 : 

96.5 

287 

99.0 

105 

95.2 

412 ‘ 

92.5 

35 

747 

97.4 

273 

99.3 

92 

95.7 

382 

93.2 

36 

703 

97.9 

260 

99.6 

87 

96.6 

356 

93.8 

37 

641 

98.5 

242 

99.6 

76 

98.7 

■ 323 

93.5 

38 

611 

98.6 

234 

99.6 

70 

98.6 

307 ' 

94.1 

39 

556 

98.6 

212 

^.5 

64 

98.4 

280 

95.4 

40 

509 

98.8 

194 

99.5 

58 

98.3 

257 

96.1 

41 

474 

99.3 

183 

99.5 

53 

100.0 

238 

96.2 

42 

445 

99.2 

174 

99.4 

50 

100.0 

221 

95.9 

43 

399 

99.1 

159 

99.4 



192 

95.3 

44 

369 

99.1 

146 

99.3 



177 

95.5 

45 

340 

99.0 

135 

99.3 

^ 


161 

95.0 


Table 137, Accumulative incidence data on total heterosexual intercourse 

Covering the life span, including pre-marital, marital, extra-^marital, and post-marital 
coitus with companions and with prostitutes. In three educational levels, and in the 
total population corrected for the U. S. Census of 1940. 


MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


567 


The percentage of the total outlet which the married male derives from 
intercourse with his spouse varies considerably with different social levels. 
For the lower level group it provides 80 per cent of the outlet during the 
early years of marriage, but an increasing proportion of the outlet as the 
marriage continues (Table 97). By 50 years of age the lower level male is 
deriving 90 per cent of his outlet from marital intercourse. On the other 
hand, males of the college level derive a larger proportion of their outlet 
(85%) from intercourse with their wives during the early years of marriage, 
but a smaller proportion of their outlet in later years. Not more than 62 per 
cent of the upper level male’s outlet is derived from marital intercourse by the 
age of 55. At no time in their lives do college-bred males depend on marital 
intercourse to the extent that lower level males do throughout most of their 
marriages. 



Figure 150. All intercourse: accumulative incidence in three educational levels 


Showing percent of total population that has ever had experience in intercourse of 
any kind, by each of the indicated ages. All data based on total population, irrespective 
of marital status, and corrected for the U. S. Census distribution. 


These data will surprise most persons because there seems to have been 
very little comprehension that marital intercourse provided anything less 
than the total outlet for married males at all levels. Several scientific and 
sociologic investigations have been based on the assumption that a study of 
marital intercourse was the equivalent of studying the sexual lives of at 
least the married portion of the population. This accords, of course, with 
the emphasis placed in Anglo-American ethical systems on marital inter- 
course as the goal of all sexual development; although there are some cul- 
tures in which a history of sexuality would be primarily a history of non- 
marital sexual activities. 

The general opinion that males become increasingly interested in extra- 
marital relations as they grow older, thus proves to be true only of the up- 



568 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


per level male. The explanation of these differences between upper and 
lower educational levels is not iimnediately available. 

It is possible that the increased frequencies of extra-marital intercourse 
among older males of the upper level are based on a conclusion that the 
early restraints on their sexual lives were not justified, and on an interest in 
securing extra-marital experience before old age has interfered with their 
capacities to do so. It sometimes happens that the decrease in the frequency 
of marital intercourse at this upper level is due to an increasing dissatisfac- 
tion with the relations which are had with restrained upper level wives. 
There are some who will ascribe the decrease in marital intercourse to the 
preoccupation of the educated male with the professional or business af- 
fairs of his life; but this explanation does not account for the fact that he 
finds a third of his total sexual outlet through channels other than marital 
intercourse. Moreover, it is to be emphasized that 19 per cent of the total 
outlet of these older males is derived neither from their wives, nor from 
extra-marital intercourse, nor from homosexual relations, but from such 
solitary activities as masturbation and nocturnal emissions. 

If we note that marital intercourse does not supply the whole of the out- 
let of married males, it is even more important to note that it does not sup- 
ply even half of the outlet of the male population taken as a whole (Figure 
127). Only 60 per cent of the white American males are married at any 
particular time (Table 1 1 extended into older age groups, and U. S. Census 
1940). Calculating from the age distribution of the total population, and 
from the mean frequencies of total outlet in each age group (Table 44), it 
develops that there are, on an average, 231 orgasms per week per hundred 
males between adolescence and old age. Calculating the orgasms secured 
in marital intercourse in each age group (Table 56), and correcting for the 
incidence of married males in the total population (Table 1 1), there prove 
to be, on an average, 106 orgasms per week which are derived from coitus 
with spouses, per hundred males of the total population (single and mar- 
ried). This means that only 45.9 per cent of the total outlet of the total 
population is derived from marital intercourse. 

Thus it will be seen that marital intercourse, although it is the most im- 
portant single source of sexual outlet, does not provide even half of the 
total number of orgasms experienced by the males in our American popu- 
lation. Allowing for the socially and legally accepted 5 or 6 per cent of the 
outlet which is secured from nocturnal emissions (Figure 126), it is to be 
concluded that approximately half of the sexual outlet of the total male 
population is being secured from sources which are socially disapproved 
and in large part illegal and punishable under the criminal codes. Marital 
intercourse, important as it is in the lives of most of the population, falls 
far short of constituting the whole of the sexual history of the American 
male. 



MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


569 


FREQUENCIES 

In the population as a whole, and in all of its subdivisions, the highest 
frequencies of marital intercourse occur in the youngest age groups. Males 
who are married between 16 and 20 start with frequencies which average 
3.9 for the population as a whole (Table 56), and many individuals at that 
age have intercourse on an average of 5, 7, 10 or more times per week. 
There is considerable individual variation, and the 15 per cent of the group 
who are capable of multiple orgasm (Table 48, Figure 36) may regularly 
secure 14, 21, or more climaces per week from intercourse with their wives. 
Frequencies drop steadily from the teens to about 2.9 at age 30, to 1.8 at 
age 50, and to 0.9 at age 60. Among all the calculations in the present study, 
there is none which falls along straighter lines. 

At the high school level the beginning frequencies of marital intercourse 
may be as high as 4.1 per week; but here, too, the frequencies drop to 
about 2.9 per week by age 30, and to similarly low rates at 60 years of age 
(Table 88). 

It is significant to find that the married males who have the highest total 
outlets, most of which depend upon high frequencies of marital intercourse, 
are, for every social level, those who became adolescent first (Table 71, 
Figure 90). Married males who became adolescent as early as 10 or 1 1 av- 
erage mean total outlets of 5 to 7 per week, if they are married during the 
age period 16 to 20, as against mean outlets of 3.3 per week for the married 
males who did not become adolescent until fifteen or later. It has already 
been pointed out (Chapter 9) that this indicates that the wife’s part in de- 
termining the frequency of marital intercourse is not as important as one 
might expect. The age at which the male became adolescent or, more strictly 
speaking, the general metabolic level which probably determines both the 
age of onset of adolescence and the intensity of a male’s sex drive, appears 
to be the prime factor in fixing the frequency of marital intercourse. 

There are no significant differences between older and younger genera- 
tions in the frequencies of their marital intercourse, in the same age pe- 
riods. 

Contrary to previous suggestions (Pearl 1925), frequencies of marital 
intercourse prove to be slightly but consistently lower among rural males 
than they are among city-bred males (Table 122), if corrections are made 
for age and social level in making such comparisons. This is in accord with 
the observation (Chapter 12) that the rural male has fewer chances to 
make socio-sexual contacts of any sort, and is more inept in making sexual 
advances even to his wife after marriage. It is also possible that there is more 
religious restraint on sexual activity among rural groups. 

It is significant to find that frequencies of marital intercourse are lower 
among religiously active Protestants and higher among inactive Protestants 




Figxire 151. Marital intercourse: individual variation in frequencies, at ages 
21-25 and 26-30, for three educational levels 

Showing percent of each population (vertical line) which has marital intercourse 
with each type of frequency (horizontal line). 

570 




MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


571 


(Table 130). The differences may amount to as much as 20 or 30 per cent. 
There are not sufficient data on Catholic or Jewish marriages to warrant 
any statements for those groups. The data on the Protestant groups are, 
however, particularly interesting because the restraints which the church has 
placed upon pre-marital relations, upon extra-marital relations, and upon 
all other types of sexual activity outside of marital intercourse, are justified 
by the explanation that the whole of one’s emotional and overt sexual life 
should be developed around one lifelong partner in marriage. It would ap- 
pear, however, that the effect of inhibitions on pre-marital sexual activity 
are carried over into inhibitions upon coitus with the married partner. 
Psychologically, this is quite what might have been expected. 

Beyond these several factors which affect the frequencies of marital in- 
tercourse, the restraint of the wife constantly lowers the frequencies in all 
segments of the population, but chiefly among better educated groups. A 
great many husbands wish their coitus were more frequent, and believe it 
would be if their wives were more interested. That this may be an expression 
of fact is peculiarly corroborated (Chapter 4) by the large number of wives 
who report that they consider their coital frequencies already too high and 
wish that their husbands did not desire intercourse so often. A very few 
wives wish for more frequent coitus; only a very few husbands wish their 
wives were not so desirous. 

These differences in interest inevitably cause difficulties in marital ad- 
justment, and there is no sexual factor which causes more difficulty at upper 
social levels. The situation depends upon basic differences in the sexual 
characteristics of males and females throughout the class Mammaha, and 
it should be realized that they do not arise in the perversities of the par- 
ticular individuals who happen to be united in a particular marriage. If 
clinicians are to provide the maximum help in individual instances of mar- 
ital maladjustment, it is fundamental that we learn as much as possible 
about the diverse origins of the sexual responses of males and females 
among higher animals in general, and among human males and females in 
particular. A considerable body of data on these points will be reported in 
subsequent volumes from the present research. 

COITAL TECHNIQUES IN MARRIAGE 

Individual variations that occur in the frequencies of marital intercourse 
are matched and possibly exceeded by variations in the techniques em- 
ployed in that intercourse. There are differences in the extent and the tech- 
niques of the pre-coital play. There are differences in the positions em- 
ployed in the intercourse. There are differences in the duration of the coital 
unions. There are differences in the desire for nudity or for clothing during 
intercourse. There are differences in preferences for light or dark. There 
are differences in places and circumstances under which the intercourse is 



SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


m 

had. There are some experimental individuals who may be involved in 
group activities in connection with their marital intercourse. 

There has been an insistence under our English- American codes that the 
simpler and more direct a sexual relation, the more completely it is con- 
fined to genital coitus, and the less the variation which enters into the per- 
formance of the act, the more acceptable the relationship is morally. This 
is the basis of much of our sex law, of a large portion of the sexual mores, 
and of the lower level’s frequent avoidance of any variety in sexual rela- 
tions — particularly if those relations are had with marital partners (Chap- 
ter 10). 

On the other hand, the educated portion of the population, especially 
within more recent generations, includes a good many persons who feel 
that any sort of activity which contributes to the significance of an emo- 
tional relationship between spouses is justified, and that no sort of sexual 
act is perverse if it so contributes to the marital relationship, even though 
exactly the same act between two persons who were not spouses might be 
considered a perversion. Even persons in high church positions have de- 
fended this thesis, with restrictions in certain cases that variety is accept- 
able only when the techniques are not an end in themselves, but a means of 
increasing the possibility and probability of conception as an outcome of 
the marriage. In the last two decades, marriage manuals have more or less 
uniformly emphasized the value of variety in coital techniques, and have 
probably encouraged an increasing proportion of the population to experi- 
ment. It is to be noted, however, that the English- American common law 
under which our courts still operate, and the specific statutes of our several 
states, make no distinctions between acts that are “contrary to nature” in 
marriage, and acts that are “contrary to nature” outside of marriage. 

There is, however, a considerable portion of the population, totalling 
perhaps a half or more of all persons, which is not interested in prolonging 
a sexual relationship. This is true, for the most part, of the more poorly 
educated portions of the population, although there are not a few upper 
level individuals who react similarly. It is a mistake to assume that a so- 
phistication of techniques would be equally significant to all persons. For 
most of the population, the satisfaction to be secured in orgasm is the goal 
of the sexual act, and the more quickly that satisfaction is attained, the 
more effective the performance is judged to be. These attitudes among 
lower educational levels may depend upon their generally lower imagina- 
tive and emotional capacities, but they are probably as dependent upon a 
sexual philosophy which makes any departure from the direct union of gen- 
italia a perversion. 

Extent of Petting. Pre-coital petting is limited in many of the lower level 
histories to the most perfunctory sort of body contact, or to a single kiss or 
two. In some cases even that much show of affection may be omitted. When 



MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


573 


this occurs at upper levels it is usually assumed to indicate some lack of af- 
fection, but it is unwarranted to make such an assumption for the great 
body of the population which regularly limits its pre-coital play (Chapter 
10). The average college-bred male is more likely to extend the pre-coital 
petting for a matter of five to fifteen minutes or more. Some individuals, 
especially younger persons in recent generations, may extend the pre-coital 
play regularly to a half hour, or to an hour or more — occasionally for sev- 
eral hours — before attempting coitus. In such a case the petting becomes 
the chief source of the satisfaction in the relationship, and the orgasm in 
which the activity finally culminates becomes significant as the climax, 
rather than as the whole of the relationship. 

Mouth Stimulation. If it is understood that sexual stimulation and re- 
sponse may involve, and usually do involve, a major portion of the nervous 
system (Chapter 5), and not merely that portion of the system which is lo- 
cated in or connected with the genitalia, it will be seen that any area of the 
body which is abundantly supphed with end organs of touch may become 
a center for erotic arousal and response. Such “erogenous zones” are most 
prominent on the lips, in the interior of the mouth and on the tongue, on 
the breasts of certain individuals, on certain portions of the genitalia of 
both males and females, and sometimes in the anal area. 

There is, however, no part of the surface of the human body which may 
not be a source of sexual stimulation and response, for there is no appre- 
ciable area of the skin which is without end organs of touch. For different 
individuals, the erotic responsiveness of different areas may vary, depend- 
ing in part on the psychologic conditioning of the individual as a result of 
his previous experience, but probably as often depending upon differences 
in the innervation of the same area in different individuals. The statement 
has been made by some psychiatrists that there is probably no portion of 
the body which could not be made an erotic area if there were sufficient 
psychologic conditioning for that area. While this is doubtful as applied to 
all individuals, our record shows that there is no part of the human body 
which is not sufficiently sensitive to effect erotic arousal and even orgasm 
for at least some individuals in the population. 

While the genitalia include the areas that are most often involved in sex- 
ual stimulation and response, it is a mistake to think of the genitaha as the 
only “sex organs,” and a considerable error to consider a stimulation or 
response which involves any other area as biologically abnormal, unnat- 
ural, contrary to nature, and perverse. Mouth, breast, anal, or other stimu- 
lations involve the same nervous system (namely the whole nervous system) 
which is involved in a genital response, and the arousal and orgasm which 
are effected byiStimulation of the other areas involve the same physiology 
(as far as we yet understand them) which is involved in arousal and orgasm 
ejffected through the stimulation of genital areas. That this is not generally 



574 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


understood is due to the considerable taboo in our culture on all non- 
genital sexual activity. As already noted (Chapters 10, 16), the lower mam- 
mals, unrestricted by social convention, know and utilize oral and anal 
stimulations as well as genital (Beach 1947); and even the most restrained 
of the human animals give evidence of their positive response by blocking 
and becoming violently upset at the mere suggestion of such activities. The 
violence of our social and legal condemnations of these phenomena is tes- 
timony to the psychologist and to the biologist that it is a basic biologic 
urge that is being repressed. The “sophisticate” who utilizes non-genital 
stimulations is, like the “sophisticate” who accepts nudity in a sexual rela- 
tion, returning to basic mammalian patterns of behavior (Chapter 6). 

In actuality, as American custom goes, genital stimulation is most often 
utilized, but mouth stimulation is involved to at least some degree among 
a considerable portion of the males and, to a somewhat lesser degree, 
among the females in the population. That the full possibilities of deep 
mouth and tongue stimulation are not more often utilized, is a measure of 
the extent to which the cultural restraints have modified human sexual be- 
havior. 

At upper social levels lip kissing is an almost invariable concomitant (in 
99.6%) of heterosexual relations (Table 93). At this level, there is a con- 
siderable amount of kissing as a show of affection between spouses through- 
out the day’s activities, and it is inevitable that the kissing should be still 
further extended in actual sexual relations. Kissing is involved both as an 
element in the pre-coital play, and as an accompaniment to actual coitus. 
At lower social levels taboos on all oral contacts are much stronger (Chap- 
ters 10, 16), and even simple lip kissing is reduced to a minimum. While it 
does occur in many (96%) of the lower level histories (Table 93), it is usu- 
ally limited in amount. 

Deep kissing, which may involve contacts of the inner lips, tongue con- 
tacts, and the stimulation of the interior of the partner’s mouth, is a fre- 
quent element (in 87%) of the pre-coital contacts at upper levels; but only 
a smaller portion (55%) of the lower social level engages in such activities 
(Table 93). Among less restrained couples deep kissing is a simultaneous 
accompaniment of actual coitus, especially at the moment of orgasm. In 
such groups mouth eroticism is developed to a considerable degree, and it 
may be fully as significant as the actual union of genitalia, or even more 
significant in effecting arousal to the point of climax. 

Breast Stimulation, Manual and oral manipulations of the female breast 
are elaborated to a considerable degree among many persons of upper 
social levels (Table 93). There is at least incidental touching of the female 
breast in nearly all (99%) of the upper level histories, and ima considerable 
proportion (85%) of the lower level histories. Oral manipulations of the 
female breast occur in 93 per cent of the upper level histories, but in only 



MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


575 


63 per cent of the histories of the married males who belong to the high 
school level, and in only 36 per cent of the histories of the married males 
who have never gone beyond grade school. 

Breast manipulation of the sort in which the upper level engages is a 
source of considerable arousal to the male who provides the manipulation. 
There is reason to believe that more males in our culture are psychically 
aroused by contemplation of the female breast than by the sight of female 
genitalia. In the light of this fact, it is interesting to observe the lengths to 
which censors and law'enforcement agencies go to prohibit the exhibition 
of genitalia, although they frequently allow the display of the nude female 
breast. How much of the American male’s interest in female breasts is cul- 
tural, and how much of it is biologically based, would be an interesting 
matter to investigate, especially in view of the frequent display of breasts 
among primitive peoples elsewhere in the world. 

There are many females who find some specific arousal in breast stimu- 
lation, but there may be even more who are not particularly aroused by 
breast contacts. Only a few females, perhaps not more than a few percent, 
are ever brought to orgasm by breast stimulation unaccompanied by 
genital contacts. 

It is important to note that females rarely attempt to manipulate male 
breasts. This may be due to the greater prominence of the female breast 
and to the wider knowledge of its eroticism. Conversely it may be due to 
the lesser prominence of the male breast and to the general lack of knowl- 
edge of its erotic capacities. It may also be due to the fact that the female 
is generally less responsive than the male erotically, and for that reason as 
well as because of social custom less often takes the initiative in any sex 
play. At any rate, most males whose experiences are confined to the hetero- 
sexual have never had their breast eroticism tested, and it has, therefore, 
been impossible to obtain data on the percentage in the population as a 
whole who have particularly sensitive breasts. Among males with extensive 
homosexual histories, however, breast manipulation is fairly frequent and 
it is commonly known in such groups that many males have highly sensi- 
tive breasts. The data from such cases indicate that there may be as many 
males as there are females who are sensitive to breast stimulation. 

Genital Stimulation, Manual. Hand manipulation of the female genitaha 
by the male occurs at least incidentally in all social levels, but its elabora- 
tion is more characteristic of the better educated groups (Table 93). With 
the anatomic information supplied by current marriage manuals, most 
upper level males have become aware of the existence of the clitoris in the 
female, and studied techniques of stimulation of the vulva in general and 
of the chtoris in particular are becoming more frequent accompaniments 
of upper level coitus (in 95%). In such groups it is now generally believed 
(again, in consequence of marriage manual instruction), that the female 



576 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


should be aroused to a considerable height before there is an actual union 
of genitalia. The biologic and psychologic desirability of this is something 
that needs further study; but the fact remains that upper level males of 
younger generations often operate on this theory. 

The most common error which the male makes concerning female sexu- 
ality is the assumption that stimulation of the interior of the vagina is 
necessary to bring maximum satisfaction to the female. This is obviously 
based upon the fact that vaginal insertion of the penis during coitus may 
result in orgasm for the female. It is a considerable question, however, how 
significant the stimulation of the interior of the vagina may be. It is certain 
that most of the physical stimulation which the female receives from actual 
coitus comes from contact of the external areas of the vulva, of the areas 
immediately inside the outer edges of the labia, and of the clitoris, with the 
pubic area of the male during genital union. 

There is a great deal of anatomic and clinical evidence that most of the 
interior of the vagina is without nerves. A considerable amount of surgery 
may be performed inside the vagina without need for anesthetics. Nerves 
have been demonstrated inside the vagina only in an area in the anterior 
wall, proximate to the base of the clitoris. There is need for much further 
research in this field, especially because there is a widespread but certainly 
unfounded opinion among psychiatrists, which is repeated among other 
clinicians involved in marriage counseling, that there is such a thing as a 
vaginal orgasm which is something different from an orgasm achieved 
through clitoral stimulation. The whole question will be reviewed in de- 
tail in our subsequent volume on the female. 

Beyond the occasional consciousness of stimulation of this limited vag- 
inal area which is known to have nerves, the female may be conscious of 
the intrusion of an object into the vagina, particularly if vaginal muscles 
are tightened; but the satisfaction so obtained is probably related more to 
muscle tonus than it is to erotic nerve stimulation. This interpretation is 
confirmed by the fact that there are exceedingly few females who mastur- 
bate by inserting objects into the vagina, and most of them who do so are 
novices, exhibitionistic prostitutes, or women who have had such pro- 
cedures recommended to them by male clinicians. Most of the female 
masturbatory techniques are labial or, more often, clitoral. A high pro- 
portion of the female homosexual relations similarly depend upon stimu- 
lating the vulva or the clitoris. The male who attempts to simulate coital 
intromission in his petting techniques is probably not so effective as the 
male who depends primarily on external stimulation of the genital labia, 
or of the clitoris. 

Genital Stimulation, Oral. In marital relations, oral stimulation of male 
or female genitalia occurs in about 60 per cent of the histories of persons 
who have been to college, although it is in only about 20 per cent of the 



MARITAL INTERCOLTRSE 


577 


histories of the high school level and in 1 1 per cent of the histories of the 
grade school level (Table 94). Because of the long-standing taboos in our 
culture on mouth-genital activity, it is quite probable that there has been 
more cover-up on this point than on most others in the present study, and 
the above figures must, therefore, represent minimum incidences. In nearly 
all of the upper level histories which involve oral contacts the males make 
contacts with the female genitalia. In about 47 per cent of the histories, the 
females make similar contacts with the male genitalia. The frequencies of 
such contacts range from a single experimental instance to regular and 
abundant elaborations of oral techniques in connection with nearly every 
sexual relation. 

Since an appreciable portion of the male homosexual contacts, and some 
part of the female homosexual contacts, may involve mouth-genital tech- 
niques, oral activities between males and females have sometimes been 
considered “homosexual.” There is, of course, no scientific justification for 
such a use of the term, and an analysis of oral contacts in the heterosexual 
does not show any homosexual element involved. It is the basic, oral eroti- 
cism of the mammal which is concerned in all mouth-genital relations, 
whatever the sex of the partner. 

The English-American common law and most of the American written 
codes condemn all mouth-genital contacts, whether they occur between 
partners of the same sex, or between partners of the opposite sex, and 
whether they occur within marriage or outside of marriage. While the laws 
are more commonly enforced in regard to such relations outside of mar- 
riage, there are instances of spouses whose oral activities became known to 
their children and through them to the neighborhood, and ultimately led 
to prosecution and penal sentences for both husband and wife. Because of 
the taboos in this country, not even psychiatrists have comprehended the 
considerable incidence of such relations among married partners. Such ac- 
tivities have been more freely discussed in certain European cultures, and 
they have, of course, been recorded from every culture in the history of the 
world, including the most ancient from which there are documentary or 
pictorial records, pottery, or other materials (from Greece, Rome, India, 
China, Japan, Peru, Bali, etc.). 

Because of the widespread taboos on the subject, the contemplation of 
participation in oral-genital activities often results in blocked emotional re- 
sponses which erupt in bitter condemnations of the partner who initiated 
the activity, and sometimes produce alimentary peristalsis resulting in nau- 
sea or diarrhea. This is, of course, the clearest sort of evidence that the 
affected individuaFs initial responses were positive, for it demands a block- 
age of a definite reaction to produce such a violent disturbance. The male, 
with his higher level of sexual responsiveness, is the one who is more often 
interested in making oral contacts, and it is the wife who is more often 



578 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


offended. This may lead to guilt feelings on the part of both of the partners. 
The refusal of the wife to accept such contacts, or the husband’s hesitancy 
to risk his wife’s refusal, may lead some upper level males to seek oral con- 
tacts with prostitutes. 

There is a not inconsiderable list of histories in which dissension over 
oral relations has caused serious disagreements in marriage, and a fair 
number of divorces have revolved around this question, although the con- 
testing partners rarely disclose the real source of their difficulty when they 
come to court action. There are several instances of wives who have mur- 
dered their husbands because they insisted on mouth-genital contacts. Un- 
fortunately, marriage counselors, clinical psychologists, and psychiatrists 
have not known enough about the basic biology of these contacts, nor 
enough about the actual frequencies of such behavior in the population, to 
be able to help their patients as often as they might, and they have not been 
able to supply courts with adequate scientific data when such cases have 
come up. The clinician who advises a patient that oral contacts are rare 
and abnormal and that they constitute sexual perversions is merely epit- 
omizing the mores. He is not supplying scientific data. On the other hand, 
the clinician who freely advises acceptance of such contacts must not over- 
look the deep emotional values which are rooted in the long-time customs 
of our society, and which for many persons are prime factors in determining 
their individual behavior. 

A list of the social problems which most often arise out of human sexual 
activity would give first places to venereal disease, bastardy, rape, and the 
contribution by adults to the delinquency of minor children. On the other 
hand, personal conflicts most often develop over masturbation, oral con- 
tacts, and the homosexual. These are the three that need especial help — 
not because they are rare, but because they are widespread, and because 
nearly every male in the population is at one time or other involved in one 
or more of them. These are the three that are most often encountered by 
the clinician, not because men are frequently abnormal or recently become 
perverse, but because all three of these are part of the basic biologic pattern 
of mammalian sexual behavior, and because no legislation or social taboos 
have been able to eliminate them from the history of the human animal. 

Positions in Intercourse. As previously indicated (Chapter 10), nearly all 
coitus in our English-American culture occurs with the partners lying face 
to face, with the male above the female. There may be as much as 70 per 
cent of the population (estimated from Table 95) which has never at- 
tempted to use any other position in intercourse. It is the better educated 
portions of the population which experiment with other positions most 
frequently. Only about half as many persons of the grade school level ever 
depart from the one position which they consider most natural. We have 
pointed out (Chapter 10) that other positions are, from any biologic stand- 



MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


579 


point, more natural, and that the standardization of a particular position 
in our society is the product of cultural forces which more often control the 
behavior of lower levels, less often of upper levels. 

The incidences and frequencies with which variant positions are em- 
ployed are shown in Table 95, where it will be observed that the second 
most common position is the one in which the female is above, facing the 
male; and among most persons who have used it, this position is found to 
be the one which most often results in orgasm for the female. Sitting po- 
sitions, standing positions, and rear entrance into the vagina as the female 
lies face down or kneels are much rarer in American patterns. Variety in 
coital position is regularly suggested by marriage manuals, but once again 
it is the male who is most often interested in experimenting. 

Anal Eroticism. There are some individuals for whom anal stimulation 
is definitely erotic, and there are a few who may be brought to orgasm by 
such stimulation. 

The mechanisms involved in such responses are the same as those which 
account for erotic response to oral, breast, or genital stimulation, and there 
is no need for special theories to explain anal reactions. It would appear, 
however, that there is considerable variation in these reactions, probably 
due to differences in the nerve supply in different individuals, as well as to 
considerable differences in psychologic conditioning on this point. There 
is some anal play in some of the marital histories, usually as an additional 
source of stimulation during vaginal coitus; and there is an occasional in- 
stance of anal coitus. However, anal activity in the heterosexual is not 
frequent enough to make it possible to determine the incidence of indi- 
viduals who are specifically responsive to such stimulation. Among males 
who have been stimulated anally in the homosexual, there are only a few 
who are particularly aroused, and only an occasional individual who is 
brought to orgasm by such techniques. 

Speed of Male Orgasm. There may be a considerable amount of inter- 
course which is had without orgasm for the female, and some males may 
fail to reach orgasm in pre-marital or extra-marital coitus or m some other 
types of sexual activity; but failures to achieve climax are almost never 
found among married males in intercourse with their wives. 

Throughout the population it is customary for the male to reach a single 
orgasm and not to attempt to continue intercourse beyond that point. Ex- 
ceptions are found chiefly among younger married males who are still in 
their teens. At that age 15 per cent of the population is capable of experi- 
encing two or more ejaculations during a limited period of time and during 
continuous erotic activity (Table 48). The number of males who are capable 
of such multiple orgasm decreases with advancing age. Not more than 7 
per cent remain so capable by age 35. 



580 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


At lower educational levels, it is usual for the male to try to achieve an 
orgasm as soon as possible after effecting genital union. Upper level males 
more often attempt to delay orgasm. For perhaps three-quarters of all 
males, orgasm is reached within two minutes after the initiation of the 
sexual relation, and for a not inconsiderable number of males the climax 
may be reached within less than a minute or even within ten or twenty 
seconds after coital entrance. Occasionally a male may become so stimu- 
lated psychically or through physical petting that he ejaculates before he 
has effected genital union. 

This quick performance of the typical male may be most unsatisfactory 
to a wife who is inhibited or natively low in response, as many wives are; 
and such disparities in the speed of male and female response are frequent 
sources of marital conflict, especially among upper social levels where the 
female is most restrained in her behavior. Nevertheless, the idea that the 
male who responds quickly in a sexual relation is neurotic or otherwise 
pathologically involved is, in most cases, not justified scientifically. There 
are clinicians who insist that ejaculation should be considered premature if 
a male is incapable of delaying until the female is ready to reach orgasm. 
Considering the many upper level females who are so adversely conditioned 
to sexual situations that they may require ten to fifteen minutes of the 
most careful stimulation to bring them to climax, and considering the fair 
number of females who never come to climax in their whole lives, it is, of 
course, demanding that the male be quite abnormal in his ability to pro- 
long sexual activity without ejaculation if he is required to match the fe- 
male partner. 

Interpretations of human behavior would benefit if there were a more 
general understanding of basic mammahan behavior. On the present issue, 
for instance, it is to be emphasized that in many species of mammals the 
male ejaculates almost instantly upon intromission, and that this is true of 
man’s closest relatives among the primates. Students of sexual activity 
among chimpanzees, for instance, report that ten to twenty seconds is all 
the time which is ordinarily needed to effect ejaculation in that species. Far 
from being abnormal, the human male who is quick in his sexual response 
is quite normal among the mammals, and usual in his own species. It is 
curious that the term “impotence” should have ever been applied to such 
rapid response. It would be diflBcult to find another situation in which an 
individual who was quick and intense in his responses was labeled anything 
but superior, and that in most instances is exactly what the rapidly ejacu- 
lating male probably is, however inconvenient and unfortunate his quaU- 
ties may be from the standpoint of the wife in the relationship. 

A portion of the upper level males do deliberately learn to delay ejacu- 
latiouj and it is probable that most males could learn to control urethral 



MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


581 


convulsions, primarily through a tightening of anal muscles, so they could 
prolong sexual activity before orgasm. But it is only a portion of the male 
population that would consider the acquirement of such an ability as a 
desirable substitute for direct and rapidly elfected intercourse. 

In the female, variations in the speed with which orgasm is achieved are 
much greater than in the male. These variations in the female will be con- 
sidered in a later volume. 

Nudity. It may again be noted that 90 per cent of the upper level males 
have intercourse without clothing (Table 95), and still others would prefer 
it so, if circumstances allowed. The female is more often inhibited on this 
point than the male, but at upper social levels she usually comes to accept 
this as the normal accompaniment of coitus. Not more than 43 per cent of 
the grade school level ever has intercourse without clothing, for nudity is 
more commonly taboo at those levels. There is evidence (Table 95) that 
nudity has been accepted by upper level groups more freely within more 
recent generations. This is one point in human sexual behavior to which ar- 
guments as to what is natural and what is unnatural have never been ap- 
plied, for there can be no question of the fact that intercourse without 
clothing is biologically normal, and that the custom of having intercourse 
with clothing is a distinctly cultural acquirement. But the upper social 
level returns to what is biologically normal behavior only after a consid- 
erable rationalization and a reasoned break with the mores. 

Preferences for Light or Dark. Among married partners, there may be 
considerable differences in preferences for having intercourse in full light, 
in subdued light, or in the dark. In general, more males prefer to have in- 
tercourse in the light, and more females prefer it in the dark. Such differ- 
ences may be ascribed to different levels of “modesty” in the two sexes, 
but the basic explanation probably lies much deeper. These preferences for 
Ught or dark are closely correlated with differences between males and fe- 
males in the erotic significances of objects that are visually observed. 

Most males, particularly in upper segments of the population, are defi- 
nitely aroused upon seeing things that are associated with sex, and most 
females are not so aroused. To have intercourse in the light increases the 
sources of erotic stimulation for most males, and means very little erotically 
to most females. In consequence, moral considerations of the sort that are 
associated with modesty may very well control the female in her behavior, 
but they do not mean so much to the male. It is probable that these differ- 
ences between the sexes are, again, dependent upon basic differences in the 
neural organizations which are involved in sexual responses (Beach? 1947). 
These matters must have more elaborate consideration in a later volume 
on the female, and always the nature of the patterns that are basic among 



Chapter 19 


EXTRA-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 

In the history of most human cultures, extra-marital intercourse has 
more often been a matter for regulation than has intercourse before mar- 
riage. Frequently this has taken the form of denying the married female 
intercourse with anyone except her husband; less often it has included a 
restriction of the male’s right to have intercourse outside of his home. 
While various issues have been involved, such regulations have been par- 
ticularly concerned with the property rights which the male has had in 
his wife, and there is no question that the extra-marital activities of the 
female became objects of concern in such early codes as the Babylonian 
(Harper 1904), Hittite (Barton 1925), Assyrian (Barton 1925), Jewish 
(Bible, Talmud), and others, because of these property rights, rather than 
because moral issues were recognized. 

In so-called primitive groups in various parts of the world today much 
the same distinctions are made between intercourse before and after mar- 
riage, and between the male’s right and the female’s right to have such re- 
lations (Malinowski 1929, Hartland 1931, Thurnwald 1931, Wissler 1922, 
Fortune 1932, Murdock 1934, Blackwood 1935, Linton 1936, Lips 1938, 
Reichard 1938, Mead 1939, Schapera 1941, Chappie and Coon 1942, Bryk 
1944, Ford 1945). Similar distinctions have been made throughout the 
history of Western European civilization, and the rights of the male in the 
female who is married to him have been a basic part of English and 
American law. It is only within the last few decades that material changes 
have been effected in this country in legal viewpoints on the relationship 
of the husband and wife. 

In line with these ancient distinctions, there are still various segments of 
our population in this country today which more or less freely accept pre- 
marital relations, while objecting strenuously to extra-marital intercourse. 
In fact it may be said that there is no segment of our American population 
which, as a whole, really accepts extra-marital activities in anything like 
the way that masturbation is accepted at upper social levels, or in the way 
that pre-marital intercourse is accepted at lower social levels. In some seg- 
ments of the population, relatively little attention is paid to the pre-marital 
intercourse which occurs among young people; but at all social levels, ex- 
tra-marital intercourse is a subject for gossip — often malicious gossip— 
often for peremptory and outraged community reaction, and quite often 
for legal penalties. The offended spouse who takes the law into his or her 

583 



584 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


own hands, and assaults and even murders the competitor in the sexual 
relation, is, in many parts of the country today, likely to be backed by a 
certain amount of public sympathy. Juries are loath to convict in such 
cases. It does not alter the fact that society knows that extra-marital inter- 
course does occur, and that it occurs with some frequency; and it seems not 
to matter that it is generally known that such intercourse usually goes un- 
punished. Society is still outraged when confronted with the specific case 
on which it is challenged to pass judgment. 

These social attitudes are particularly interesting in view of the fact that 
a considerable proportion of those who react most violently against the 
known instances of extra-marital relations, may have similar experience in 
their own histories. Nearly three-quarters (72%) of the males in the Terman 
study (Terman 1938) admitted that they wished on occasion to have extra- 
marital intercourse, and a similarly high proportion of the males in our 
present study have expressed the same desire. Furthermore, many of them 
actually have such extra-marital relations. The pretense that these persons 
make in defending the codes clearly indicates a conflict in their own minds 
concerning the social significance of such relations. If our society is ever to 
act more intelligently on these matters, it will need more factual data. Even 
the scant data that we can offer here may prove of some significance. 

REFERENCES 

Specific material on the occurrence of extra-marital intercourse in vari- 
ous age groups and in various social divisions of the population, has al- 
ready been given in tables and charts, and in earlier discussions in the 
present volume, as follows: 

PAGE TABLE HGURE NATURE OF DATA 

248, 257- 54, 64 71-76 Age, marital status, and non-marital intercourse with com- 
259, 281- pamons 

285 

250-253, 55, 65 77-82 Age, marital status, and intercourse with prostitutes 
281-289 

348, 354 85 103 Total extra-marital intercourse as related to social level 

350, 383 86 Extra-marital intercourse with companions, and social level 

351-354 87 Intercourse with prostitutes, as related to educational level 

456 120 Total non-marital intercourse and rural-urban background 

480-481 129 Total extra-marital intercourse and religious background 

382, 489, 97 127, Extra-marital intercourse as part of the total sexual outlet 

492-493 131-133 

INCIDENCES AND FREQUENCIES 

We have found a great many persons who would like to know how many 
males have extra-marital intercourse. Obviously this considerable interest 
depends upon the fact that most of the married males who ask the question 
have already had such experience, or would like to have it if they could 
reconcile it with their consciences and if it could be managed without in- 
volving them in legal difficulties or pubhc scandal. 



EXTRA-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


585 


At the same time, this considerable interest also indicates that many in- 
dividuals fear that their extra-marital histories may become known. In 
consequence, it has been peculiarly difficult, in the present study, to secure 
anything like adequate data on this aspect of human sexual activity. There 
is probably nothing in the histories of older married males who belong to 
better educational and social levels that has more often been responsible 
for their refusal to contribute to the present research. Many of the persons 
who have contributed only after some months or years of refusal to do so, 
prove to have nothing in their histories that would explain their original 
hesitancy except their extra-marital intercourse. Even those who have con- 
tributed more readily have probably covered up on this more often than 
on any other single item. We have reason for believing that most of the 
persons who have criticized the adequacy of the present study, on the 
ground that they were able to go through a history “without telling every- 
thing,” were individuals who failed to record their own extra-marital ex- 
perience. Considering that the legal penalties for such sexual activity are 
rarely enforced, and that most males feel that such activity is highly de- 
sirable and not exactly wrong, it is particularly interesting to observe this 
considerable disturbance over the issue. Only the fear of the social (as op- 
posed to the legal) consequences can explain this reticence about extra- 
marital sexual performance. 

It has so far been impossible to secure hundred percent samples from 
men of the type that belongs to business organizations, business executive 
groups, and service clubs; and we have every reason for believing that 
extra-marital intercourse is the source of the hesitance of many of the in- 
dividuals in such groups to cooperate. Consequently, the incidence and 
frequency figures which are given here must represent the absolute mini- 
mum, and it is not at all improbable that the actuality may lie 10 to 20 
per cent above the figures now given. 

Hamilton (1929) found 28 per cent of his hundred men with records of 
extra-marital intercourse. His figure would have been higher if he had dealt 
with older men. In the present study, something over a third (27% to 37%) 
of the married males in each of the five-year age periods have admitted 
some experience in extra-marital intercourse (Table 64, Figure 73). Since 
these are active incidence data, the accumulative figure must amount to 
something more than that. Because of the inadequacy of the record it has 
been impossible to construct accumulative incidence curves by the usual 
techniques (Chapter 3), and we can only estimate from these active inci- 
dence figures. 

On the basis of these active data, and allowing for the cover-up that has 
been involved, it is probably safe to suggest that about half of all the mar- 
ried males have intercourse with women other than their wives, at some 
time while they are married. 



AGE GROUP * 21-25 
EOUC LEVEL 0-8 



Figure 152. Total extra-marital intercourse: individual variation in frequencies, 
at ages 21-25 and 26-30, for three educational levels 

Showing percent of each population (vertical line) which has extra-marital intercourse 
with each type of frequency (horizontal line). 

586 




EXTRA-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


587 


About 40 per cent of the high school and college males have admitted 
extra-marital relations (Tables 85, 86). For the grade school group, a higher 
percentage of the younger males have given such records, but only a smaller 
percentage (19%) of the older males. There are several possible explana- 
tions of this discrepancy, but there is so much likelihood of cover-up here 
that the question cannot be resolved at this time. 

The most striking thing about the occurrence of extra-marital intercourse 
is the fact that the highest incidences for the lower social levels occur at the 
younger ages, and that the number of persons involved steadily decreases 
with advancing age (Table 85). Lower level males who were married in the 
late teens have given a record of extra-marital intercourse in 45 per cent of 
the cases, whereas not more than 27 per cent is actively involved by age 40 
and not more than 19 per cent by age 50. In striking contrast, the lowest 
incidences of extra-marital intercourse among males of the college level 
are to be found in the youngest age groups, where not more than 15 to 20 
per cent is involved, and the incidence increases steadily until about 27 
per cent is having extra-marital relations by age 50. 

Similarly, the highest frequencies of extra-marital intercourse are to be 
found among the younger males of the lower educational levels, but the 
frequencies drop steadily with advancing age (Tables 85, 97). Between 16 
and 20, males of the lower educational level who are actually involved in 
extra-marital intercourse average such contacts more than once (1.2) per 
week; but by age 55 the frequencies have dropped to hardly more than once 
in two weeks (0.6 per week). On the other hand, college males of the active 
population begin with frequencies of a little more than once in two or three 
weeks between the ages of 16 and 30, but finally arrive at frequencies that 
are nearer once a week by the time they are 50. 

We have previously suggested (Chapters 10, 18) possible explanations for 
these diverse patterns in the extra-marital intercourse of different social 
levels, of which explanations the most likely is the fact that lower level 
males have an abundance of pre-marital intercourse, and there is some 
carry-over of that type of promiscuity after marriage. On the other hand, 
upper level individuals are the ones with the most restrained pre-marital 
histories, and they lose that restraint only gradually and do not so often 
embark on extra-marital relations until later years. We cannot explain why 
there should be a cessation of extra-marital activity among so many older 
males of the lower level. It cannot be entirely due to their generally poorer 
physical condition at older ages, for it is the percent of their total outlet 
from extra-marital relations which has dropped, from an original of 12 per 
cent to 6 per cent. Meanwhile, among males of the college level, the percent 
of the total outlet which is derived from extra-marital intercourse has in- 
creased from an original 3 per cent in the earlier years to 14 per cent by age 
50 (Table 97). 



588 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Between 16 and 20, married males of the grade school level have 10.6 
times as much extra-marital intercourse as males of the college level. To 
make another comparison, married males of occupational classes 2 and 3 
(the laborers and semi-skilled workmen) have 16.7 times as much extra- 
marital intercourse during their late teens as rhales of occupational class 7 
(the future professional group). 

For most males, at every social level, extra-marital intercourse is usually 
sporadic, occurring on an occasion or two with this female, a few times with 
the next partner, not happening again for some months or a year or two, 
but then occurring several times or every night for a week or even for a 
month or more, after which that particular affair is abruptly stopped. The 
averaged data may show mean frequencies of once a week or two, but the 
whole of the year’s total is likely to have been accumulated on a single trip 
or in a few weeks of the summer vacation. There are extreme instances of 
younger males whose orgasms, achieved in extra-marital relations, have 
averaged as many as eighteen per week for periods of as long as five years; 
but these are unusual cases. Lower level males are the ones who are most 
likely to have more regularly distributed experience, often with some vari- 
ety of females. Among males of the college level extra-marital relations 
are almost always infrequent, often with not more than one or two or a 
very few partners in all of their lives, and usually with a single partner over 
a period of some time — in some cases for a number of years. 

Extra-marital intercourse occasionally accounts for a fair portion of the 
outlet of the married males of certain segments of the population. It ac- 
counts for 1 1 per cent of the outlet of married males of the grade school 
level during their late teens (Table 86). But more often it is a smaller part 
of the total picture. It ultimately accounts for something between 5 and 10 
per cent of the total orgasms of all the married males in the population 
(Table 64). 

Prostitutes supply something between 8 and 15 per cent of all extra- 
marital intercourse (Chapter 20). Obviously, most of the extra-marital ac- 
tivities are had with companions. For lower level males, these may be semi- 
professional pick-ups, but are often married women of their own class. For 
the upper level males, the contacts may be had with females of any social 
level, but many of them are had with their own social level. 

Extra-marital intercourse occurs most frequently among males who live 
in cities or towns; less frequently in rural populations (Table 120). At the 
grade school level, the number of urban males involved may be 20 to 60 per 
cent higher than the number of rural males who are having extra-marital 
experience, and the frequencies in this lower educational level are higher 
among urban males, especially in the early twenties. Among the college- 
bred males, the city-raised individuals are involved two or three times as 



EXTRA-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


589 


often as the rural males, but the frequencies seem to be higher in the rural 
group. 

To judge from those few groups on which religious data are available 
(Table 129), extra-marital intercourse seems to occur much more frequently 
among those who are less actively concerned with the church, and much 
less frequently among males who are devoutly religious. The differences 
between devout and inactive members of any religious group are, however, 
nowhere near so great as the differences between social levels. The com- 
munity acceptance or non-acceptance of extra-marital intercourse is much 
more effective than the immediate restraints provided by the present-day 
religious organization. But since the sex mores originated in religious codes 
(Chapter 13), it is, in the last analysis, the church which is the origin of the 
restrictions on extra-marital intercourse. 

RELATION TO OTHER OUTLETS 

There seems to be no question but that the human male would be pro- 
miscuous in his choice of sexual partners throughout the whole of his life 
if there were no social restrictions. This is the history of his anthropoid an- 
cestors, and this is the history of unrestrained human males everywhere. 
The human male almost invariably becomes promiscuous as soon as he be- 
comes involved in sexual relations that are outside of the law. This is true 
to a degree in pre-marital and in extra-marital intercourse, and it is true 
of those who are most involved in homosexual activities. 

The human female is much less interested in a variety of partners. This 
is true in her pre-marital and extra-marital histories and, again, it is strik- 
ingly true in her homosexual relations. The easy explanation that the fe- 
male is basically more moral, and the male less moral, does not suffice. 
These differences must be more dependent upon differences in the sexual 
responsiveness of males and females, and particularly upon differences in 
the conditionability of the two sexes. The average female is not aroused by 
nearly so many stimuli as is the male, and finds much less sexual excite- 
ment in psychic associations or in any sensory stimulations outside of the 
purely tactile. These differences are similar to those found between males 
and females in the lower mammals, and there is a good deal of evidence 
(Beach 1947) that they depend upon differences in the nervous organization 
on which sexual behavior depends. 

In practical terms this means that there are a great many human females 
who find it incomprehensible that so many human males should look for 
sexual relations with women other than their wives. On the other hand, 
most males see some force to the argument that variety is attractive in any 
sort of situation, whether it concerns the literature that one reads, the music 
that one hears, the recreation in which one engages, the food which one 
eats, the type of sex relations which one has, or the sexual partners with 



590 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


whom the relations are had. This philosophy has been frankly expressed by 
a considerable number of males who have contributed histories to the 
present study, even though some of them immediately add that for moral 
and social reasons they have not had extra-marital intercourse and will not 
have extra-marital intercourse, however much they may desire it. 

There is, of course, a smaller portion of the females, the number of 
whom we have not yet calculated, who find variety in sexual relations as 
interesting as any of the males find it. 

Extra-marital intercourse, then, may occur irrespective of the availability 
or frequency of other sorts of sexual outlet, and without respect to the satis- 
factory or unsatisfactory nature of the sexual relations at home. Most of 
the male’s extra-marital activity is undoubtedly a product of his interest in 
a variety of experience. On the other hand, there is certainly a portion of 
his extra-marital intercourse which is the product of unsatisfactory rela- 
tions with his wife. When she fails to be interested in sexual relations with 
her husband, when she is less interested than he is, when she refuses to have 
intercourse as frequently as he would like it, when she refuses to allow the 
variety in pre-coital techniques that the male would like to have, or when 
she accedes to such techniques without evidencing an interest equal to that 
of the male, she is encouraging him to find extra-marital relations. The 
wife’s refusal of mouth-genital contacts (Chapter 18) with her husband is 
a factor in sending some males elsewhere for such experience. 

All of these same factors may, of course, operate to lead a sexually re- 
sponsive wife into extra-marital intercourse; but that is not so often true 
as is the reverse situation. 

It is not yet clear how much relation there is between experience in pre- 
marital intercourse and experience in extra-marital intercourse. Exact cor- 
relations will have to be published later. Certainly^ there are histories of 
males who had an abundance of pre-marital intercourse and who never 
have any sort of extra-marital intercourse; and there are histories of males 
who had no pre-marital intercourse but who begin a considerable amount 
of extra-marital intercourse as soon as they are married. There are histories 
of males who are examples of every other type of relationship between these 
two phenomena. A multiplicity of factors must be involved, and it will take 
careful analyses to identify what correlations may exist. 

It is true, as just noted, that males of the lower educational levels are the 
ones who have the most pre-marital intercourse, and they are the ones who 
have the most extra-marital intercourse in early marriage. And it is also 
true that the college level males have the least pre-marital intercourse, and 
the least extra-marital intercourse in early marriage. But the correlations 
he in the basic attitudes of the social groups which are involved, and they 
are not a direct effect of pre-marital behavior on extra-marital patterns. 



EXTRA-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


591 


SOCIAL SIGNIFICANCE 

Throughout the literature of the world, extra-marital intercourse has 
provided an overwhelming abundance of material for biography, drama, 
fiction, and serious essay. There is probably no sexual theme that has ap- 
peared more often in the world’s literature, both great and small, in all ages 
and among all nations. Most often the relationships have been portrayed 
as highly desirable, intrinsically sinful, certain of obstruction by a con- 
spiracy of social forces, and doomed to tragic failure which becomes most 
tragic when it appears that the illicit relations would have been the higher 
destiny if social conventions had not interfered. That extra-marital rela- 
tions are generally desired has, evidently, been known to all men through- 
out the ages. That they seldom work out in society as it is constructed has 
been at least believed by the writers of all ages. 

Current sociologic, clinical, sex educational, and religious literature re- 
peats, for the most part, this conviction that extra-marital intercourse al- 
ways does damage to marriages (e.g,, Armitage 1913, Forel 1922, Meyer 
1927, Eddy 1928, Hamilton 1929, Lindsey and Evans 1929, Amer. Soc. 
Hyg. Assoc. 1930, Rice 1933, 1946, Ruland and Rattler 1934, Robinson 
1936, Ellis 1936, Clark 1937, Benjamin 1939, Popenoe 1943, Rockwood 
and Ford 1945, Seward 1946). In this literature, the judgment against extra- 
marital intercourse is alihost uniform. Only an occasional writer suggests 
that there may be values in such experience which can be utilized for human 
needs. 

The public record is replete with instances of marital infidelities which 
have wrecked homes and destroyed individuals. The counselor and clin- 
ician see a stream of cases in which marital diflaculties turn around the 
extra-marital activities of the husband or of the wife. The scientist can add 
httle that is new in the record of such cases, and he cannot minimize their 
significance in our social organization. 

There is, nevertheless, room for a scientific examination of the real bases 
of the difficulties that develop out of extra-marital sexual relations. Is it 
inevitable that extra-marital intercourse should lead to difficulties, or do 
the difficulties originate in the mores of the group? What proportion of all 
extra-marital relations lead to marital disturbances? The publicly known 
clinical cases may, like chnical cases of other sorts, represent only the dis- 
turbed segment of the group that has extra-marital experience, and may 
not adequately represent the situation as a whole. Do extra-marital rela- 
tions ever contribute to the effectiveness of a marriage? What effect does 
such activity ultimately have upon the personalities of those who are in- 
volved? Certainly society may be concerned with securing objective an- 
swers to these questions. 

In gathering the thousands of married histories which have entered into 
the present study, we have begun the accumulation of a considerable body 
20 



592 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


of material on the factors that contribute to marital stability and, con- 
versely, to marital discord. With the further continuation of this study, 
analyses of these data will be undertaken in a later publication. For the 
present, only fragments can be offered that bear on the social significance 
of extra-marital intercourse. 

Certainly many different sorts of situations are involved. There are many 
factors that may affect the outcome of the extra-marital activities, and the 
record is much more diverse than has generally been believed. 

At lower social levels, where the most extra-marital intercourse occurs, 
wives rather generally expect their husbands to “step out,” and some of 
them rather frankly admit that they do not object provided they do not 
learn of the specific affairs which are carried on. Nevertheless, extra- 
marital intercourse is the sexual factor which is most often involved in 
marital discord at that level. Diversion of the interest and affection of the 
spouse who has the extra-marital relation results in jealousy and bitter 
hatreds, and these lead to endless quarrels and vicious fighting with, oc- 
casionally, murder as the outcome. A portion of the non-support which is 
so common at this level is the result of the male’s distraction by females 
other than his wife. Desertions, separations, and divorce, in this group, 
are frequently the outcome of these extra-marital attractions. 

Nevertheless, a portion of the extra-marital intercourse at lower levels 
is had without apparent interference with the affection between the spouses, 
or with the stability of the marriage. The data are as yet insufficient to war- 
rant a statistical measure of the frequency of each type of situation. 

Extra-marital intercourse is less often accepted in middle class groups. 
While it may not involve as much quarreling and fighting, it often leads to 
divorce. How often it occurs without causing trouble is a matter that still 
needs to be determined. 

The extra-marital intercourse of the upper social level much less often 
causes difficulty, because it is usually unknown to anyone except the two 
persons immediately involved. On occasion it does become known and 
causes marital discord and divorce. On the other hand, it is sometimes had 
with the knowledge of the other spouse who may even aid and encourage 
the arrangement. Such a frank and open acceptance of the partner’s non- 
marital sexual relations is practically unknown at lower social levels, and 
at all levels is a source of astonishment to persons with strict moral codes. 

Wives, at every social level, more often accept the non-marital activities 
of their husbands. Husbands are much less inclined to accept the non- 
marital activities of their wives. It has been so since the dawn of history. 
The biology and psychology of this difference need more careful analysis 
than the available data yet afford. 



EXTRA-MARITAL INTERCOURSE 


593 


The significance of extra-marital intercourse may more often depend 
upon the attitudes of the spouses and of the social groups to which they 
belong, than upon the effect of the actual intercourse upon the participat- 
ing individuals. Few difficulties develop out of extra-marital intercourse 
when the relationships are unknown to anyone but the two persons having 
the intercourse. There are histories of long-continued extra-marital rela- 
tionships which seem to have interfered in no way with the marriages, until 
the other partner or partners discovered the infidelity. Then they immedi- 
ately filed suit for divorce. 

Extra-marital intercourse most often causes difficulty when it involves 
emotional and affectional relations with the new partner who takes prec- 
edence over the spouse. Conversely, the extra-marital contacts most often 
avoid trouble when they are social alfairs without too much emotional con- 
tent. There are a few males who can carry on emotional relationships with 
two or more partners simultaneously, but there are many more who do not 
succeed at such an arrangement. 

There are some individuals among our histories whose sexual adjust- 
ments in marriage have undoubtedly been helped by extra-marital experi- 
ence. Sometimes this depends upon their learning new techniques or ac- 
quiring new attitudes which reduce inhibitions in their marital relations. 
Some women who have had difficulty in reaching orgasm with their hus- 
bands, find the novelty of the situation with another male stimulating them 
to their first orgasm; and with this as a background they make better ad- 
justments with their husbands. Extra-marital intercourse has had the effect 
of convincing some males that the relationships with their wives were more 
satisfactory than they had realized. 

There are a few cases of married couples who have ceased sexual rela- 
tions with each other, but who maintain happy and socially successful 
homes while each of the partners finds the whole of his or her sexual outlet 
outside of marriage. There are cases of males who are totally impotent with 
their wives, although they are successful in extra-marital relations which 
they may carry on throughout the whole of their marital histories, while 
the wives similarly maintain Hfelong relations with men other than their 
husbands. 

In both lower and upper level histories, there are cases where the children 
in the home are the offspring of the extra-marital relationships. Both 
spouses may accept the situation, and it may cause no difficulty as long as 
the neighbors and the law are unaware of the fact. 

The histories of persons born and raised in Continental Europe usually 
involve a great deal of extra-marital intercourse, and such histories should 
be carefully studied in any scientific analysis of the outcome of such rela- 
tions. 



594 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


By and large, it is not a large proportion of the population that accepts 
an unlimited amount of extra-marital intercourse. Even those individuals 
who publicly defend the desirability of such relationships usually have nota- 
bly few experiences in their actual histories. Whether this is a tribute to the 
effectiveness of the mores in controlling the behavior of persons who think 
that they are emancipated, or whether it is evidence that extra-marital in- 
tercourse entails difficulties that they did not anticipate, or whether it 
merely indicates that successful extra-marital relations are carried on with 
difficulty under our present social organization, it is impossible to say at 
this time. Certainly the psychologist and social scientist, and society in 
general, need a great many more specific data before there can be any final 
evaluation of the effects of extra-marital intercourse on individuals and on 
their relations to their homes and to the society of which they are a part. 



Chapter 20 

INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 


By sociological and legal definition a prostitute is an individual who 
indiscriminately provides sexual relations in return for money payments. 
The practical interpretation of the term emphasizes the fact that a prostitute 
accepts a sexual relation with almost anyone, stranger or acquaintance, who 
offers to pay, and that the payment is in currency rather than in goods 
or services. 

It is impractical to confine the term to those persons who derive their 
whole living or any particular part of it from prostitution, for a very high 
number of the females who engage in such activities do so as a minor 
adjunct to their regular occupations. The person who is specifically paid 
for a single sexual relation is, for that particular occasion, a prostitute. 

The definition requires that payment for a sexual relation be in currency 
and be made for each particular contact. If the term prostitution were to 
be applied to all sexual acts for which either participant received some 
valuable consideration, it would be impossible to draw a line between the 
most obvious sort of commercialized prostitution and the relationships of 
every husband and wife. The girl who has to be taken to dinner or to an 
evening’s entertainment before she will agree to intercourse with her boy 
friend or fiance is engaged in a more commercialized relationship than she 
would like to admit. The gifts that are bestowed by males of all social 
levels upon girls with whom they keep company may be cloaked with fine 
sentiments, but they are, to a considerable degree, payment for the inter- 
course that is expected. 

At lower social levels there is often an elaborate arrangement by which 
the girl in the pre-marital or extra-marital relation is provided with stock- 
ings, dresses, fur coats, and other materials of value, for sexual relation^ 
which would be immediately stopped if the male failed to provide such 
gifts as the girl considered commensurate with her contribution, and with 
his ability to pay. In some lower levels it is quite customary for the male 
to share his pay envelope with the girl from whom he is securing regular 
intercourse, and at all social levels there may be some sort of regular 
contribution to the support of the girl’s home, if the sexual relation is 
continued over any period of time. It is difficult to characterize such rela- 
tionships as prostitution. It is even more misleading to apply the term to the 
case of the wife who demands payment in coin of the realm each time she 
engages in intercourse with her husband (and such arrangements are 

595 



596 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


recorded in the histories). But it is only rarely that there is any difficulty 
in recognizing the situations that do deserve to be called prostitution. 

There are four types of prostitution. The commonest involves hetero- 
sexual relations for which the female is paid. This is the type of prostitution 
with which the present chapter is concerned. 

There is, however, a homosexual prostitution among males who provide 
sexual relations for other males; and such homosexual prostitutes are, in 
many large cities, not far inferior in number to the females who are engaged 
in heterosexual prostitution. Male homosexual prostitutes less often derive 
their main income from such activities, and less often engage in prostitu- 
tion for any long period of years. 

There is also a heterosexual prostitution in which females pay males for 
sexual relations, but this situation is not common. 

The rarest of the four types of prostitution involves females who are 
paid for the homosexual relations with which they supply other females. 

Any extensive treatment of the subject should cover all four types of 
prostitution, and should analyze the basic elements in all of them. But 
the present chapter is not concerned with prostitution as a social institu- 
tion, nor with prostitutes. Those subjects will provide the material for a 
later volume. The present chapter deals only with the behavior of the males 
who pay female prostitutes for a portion of their sexual outlet. 

REFERENCES 

Data on the part which intercourse with prostitutes plays in the sexual 
hves of males in various segments of the population have already been 
detailed in this volume in tables and charts, and in discussions in the text, 
as follows : 


PAGE 

67, 68 

TABLE 

HGURE 

NATURE OF DATA 

Items on prostitution covered in interview 

116,117 

12 

14 

Calculation of accumulative incidence curve 
on coitus with prostitutes 

234 

49 


Range of variation in intercourse with pros- 
titutes 

251,253 

55, 65 

77-82 

Age and marital status affecting intercourse 
with prostitutes 

353-357 

87, 113, 115 

102, 106, 154 

Relation of social level to intercourse with 
prostitutes 

404,413 

100, 104, 105 

113,114,122, 123 

Comparisons of two generations, in inter- 
course with prostitutes 

455 

121 

125 

Rural-urban backgrounds and intercourse 
with prostitutes 

488-493 

96, 97 

127-133 

Portion of total outlet derived from pros- 
titutes 

597-600 

138 

153, 154 

Accumulative incidence of intercourse with 
prostitutes 

601-603 


155 

Individual variation in frequencies of inter- 
course with prostitutes 



INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 


597 


INCIDENCES AND FREQUENCIES 

There is a widespread opinion, both in the public at large and among 
social scientists, that prostitution provides the major source of non-marital 
sexual outlet for most of the male population. However, though it may 
play a more important part in the sexual patterns of some other countries, 
in the United States the number of males who go to prostitutes is not so 
high as is generally believed, and the frequencies with which they go are 
very much lower than almost anyone has realized. 

There have been very few attempts to obtain statistical data on the 
incidence of contacts with prostitutes (Eddy 1928, Taylor 1934, Reitman 
in Robinson 1936, Bromley and Britten 1938), and these data have been 
inadequate. Similarly, it would appear that the frequencies of such contacts 
in any large segment of the population have never been investigated 
(although there are reports on isolated cases), and this is astounding in 
view of the tremendous interest that so many agencies have had in con- 
trolling the frequencies of such contacts. Law enforcement officers, the 
reports of vice societies, popular sex books and pamphlets, novels, and 
even the best of the literature written in this country have made prostitu- 
tion appear much more significant than it actually proves to be in the 
total sexual life of the American male. 

We find that about 69 per cent of the total white male population ulti- 
mately has some experience with prostitutes (Table 138, Figure 153). 
Many of these males, however, never have more than a single experience 
or two, and not more than 15 or 20 per cent of them ever have such rela- 
tions more often than a few times a year, over as much as a five-year period 
in their lives. This means that there is nearly a third (31%) of the popula- 
tion that never has any sort of sexual contact with prostitutes. There are, 
of course, a few males who never have heterosexual relations except with 
prostitutes, but this happens very rarely. 

Ultimately, something between 3.5 and 4 per cent of the total outlet of 
the total male population (single and married) is drawn from relations 
with female prostitutes (Tables 96, 97, Figures 126, 127). This is not a 
very large portion of the total outlet. Nocturnal emissions are more im- 
portant (Chapter 15), and the homosexual accounts for two or three times 
as many orgasms among males (Chapter21). Only petting to climax (Chap- 
ter 16) and animal intercourse (Chapter 22) account for smaller parts of 
the outlet. In the college group, where intercourse with prostitutes is at its 
lowest, the homosexual may provide ten to twenty times as much of the 
outlet as prostitutes do. Many groups interested in controlling non-marital 
sexual activities have centered their attention upon prostitution when, in 
actuality, it accounts for less than a tenth of the non-marital outlet of the 
male population. Intercourse with prostitutes is much more important 
socially than it is as a means of outlet. 



598 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Total Intercourse with Prostitutes: Accumulative Incidence 


AGE 

1 

TOTAL POPULATION 

U. S. CORRECTIONS 

EDUC. LEVEL 

0-8 

EDUC. LEVEL 

9-12 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13 + 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

3995 

0.0 

685 

0.0 

494 

0.0 

2816 

0.0 

9 

3995 

0.0 

685 

0.0 

494 

0.0 

2816 

0.0 

10 

3995 

0.0 

685 

0.0 1 

494 

0.0 

2816 

0.0 

11 

3994 

0.0 

684 

0.1 

494 

0.0 

2816 

0.0 

12 

3994 

0.3 

684 

0.6 

494 

0.2 

2816 

0.0 

13 

3994 

0.7 

684 

1.0 

494 

0.6 

2816 

O.l 

14 

3991 

2.6 

681 

3.5 

494 

2.6 

2816 

0.8 

15 

3985 

7.4 

675 

7.7 

494 

8.5 

2816 

2.3 

16 

3964 

16.1 

658 

18.4 

491 

17.7 

2815 

4.8 

17 

3905 

26 0 

621 

28.8 

471 

28.9 

2813 

9.1 

18 

3767 

36.7 

594 

40.6 

436 

40.6 

2737 

13.6 

19 

3536 

42.1 

564 

45.6 

399 

46.6 

2573 

17.4 

20 

3231 

45.3 

536 

50.9 

357 

48.5 

2338 

20.6 

21 

2857 

48.6 

512 

54.7 

313 

52.1 

2032 

22.2 

22 

2454 

51.4 

492 

58.3 

290 

54.5 

1672 

24.9 

23 

2137 

54.5 

476 

59.5 

264 

59.1 

1397 

25.7 

24 

1845 

55.9 

456 

61.6 

237 

60.3 

1152 

26,6 

25 

1659 

58.6 

436 

65,1 

221 

62.9 

1002 

28.6 

26 

1515 

59.9 

424 

66.5 

207 

64.3 

884 

29.4 

27 

1379 

61.2 

409 

67.5 

196 

65.8 

774 

30.5 

28 

1273 

63.3 

395 

67.6 

179 

69.8 

699 

32.0 

29 

1164 i 

64.0 

371 

67.7 

159 

71.1 

634 

32.0 

30 

1069 

65.5 

355 

69.6 

141 

73.0 

573 

33.2 

31 

993 

66.1 

335 

70.1 

129 

73.6 

i 529 

34.0 

32 

934 

65.9 

323 

70.9 

119 

72.3 

492 

33.7 

33 

873 

66.4 

309 

71.2 

116 

73.3 

448 

33.3 

34 

819 

66.7 

300 

72.0 

107 

72.9 

412 

33.7 

35 

762 

68.0 

286 

72,7 

94 

73.4 

382 

34.6 

36 

718 

70.0 

273 

74.4 

89 

76.4 

356 

35.7 

37 

656 

69.8 

255 

73.7 

78 

76.9 

323 

35.6 

38 

625 

69.6 

246 

73.6 

72 

76.4 

307 

36.2 

39 

570 

68.2 

224 

72.3 

66 

74.2 

280 

36.4 

40 

521 

68.8 

204 

73.5 

60 

71.7 

257 

36.6 

41 

485 

68.9 

192 

74.0 

55 

70.9 

238 

37.0 

42 

456 

68.1 

183 

73,8 

52 

69.2 

221 

35.3 

43 

409 

67.2 

167 

73.1 

50 

68.0 

192 

34.4 

•44 

379 

66.8 

154 

72.7 



177 

36.2 

45 

348 

68.7 

141 

73.0 



161 

38.5 


Table 138. Accumulative incidence data on total intercourse with prostitutes 

Covering the life span, including pre-marital, extra-marital, and post-marital histories. 
In three educational levels, and in the total population corrected for the U. S. Census 
of 1940. 



INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 


599 


Among single males, the percentage of the total outlet derived from 
contacts with prostitutes increases markedly with age (Table 65, Figure 
78), beginning at 3.7 per cent in the late teens, rising to nearly 10 per cent 
by age 30, and going still higher for those relatively few males who are 
still unmarried in the later years. For married males (Table 65, Figure 78), 
hardly more than 1 per cent of the outlet is derived from extra-marital 
intercourse with prostitutes, and this lowers the average for all males, 
single and married. 

Prostitutes provide only about a tenth of the male’s total pre-marital 
intercourse: 8.6 per cent between ages 16 and 20, 13.3 per cent between 



AGE 


Figure 153. Intercourse with prostitutes: accumulative incidence in total U. S. 

population 

Showing percent of total population that has ever had intercourse with prostitutes 
by each of the indicated ages. All data based on total population, irrespective of marital 
status, and corrected for the U. S. Census distribution. 

21 and 25, and even more of the pre-marital intercourse of the males who 
are still unmarried at later ages (Tables 64, 65). It is to be noted that an 
increasing proportion of the extra-marital intercourse comes from prosti- 
tutes as the male grows older. This is partly due to his decreasing abihty 
to find sexual partners, particularly partners of attractive, younger ages. 
It is also due, however, to the fact that the older male finally reaches the 
point where he considers it simpler to go to prostitutes than to try to court 
and win the favors of a girl who is not a prostitute. 

In the same fashion, and for the same reasons, intercourse with prostitutes 
supplies an increasing proportion of the outlet of the males who have been 
previously married, but who are now widowed, separated from their wives, 
or divorced (Table 65, Figure 78). 


600 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Among married males, prostitutes provide about 11 per cent of the 
extra-marital outlet between ages 16 and 20, over 16 per cent of that outlet 
by age 30, and 22 per cent of the extra-marital outlet at age 55 (Tables 64, 
65). This apparent increase, however, is not due to any increase in actual 
frequencies, but to the fact that the total outlet drops steadily through 
the years, while intercourse with prostitutes is maintained with more or 
less constant frequencies over a period of several decades. 

The incidence and frequency figures vary tremendously for different 
segments of the population, and it is misleading to discuss the place of 
prostitution in the population as a whole. Contacts with prostitutes are 
most frequently had by males of the lowest social levels. By 25 years of 



A6E 


Figure 154. Intercourse with prostitutes: accumulative incidence in three educa- 

^onal levels 

Showing percent of each population that has ever had intercourse with prostitutes 
by each of the indicated ages. All data based on total population, irrespective of marital 
status. 

age, 74 per cent of the males who never went beyond grade school have 
had some intercourse with prostitutes (Table 87), while only 54 per cent 
of the males of the high school level, and only 28 per cent of the males of 
the college level, have had such experience. Among single males of the 
group that never goes beyond the eighth grade, as much as 6 per cent of 
the total sexual outlet is derived from prostitutes in the late teens, 14.3 per 
cent by the late twenties, and 23.4 per cent by the late thirties, if the male 
is not yet married by that time (Table 96). Among the boys who go to high 
school the figures start at 3 per cent in the late teens and climb to 10.3 per 
cent in the middle thirties. For males of the college level less tbap l per 
cent of the total sexual outlet is derived from prostitutes in the late teens, 
and only 3 per cent in the late twenties. This is one of the most striking 
differences between the patterns of college males and the patterns of all 
other groups. 



INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 


601 


Among all married males, it is never more than 1.7 per cent of the total 
sexual outlet which is derived from prostitutes in any particular age period 
(Table 97) and, again, it is the married male of the college level who draws 
the lowest percentage of his outlet from professional sources. 

The actual frequencies of intercourse with prostitutes begin at very 
low levels in the early adolescent years, but they do begin there. Law 
enforcement officers are especially interested in trying to prevent young 
boys from having such relations, and it is very difficult to get a prostitute 
to admit that she has ever had relations with any boy under 18. But 
nearly 8 per cent of the males who have contributed to the present study 
have reported that they had such relations before or by the time they were 
15 years of age. Frequencies of contacts steadily rise until^they average 
about once in three weeks (0.3 per week) for the total population of un- 
married males in their thirties (Table 65, Figure 77). For those males who 
actually have such relations, the frequencies start in the earliest adolescent 
years at once in four weeks and rise to twice in three weeks (0.6 per week) 
by the thirties. If these calculations are broken down by social levels 
(Table 87, Figure 102), the active frequencies for those boys who never 
go beyond the eighth grade in school start at about once in three weeks and 
rise to once in two weeks or twice in three weeks. For the boys who go to 
high school but not beyond, the frequencies (of the active population) 
start at some lower level and rise to once in four weeks by age 20, once in 
three weeks by age 25, and somewhat higher in the later years for the males 
who remain unmarried. For the males who belong to the college level, 
those who have any intercourse at all with prostitutes average only once 
in six to ten weeks, unless they remain unmarried past the age of 25, when 
the frequencies rise rather considerably. 

Between 16 and 20, males of the grade school level have intercourse with 
prostitutes 9 times as often, and males of the high school level have it more 
than 4 times as often as males of the college level (Table 87, Figure 102), 
By the early thirties, males of the grade school level have intercourse with 
prostitutes about 36 times as frequently as males of the college level. 
Making the comparisons by occupational classes, the record is that males 
of classes 2 and 3 (the laboring group and the semi-skilled workmen) and 
possibly class 4 (the skilled workmen) have 5 to 10 times as many contacts 
with prostitutes during their late teens as males of occupational class 7 
(the future professional group) (Table 113, Figure 102). The differences 
become even greater in later years. Public health officials who are inter- 
ested in controlling the spread of venereal disease might profitably give 
maximum attention to educating the groups which have the most frequent 
contacts with prostitutes and with other girls. 

For the population taken as a whole, including single and married 
males of all social levels and of all ages above adolescence, the mean 




Figure 155. Pre-marital intercourse with prostitutes: individual variation in 
frequencies, in four age groups, at three educational levels 

Showing percent of each population (vertical line) which has pre-marital intercourse 
with prostitutes with each type of frequency (horizontal line). 

602 



INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 


603 


average frequency of intercourse with prostitutes is 0.093 per week, which 
is a little less than 5 times per year. With this average figure, which has been 
very carefully calculated on the basis of all of our available data, it is 
possible to estimate the average number of contacts that are being made 
with prostitutes in any particular city or state, per week or per year. In- 
frequent as such contacts are in relation to the total sexual activity of the 
average male, they amount to a good deal in absolute frequencies in any 
population as a whole. 

In the total U. S. population 34.3 per cent (calculated from U. S. Census 
1940) consists of males who are above the age of onset of adolescence and 
under the age of impotence and, therefore, eligible for intercourse with 
prostitutes. The frequencies of contacts per million of total population 
should then total close to 1,659,000 per year. In terms of the town of 
100,000 inhabitants, the contacts average about 3,190 per week. If the 
police force in such a community fails to make that many arrests each 
week for association with prostitutes, this may be taken as a measure of 
the difficulty of facing the actuahties of human behavior. With such data, 
it should be possible to sense the magnitude of the problem of eliminating 
prostitution. And yet contacts with prostitutes, as just noted, represent 
only a small part of the non-marital (and therefore largely taboo or illegal) 
sexual activity of the community. 

In view of the efforts that have been made in the last decade or two to 
control heterosexual prostitution, it is important to note that the data 
show (Tabic 100, Figures 113, 114) that the percentage of males in each 
social level who are frequenting prostitutes today is almost precisely the 
same as the percentage which had such experience twenty or more years 
ago (Chapter 11). While there are considerable differences in frequencies 
for males of the different social levels which may live together in the same 
town, there are practically no differences between the males of two genera- 
tions which are as far apart as the two world wars. 

The frequencies of contacts with prostitutes have, however, been signif- 
icantly reduced, undoubtedly as a result of the educational campaigns and 
the legal moves which have more recently been made against prostitution. 
The present-day male is making such contacts only two-thirds, or even 
half, as often as the older generation did (Table 104). In compensation, 
however, there has been a deifinite increase in the amount of intercourse 
with girls who are not prostitutes, and the totals for pre-marital intercourse 
have not been materially changed. 

Moreover, prostitution does not now occupy the thinking of males as it 
did in past generations. Males of the older generation visited houses of 
prostitution, not only in search of intercourse, but on sightseeing trips and 
in social groups as well. They were more often involved in the non-sexual 
activities that occurred in the established houses, such as drinking, gam- 



604 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


bling, etc. Present day prostitution is more often a matter of dealing with 
an individual girl who operates on her own. In practically every large city 
in the United States, those who are acquainted with conditions can locate 
prostitutes easily enough; but since organized houses have been eliminated 
in most of these cities, the stranger in town may have considerable difficulty 
in making such contacts. It is our impression, which will need more statis- 
tical support before it is established, that the number of girls who are now 
involved in prostitution is not materially smaller than the number which 
was so engaged ten or twenty years ago. Their manner of operation, 
however, has been materially changed, and the number of contacts they 
make per week has been appreciably reduced. 

TECHNIQUES 

This is not the place to make any detailed study of the way in which 
prostitution is managed, how its contacts are made, and the types of rela- 
tions which it offers. It is appropriate in this volume, however, to note 
certain situations that arise when persons of diverse social levels, the 
prostitute and her client, undertake to have sexual relations together. 
Most prostitutes originate in lower social levels, from which they have 
acquired lower level patterns of behavior (Chapter 10). These lower level 
attitudes still persist even after a girl goes into prostitution. Some of her 
trade comes from her own social level, and then her own patterns of coitus 
suffice; but a large part of her trade is, as we have seen, from the high 
school level, and a smaller but financially very significant part of it comes 
from the college level. In their pre-coital and coital techniques, both the 
high school and college groups want something that is usually foreign to 
the prostitute’s background. For the sake of her trade, she may agree to 
such overt activity as these males desire but, interesting to note, she still 
would refuse to use such techniques with her husband or boy friend. 

Specifically, elaborated pre-coital petting, kissing, oral manipulation of 
the breast, and mouth-genital contacts are taboo in the level from which 
the girl comes (Chapter 10). The introduction of nudity in intercourse is 
often foreign to her previous experience. Variety in coital positions and, 
in fact, all techniques which involve anything except simple genital union 
may be, in her estimation, perversions, “freakish,” or “queer.” Even the 
prostitute who goes furthest in supplying variety for the upper level male, 
may never accept such things as normal or right. She may not be partic- 
ularly worried about her own behavior — that is a necessary part of the 
business — but she does not respect the male who wants such things. There 
are, of course, exceptions. A few of the lower level prostitutes consider 
that a male should be allowed any sort of sexual activity for which he 
pays, and a few prostitutes become erotically interested in the full variety 
of activities in which they engage. There are a few who are better educated, 
with high school or even college backgrounds ; and there are some who are 



INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 


605 


physically energetic, mentally alert, and intelligent. They more readily 
accept and provide the variety of techniques which the upper level males 
find most satisfactory, and some of these girls may develop long-time rela- 
tions which make them full-fiedged mistresses. 

The lower level prostitute more often learns to accept the high school or 
college male’s desire for nudity during intercourse, and his desire for inter- 
course in the light. These things are usual in relations with prostitutes 
today, although they were far from being so in past generations. In a 
certain number of cases, the prostitute may provide mouth-genital contacts. 

The cheaper prostitute restricts each sexual relation to a minimum of 
time. This, again, is part of the pattern of her social level and is, of course, 
more convenient for her. In the days of established houses, contacts 
were often limited to five minutes or less, and extra payment was 
demanded for any extension of activity. The educated male’s interest in 
protracted relationships which involve social contacts and pre-coital 
petting is likely to go unsatisfied with most prostitutes. Only the occasional 
girl in the more expensive house of prostitution, or the still more expensive 
partner maintained by the financially richer male, has been wdlling to 
make a prolonged social performance of each contact. 

As a result of the more recent drives against organized prostitution, 
many of the better houses have been closed. The poorest houses have been 
the ones that have most often survived. As noted previously, most prosti- 
tution has become a matter of an individual girl operating on her own. 
These girls often have no place to take thomen except to the back alley, to 
their own very poor homes, or to the cheap hotels to which they have en- 
trance. Consequently, the conditions under which the sex relations are now 
had are still less satisfactory to the upper level male than they were in the 
older generation. The sexually expert and socially more effective girl who 
remains in prostitution operates for very high fees and restricts her contacts 
to a very hmited clientele with which she has had long-time acquaintance. 

SIGNIFICANCE OF PROSTITUTION 

The world’s literature contains hundreds of volumes whose authors 
have attempted to assay the social significance of prostitution. For an 
activity which contributes no more than this does to the sexual outlet of 
the male- population, it is amazing that it should have been given such 
widespread consideration. Some of the attention which the subject has 
received, and certainly many of the books that have been written about 
it, have undoubtedly been inspired by erotic interest; but a major part 
of the interest has centered around this question of the social significance 
of prostitution. The extent of the attention which the subject still receives 
in this country today is, as we have shown, all out of proportion to its 
significance in the fives of most males, and this makes one skeptical of 



606 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


using the older literature as a source of information on the place of prosti- 
tution in past generations and past centuries. Certainly the older accounts 
would make it appear that prostitution was much more important in the 
life of the male who lived any time between the dawn of history and 
World War I than we have evidence of its having been since then. 

There has always been a considerable relation between prostitution and 
other underworld activities, including gambling, bootlegging, dope 
peddling, robbery, and other activities. A very high percentage of the 
prostitutes rob their clients whenever the opportunity affords. Often 
strong-arm robbery, assault, and occasionally murder are involved. These 
activities, more than the sexual relations themselves, have concerned law 
enforcement oflBcers and all others who have been interested in main- 
taining orderly communities. The relation of prostitution and venereal 
disease has supplied the argument most often used in recent decades for 
the suppression of organized prostitution (e,g., W. S. Hall 1907, 1909, 
Exner 1914, Bigelow 1916, Coppens and Spalding 1921, U. S. Public 
Health Service 1921, 1937, Forel 1922, Martindale 1925, Eddy 1928, 
Meyer 1929, Dickerson 1930, Weatherhead 1932, Rice 1933, Ruland and 
Rattler 1934, Ellis 1936, Robinson in Robinson 1936, Stone and Stone 
1937, Haire 1937, Clarke 1938, Rosanoff 1938, Crisp 1939, Kirkendall 
1940,*Snow 1941, Bowman 1942, Dickerson 1944, 1946, Koch and Wilbur 
1944, Popenoe 1946, McPartland 1947). This is not the place to discuss the 
scientific data which are available on these social problems. 

Throughout history, there have been few social institutions which have 
been objects of as continuous condemnation and concentrated attack as 
the institution of heterosexual prostitution; and this undoubtedly reflects 
a widespread judgment that there are basic faults in the institution. On the 
other hand, prostitution continues to exist, and one may well ask why men 
continue to go to prostitutes. It is probable that prostitution is no excep- 
tion to the economic laws, and it continues to exist because there is a 
sufficient demand for what it offers (see, for instance, Forel 1922, Weather- 
head 1932, Ellis 1936, Benjamin 1939, Faris in Hunt 1944, Popenoe 1944, 
Sadler and Sadler 1944). 

First of all, men go to prostitutes because they have insufficient sexual 
outlets in other directions, or because prostitution provides types of sexual 
activity which are not so readily available elsewhere. Many men go to 
prostitutes to find the variety that sexual experience with a new partner 
may offer.* Some men go because they feel that the danger of contracting 
venereal disease from a prostitute is actually less than it would be with a 
girl who was not in an organized house of prostitution. Some males 
experiment with prostitution just to discover what it means. In many cases 
some social psychology is involved as groups of males go together to look 
for prostitutes. 



INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 


607 


At all social levels men go to prostitutes because it is simpler to secure a 
sexual partner commercially than it is to secure a sexual partner by court- 
ing a girl who would not accept pay. Even at lower social levels, where 
most males find it remarkably simple to make frequent contacts with girls 
who are not prostitutes, there are still occasions when they desire inter- 
course immediately and find it much simpler to obtain it from a prostitute. 
As for college-bred males, a great majority of them are utterly ineffective 
in securing intercourse from any girl whom they have not dated for long 
periods of time and at considerable expense; and in some cases, their only 
chance to secure coital experience is with a prostitute. This is, of course, 
particularly true if the male is away from home in a strange town. 

j Hundreds of males have insisted that intercourse with a prostitute is 
cheaper than intercourse with any other girl. The cost of dating a girl, 
especially at the upper social level, may mount considerably through the 
weeks and months, or even years, that it may take to arrive at the first 
intercourse. There are flowers, candy, “coke dates,” dinner engagements, 
parties, evening entertainments, moving pictures, theatres, night clubs, 
dances, picnics, week-end house parties, car rides, longer trips, and all 
sorts of other expensive entertainment to be paid for, and gifts to be made 
to the girl on her birthday, at Christmas, and on innumerable other special 
occasions. Finally, after all this the girl may break off the whole affair as 
soon as she realizes that the male is interested in intercourse. Before the 
recent war the average cost of a sexual relation with a prostitute was one to 
five dollars. This was less than the cost of a single supper date with a girl 
who was not a prostitute; and even at the inflated prices of prostitution 
which prevailed during the war, the cost did not amount to more than 
many a soldier or sailor was obliged to spend on another girl from whom 
he might or might not be able to obtain the intercourse which he wanted. 

Men go to prostitutes because they can pay for the sexual relations and 
forget other responsibilities, whereas coitus with other girls may involve 
them socially and legally beyond anything which they care to undertake. 

Men go to prostitutes to obtain types of sexual activity which they are 
unable to obtain easily elsewhere. Few prostitutes offer any variety of 
sexual techniques, but many of them do provide mouth-genital contacts. 
The prostitute offers the readiest source of experience for the sadist or 
the masochist, and for persons who have developed associations with non- 
sexual objects (fetishes) which have come to have sexual significance for 
them because of some contact they have had in the past. Most males who 
have participated in sexual activities in groups have found the opportunity 
to do so with prostitutes. Nearly all of the opportunity that males have to 
observe sexual activity is connected with prostitutes, and such experiences 
are in the history of many more persons than is ordinarily realized. 



608 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Some men go to prostitutes because they are more or less ineffective in 
securing sexual relations with other girls. This may be true of males who 
are unusually timid. Persons who are deformed physically, deaf, blind, 
severely crippled, spastic, or otherwise handicapped, often have consider- 
able difficulty in finding heterosexual coitus. The matter may weigh heavily 
upon their minds and cause considerable psychic disturbance. There are 
instances where prostitutes have contributed to establishing these indi- 
viduals in their own self esteem by providing their first sexual contacts. 

Finally, at the lower social levels there are persons who are feeble- 
minded, physically deformed, and so repulsive and offensive physically 
that no girl except a prostitute would have intercourse with them. Without 
such outlets, these individuals would become even more serious social 
problems than they already are. 

The exclusively homosexual male, however, is not the person to be 
helped by a prostitute. There are numerous histories of such males being 
advised % clinicians, or led by some friend, or forced by some hilarious 
group of male companions into attempting intercourse with a prostitute. 
In a high proportion of such cases the male proves impotent, and his 
psychic problem is thereby intensified. Even when the intercourse is more 
or less successful, it is likely to prove distasteful because of the unesthetic 
conditions under which it is had. The introduction of the homosexual 
male to heterosexual experience should come through friendships which 
lead to affection and spontaneously erotic developments. 

There is constant rumor of an increase in the frequency of forced inter- 
course or outright rape among the girls of a community where prostitution 
has been suppressed. We have no adequate data to prove the truth or 
falsity of such reports. 

Neither are we convinced that there has been any sufficiently objective 
study of the place of prostitution in the spread of venereal disease, as 
compared with the spread of such disease through sexual contacts with 
lower level girls who are not prostitutes. 

The significance of prostitution to the male who goes to the prostitute 
must depend very much upon the sort of person who is involved and the 
social background from which he comes. At lower social levels there are 
some who find intercourse with prostitutes distasteful, but in a much larger 
number of cases there are no objections to the type of relation that is had. 
In not a few cases, the male insists that intercourse with a prostitute is 
superior to intercourse with most other girls. The lower level male is not 
particularly concerned with the responsiveness or unresponsiveness of his 
female partner, and he is not interested in a particularly emotional expe- 
rience in coitus, does not want any elaboration of pre-coital petting, and 
does not object esthetically to the sorts of situations under which most of 



INTERCOURSE WITH PROSTITUTES 


609 


the intercourse occurs. He likes a matter-of-fact performance in which there 
are no emotional and no social obligations incurred. Most often he prefers 
the prostitute, however, because she expects that there will be intercourse, 
and does not offer the objections that other girls, even his wife, may offer 
against sexual relations. 

On the other hand, the upper level males who have contributed to the 
present study have almost unanimously agreed that intercourse with a 
prostitute is not nearly so satisfactory as the intercourse which may be 
had with other girls. This is undoubtedly the prime reason why most upper 
level males do not return to prostitutes more often than they do. The 
complaints turn largely around the fact that a sexual relation which is 
commercialized lacks the affection which makes a sexual relation signifi- 
cant in marriage, or even in non-marital relations with girls who are not 
prostitutes. The upper level male dislikes the limitation on petting in his 
relations with prostitutes. He commonly complains about the genital 
inadequacies of the prostitute, and this in most instances means that she 
is not responding erotically. In consequence, she does not stimulate the 
emotionally sensitive, upper level male. There is a fair number of upper 
level males who find themselves impotent in attempting intercourse with 
prostitutes, and this means that they are not psychically satisfied by the 
situation. 

What effect intercourse with prostitutes may have upon the personahty 
of the male who is involved, is a matter which will need careful investiga- 
tion by a quahfied psychologist or psychiatrist. 



Chapter 21 


HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 

In the total male population, single and married, between adolescence 
and old age, 24 per cent of the total outlet is derived from solitary sources 
(masturbation and nocturnal emissions), 69.4 per cent is derived from 
heterosexual sources (petting and coitus), and 6.3 per cent of the total 
number of orgasms is derived from homosexual contacts. It is not more 
than 0.3 per cent of the outlet which is derived from relations with animals 
of other species. 

Homosexual contacts account, therefore, for a rather small but still 
significant portion of the total outlet of the human male. The significance 
of the homosexual is, furthermore, much greater than the frequencies of 
outlet may indicate, because a considerable portion of the population, 
perhaps the major portion of the male population, has at least some homo- 
sexual experience between adolescence and old age. In addition, about 60 
per cent of the pre-adolescent boys engage in homosexual activities (Chapter 
5), and there is an additional group of adult males who avoid overt con- 
tacts but who are quite aware of their potentialities for reacting to other 
males. 

The social significance of the homosexual is considerably emphasized 
by the fact that both Jewish and Christian churches have considered this 
aspect of human sexuality to be abnormal and immoral (Chapter 13). 
Social custom and our Anglo-American law are sometimes very severe in 
penalizing one who is discovered to have had homosexual relations. In 
consequence, many persons who have had such experience are psychically 
disturbed, and not a few of them have been in open conflict with the social 
organization. 

It is, therefore, peculiarly difficult to secure factual data concerning the 
nature and the extent of the homosexual in Western European or American 
cultures, and even more difficult to find strictly objective presentations of 
such data as are available. Most of the literature on the homosexual repre- 
sents either a polemic against the heinous abnormality of such activity, 
or a biased argument in defense of an individual’s right to choose Ms 
patterns of sexual behavior. 

Until the extent of any type of human behavior is adequately known, it 
is difficult to assess its significance, either to the individuals who are in- 
volved or to society as a whole; and until the extent of the homosexual is 

610 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


611 


known, it is practically impossible to understand its biologic or social 
origins. It is one thing if we are dealing with a type of activity that is 
unusual, without precedent among other animals, and restricted to pecuhar 
types of individuals within the human population. It is another thing if 
the phenomenon proves to be a fundamental part, not only of human 
sexuality, but of mammalian patterns as a whole. The present chapter is, 
therefore, wholly confined to an analysis of the data which we now have 
on the incidence and the frequencies of homosexual activity in the white 
male population in this country. Analyses of the factors which affect the 
development of both heterosexual and homosexual patterns of behavior 
will be presented in a subsequent volume in this series. 

REFERENCES 

Specific data on the incidences and frequencies of overt homosexual 
contacts in various segments of the male population have already been 
detaile4 in this volume in tables and charts, and in discussions in the text, 
as follows: 


PAGE 

TABLE 

HGURE 

68-70 

84-88 

2 

4, 155-162 

94-102 

3,6 

13 

121-125, 

13 


129, 130 

133-143 

16, 20 

21 

143-147 

21,23 

24 

162, 167- 

24 

25 

168 

166-167 

25 


168 

26 


169-171 

27 


168, 171- 

28 

26 

172 

171, 174 

29 


175-181 

30-34 


190-191 

38 

30 

196 


31 

211-213 

42 


232-233 

48 


234-235 

49 


258-261, 

58, 66 

83-88 

285-293 


NATURE OF DATA 

Homosexual items covered in the inter- 
view 

Size of sample necessary to establish 
homosexual data 

Comparisons of data obtained from hun- 
dred percent and partial samples 
Compansons of data on original histories 
and retakes 

Comparisons of data obtained by three 
interviewers 

Comparisons of data obtained in succes- 
sive periods 

Ages involved in pre-adolescent homo- 
sexual play 

Number of years involved in pre-adoles- 
cent homosexual play 
Sex of companions of pre-adolescent boys 
Techniques in pre-adolescent sex play 
Age of first pre-adolescent sex play 

Continuity of pre-adolescent sex play 
with adolescent activity 
Pre-adolescent eroticism and orgasm 
Sources of first ejaculation 
Examples of combinations of sources of 
outlet 

Sexual outlet in a restrained group of 
males 

Capacity for multiple orgasm in sexual 
contacts 

Range of variation in homosexual con- 
tacts 

Homosexual contacts in relation to 
marital status and age 



612 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 

PAGE 

TABLE 

FIGURE 

NATURE OF DATA 

300, 312, 
315, 317, 
320-321 

68, 77-78 

91,94 

Homosexual contacts in relation to age 
at onset of adolescence 

357-362, 

382-384 

90, 94, 96-97,114- 
115, 141-146 

105-106 

Homosexual contacts and social level 

369-373 

94 


Oral techniques in sexual contacts 

402-403, 

410-413 

100, 104-105 

112 

Comparisons of incidences and frequen- 
cies of the homosexual in two succes- 
sive generations 

455-460 

123 


Homosexual contacts and rural-urban 
backgrounds 

482-484 

131 


Homosexual outlets and religious back- 
grounds 

378, 382, 
488-493 
512 

523-527 

96, 97 

126, 128-133 

Portion of total outlet derived from 
homosexual contacts 

Relation of masturbation and the homo- 
sexual 

Significance of dream content 

617-629 

139-140 

3,6,156-158 

Accumulative incidence of homosexual 
contacts 

631-636 


159-160 

Individual variation in frequencies of 
homosexual contacts 

636-650 


161 

Heterosexual-homosexual rating scale 

640-656 

141-150 

162-169 

Distribution of heterosexual-homosexual 
ratings in the male population 

658 


170 

Development of heterosexuality and 
homosexuality, by age periods 


DEFINITION 

For nearly a century the term homosexual in connection with human 
behavior has been applied to sexual relations, either overt or psychic, 
between individuals of the same sex. Derived from the Greek root homo 
rather than from the Latin word for man, the term emphasizes the sameness 
of the two individuals who are involved in a sexual relation. The word is, 
of course, patterned after and intended to represent the antithesis of the 
word heterosexual, which apphes to a relation between individuals of 
different sexes. 

The term homosexual has had an endless list of synonyms in the technical 
vocabularies and a still greater list in the vernaculars. The terms homogenic 
love, contrasexuality, homo-erotism, similisexualism, uranism and others 
have been used in English (Legman in Henry 1941). The terms sexual 
inversion, intersexuality, transsexuaUty, the third sex, psychosexual her- 
maphroditism, and others have been apphed not merely to designate the 
nature of the partner involved in the sexual relation, but to emphasize the 
general opinion that individuals engaging in homosexual activity are 
neither male nor female, but persons of mixed sex. These latter terms are, 
however, most unfortunate, for they provide an interpretation in anticipa- 
tion of any sufficient demonstration of the fact; and consequently they 
prejudice investigations of the nature and origin of homosexual activity. 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


613 


The term Lesbian, referring to such female homosexual relations as were 
immortalized in the poetry of Sappho of the Greek Isle of Lesbos, has 
gained considerable usage within recent years, particularly in some of the 
larger Eastern cities where the existence of female homosexuality is more 
generally recognized by the public at large. Although there can be no 
objection to designating relations between females by a special term, it 
should be recognized that such activities are quite the equivalent of sexual 
relations between males. 

It is unfortunate that the students of animal behavior have applied the 
term homosexual to a totally different sort of phenomenon among the 
lower mammals. In most of the literature on animal behavior it is applied 
on the basis of the general conspectus of the behavior pattern of the animal, 
its aggressiveness in seeking the sexual contact, its postures during coitus, 
its position relative to the other animal in the sex relation, and the conform- 
ance or disconformance of that behavior to the usual positions and activities 
of the animal during heterosexual coitus (Ball, var. titles; Beach, var. 
titles, espec. 1947). 

In most mammals the behavior of the female in a heterosexual perform- 
ance usually involves the acceptance of the male which is trying to make in- 
tromission. The female at such a moment is less aggressive than the male, 
even passive in her acceptance of the male’s approaches, and subordinate 
in position to him during actual coitus. This means that the female usually 
hes beneath the male or in front of him during copulation, either submitting 
from the very beginning of the sexual relation or (as in the cats, ferret, 
mink, and some other animals) being forced into submission by the assault 
of the male. In the case of the mink, the female is far from being passive 
during the initial stages of the contact, and the courting performances 
involve as strenuous fighting as the most extreme non-sexual circumstances 
could produce. There is no sexual relation, however, until the female has 
been sufficiently subdued to allow the male to effect coitus. In the case of 
the rat, the female which is in heat as the result of the hormones which her 
ovaries secrete near the time of ovulation, is more readily induced to crouch 
on the floor, arch her back (in lordosis) so her body is raised posteriorly, 
and pass into a nervous state which is characterized by a general rigidity 
of most of the body, but by a constant and rapid trembling of the ears and 
by peculiar hopping movements. This is the behavior which is character- 
istic of the female in a heterosexual contact, and this is what the students 
of animals describe as typically feminine behavior. 

Throughout the mammals it is the male which more often (but not 
always) pursues the female for a sexual contact. In species where there is a 
struggle before the female submits to coitus, the male must be physically 
dominant and capable of controlling the female. In the ultimate act it is 
the male which more often mounts in back of the female and makes the 



614 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HimAN MALE 


active pelvic thrusts which effect intromission. This is the behavior that 
students of the lower mammals commonly refer to as typically masculine 
behavior. 

But among many species of mammals and, indeed, probably among 
all of them, it not infrequently happens that males and females assume other 
than their usual positions in a sexual contact. This may be dependent upon 
individual differences in the physiology or anatomy of certain individuals, 
on differences in hormones, on environmental circumstances, or on some 
previous experience which has conditioned the animal in its behavior. 

In a certain number of cases the assumption of the attitudes and posi- 
tions of the opposite sex, among these lower mammals, seems to depend upon 
nothing more than the accident of the position in which the individual finds 
itself. The same male rat that has mounted a female in typical heterosexual 
coitus only a few moments before, may crouch on the floor, arch its back, 
and rear its posterior when it is approached by another rat from the rear. 
The same female which rises from the floor where she has been crouching 
in front of a copulating male may bump into another rat as she runs around 
the cage, rear on her haunches in front of the decumbent partner, and go 
through all of the motions that a male ordinarily goes through in hetero- 
sexual copulation. She may move her pelvis in thrusts which are quite hke 
those of the male. She may strike her genital area against the genital area 
of the rat in front, quite as she would if she had a penis to effect entrance. 
And, what is most astounding, she may double up her body as she pulls 
back from the genital thrusts and manipulate her own genitaUa with her 
mouth (Beach 1947), exactly as the male rat ordinarily manipulates his 
penis between the thrusts that he makes when he is engaged in the mascu- 
line role in the usual type of heterosexual relation. 

The assumption by a male animal of a female position in a sexual rela- 
tion, or the assumption by a female of a position which is more typical of 
the male in a heterosexual relation, is what the students of animal behavior 
have referred to as homosexuality. This, of course, has nothing whatsoever 
to do with the use of the term among the students of human behavior, 
and one must be exceedingly careful how one transfers the conclusions 
based on these animal studies. 

In studies of human behavior, the term inversion is applied to sexual 
situations in which males play female roles and females play male roles in 
sex relations. Most of the data on “homosexuality” in the animal studies 
actually refer to inversion. Inversion, of course, may occur in either hetero- 
sexual or homosexual relations, although there has been a widespread 
opinion, even among students of human psychology, and among some 
persons whose experience has been largely homosexual, that inversion is 
an invariable accompaniment of homosexuality. However, this generaliza- 
tion is not warranted. A more elaborate presentation of our data would 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


615 


show that there are a great many males who remain as masculine, and a 
great many females who remain as feminine, in their attitudes and their 
approaches in homosexual relations, as the males or females who have 
nothing but heterosexual relations. Inversion and homosexuality are two 
distinct and not always correlated types of behavior. 

More recently some of the students of animal behavior {e.g.. Beach in 
later papers) have used the term bisexual to apply to individuals which 
assume sometimes male and sometimes female roles during sexual activ- 
ities. This, however, is not a happy correction of the terminology, because 
the term bisexual has a long-standing meaning in biology which is totally 
different than the meaning intended here. Moreover, in regard to human 
behavior, the term bisexual has already been misapplied to persons who 
include both heterosexual and homosexual activities in their current 
histories. (See the discussion on “Bisexuality” in a later section in this 
chapter.) The student of animal behavior is observing an inversion of 
behavior patterns, and this is a phenomenon apart from either homo- 
sexuahty or bisexuality, as those terms have ordinarily been used. 

The inappropriate use of the term homosexual in the literature on animal 
behavior has led to unfortunate misinterpretations of the data. Thus, for 
instance, several investigators {e.g,, Ball, Beach, Stone, Young, et al.) have 
shown that the injection of gonadal hormones may modify the frequency 
with which an animal shows an inversion of behavior of the sort described 
above. Among many clinicians this work has been taken to mean that the 
sex hormones control the heterosexuality or homosexuality of an indi- 
vidual’s behavior. This, of course, is a totally unwarranted interpretation. 
The animal work merely shows that there may be an inversion of female and 
male roles as a result of hormonal injections. It points to a relationship 
between the amount of hormone and the aggressiveness of an individual in 
approaching other animals for sexual relations. The injection of male 
hormones quite generally increases the frequency and intensity of an 
animal’s reactions, but there is no evidence that it affects its choice of a 
partner in a sexual relation (Kinsey 1941). Beach (1947) makes the signifi- 
cant observation that the males who most often assume the female type of 
behavior are the ones who “invariably prove to be the most vigorous copu- 
lators,” when they assume the more usual masculine role in coitus. There 
is clinical experience with the human male which similarly shows that the 
intensity of his sexual activity is increased when male hormones are ad- 
ministered, while his choice of a partner (/.c., his heterosexuality or his 
homosexuality) is not modified. 

If the term homosexual is restricted as it should be, the homosexuahty or 
heterosexuality of any activity becomes apparent by determining the sexes 
of the two individuals involved in the relationship. For instance, mouth- 
genital contacts between males and females are certainly heterosexual, 



616 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


even though some persons may think of them as homosexual. And although 
one may hear of a male “who has sex relations with his wife in a homo- 
sexual way,” there is no logic in such a use of the term, and analyses of the 
behavior and of the motivations of the behavior in such cases do not show 
them necessarily related to any homosexual experience. 

On the other hand, the homosexuality of certain relationships between 
individuals of the same sex may be denied by some persons, because the 
situation does not fulfill other criteria that they think should be attached 
to the definition. Mutual masturbation between two males may be dis- 
missed, even by certain clinicians, as not homosexual, because oral or 
anal relations or particular levels of psychic response are required, accord- 
ing to their concept of homosexuality. There are persons who insist that 
the active male in an anal relation is essentially heterosexual in his behavior, 
and that the passive male in the same relation is the only one who is homo- 
sexual. These, however, are misapplications of terms, which are often 
unfortunate because they obscure the interpretations of the situation which 
the clinician is supposed to help by his analysis. 

These misinterpretations are often encouraged by the very persons who 
are having homosexual experience. Some males who are being regularly 
fellated by other males without, however, ever performing fellation 
themselves, may insist that they are exclusively heterosexual and that they 
have never been involved in a truly homosexual relation. Their consciences 
are cleared and they may avoid trouble with society and with the police 
by perpetrating the additional fiction that they are incapable of responding 
to a relation with a male unless they fantasy themselves in contact with a 
female. Even clinicians have allowed themselves to be diverted by such 
pretensions. The actual histories, however, show few if any cases of sexual 
relations between males which could be considered anything but homo- 
sexual. 

Many individuals who have had considerable homosexual experience, 
construct a hierarchy on the basis of which they insist that anyone who 
has not had as much homosexual experience as they have had, or who is 
less exclusively aroused by homosexual stimuli, is “not really homosexual.” 
It is amazing to observe how many psychologists and psychiatrists have 
accepted this sort of propaganda, and have come to believe that homo- 
sexual males and females are discretely different from persons who merely 
have homosexual experience, or who react sometimes to homosexual stim- 
uh. Sometimes such an interpretation allows for only two kinds of males 
and two kinds of females, namely those who are heterosexual and those 
who are homosexual. But as subsequent data in this chapter will show, 
there is only about half of the male population whose sexual behavior is 
exclusively heterosexual, and there are only a few percent who are exclu- 
sively homosexual. Any restriction of the term homosexuality to individuals 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


617 


who are exclusively so demands, logically, that the term heterosexual be 
apphed only to those individuals who are exclusively heterosexual; and this 
makes no allowance for the nearly half of the population which has had 
sexual contacts with, or reacted psychically to, individuals of their own as 
well as of the opposite sex. Actually, of course, one must learn to recognize 
every combination of heterosexuality and homosexuality in the histories of 
various individuals. 

It would encourage clearer thinking on these matters if persons were not 
characterized as heterosexual or homosexual, but as individuals who have 
had certain amounts of heterosexual experience and certain amounts of 
homosexual experience. Instead of using these terms as substantives which 
stand for persons, or even as adjectives to describe persons, they may 
better be used to describe the nature of the overt sexual relations, or of 
the stimuli to which an individual erotically responds. 

PREVIOUS ESTIMATES OF INCIDENCE 

Many persons have recognized the importance of securing specific 
information on the incidence of the homosexual. The clinician needs to 
know how far the experience of his patient departs from norms for the 
remainder of the population. Counselors, teachers, clergymen, personnel 
officers, the administrators of institutions, social workers, law enforcement 
officers, and still others who are concerned with the direction of human 
behavior, may completely misinterpret the meaning of the homosexual 
experience in an individual’s history, unless they understand the incidence 
and frequency of such activity in the population as a whole. 

Administrators in prisons, mental institutions, public and private 
schools, colleges and universities, the Army and the Navy, Y.M.C.A. and 
scouting activities, and of all other sorts of groups, must understand the 
part which the homosexual plays in the life of the total male population, 
before they can understand the significance of the behavior of the particular 
individuals with whom they are called upon to deal. Scientific explanations 
of the origin and development of the homosexual, and, for that matter, of 
the heterosexual, will not be on any sound basis until we know the number 
of persons who are involved in each type of activity, the ages at which 
they first become involved, and the ages at which they are most frequently 
involved. There is no other aspect of human sexual activity about which it 
has been more important to have some precise knowledge of the incidences 
and frequencies. 

There are many persons who believe the homosexual to be a rare phe- 
nomenon, a chnical curiosity, and something which one may never meet 
among the sorts of persons with whom he would associate. On the other 
hand, there are some clinicians and some persons who have had first-hand 
contacts in the homosexual, who have estimated that something between 
50 and 100 per cent of the population has such experience. 



618 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


There is undoubtedly a tendency on the part of some males who have 
had frequent homosexual contacts to exaggerate in their estimates. Some 
of these more promiscuous males have actually tested the responses of 
hundreds and sometimes of thousands of males whom they have invited 
to have homosexual relations. Many of them insist that a very high propor- 
tion of all the males whom they have approached have accepted such rela- 
tions, and it is upon this fact that they base their opinion that most males 
are “homosexuar or that they are “partly homosexual,” or that they “are 
really homosexual even though they may not be aware of it and may not 
have had actual experience.” But they overlook the fact that the experi- 
enced male does not actually invite anyone to have sexual relations until 
he has had such social contact as may indicate the final success of his 
sexual approach. His contacts are, therefore, really confined to a veiy 
select portion of the males whom he meets. 

Satisfactory incidence figures on the homosexual cannot be obtained by 
any technique short of a carefully planned population survey. The data 
should cover every segment of the total population. There is no other 
aspect of human sexual behavior where it is more fundamental that the 
sample be secured without any selection of cases which would bias the 
results. Many persons with homosexual experience very naturally hesitate 
to expose their histories. On the other hand, there are some who are so 
upset by personal conflicts or social diflBculties that have developed out 
of their homosexual activities that they are anxious to discuss their prob- 
lems with an investigator whom they have come to trust. In consequence, 
if one depends only upon volunteers in a survey, it is impossible to know 
whether homosexual histories are represented in an undue proportion, or 
less often than their actual incidence would demand. In order to secure 
data that have any relation to the reality, it is imperative that the cases be 
derived from as careful a distribution and stratification of the sample as 
the public opinion polls employ, or as we have employed in the present 
study. 

Unfortunately, no previous attempts to assess the incidence of the homo- 
sexual have begun to satisfy these demands for statistical adequacy. The 
incidence figures which are most often quoted are derived from the 2 to 5 
per cent estimate which Havelock Ellis made for England (Ellis 1936), and 
from the more elaborate calculations made by Hirschfeld, chiefly for Ger- 
many (as finally summarized in Hirschfeld 1920). The professional litera- 
ture, if it does not cite these studies, rarely quotes any other sources except 
“the best informed students of the subject” (e.g., Haire 1937, Rosanoff 
1938, Squier in Folsom 1938, Painter 1941, Moore 1945, et al.); and 
through devious channels these data have become general property among 
people who have no idea of their origin. Terman and Miles (1936) do 
credit a 4 per cent estimate to “the university medical staff in one of the 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


619 


largest of American universities.” And there is a bare statement in McPart- 
land (1947) which reports a current “guess” that “the number of potential 
homosexuals in the United States is in the neighborhood of 8,000,000 or 
higher”— a figure that represents about 6 per cent of the total male and 
female population. 

As for Ellis’ estimate of a 2 to 5 per cent incidence figure for males, and 
double that figure for females, it is to be noted that this follows a review 
of the Hirschfeld data, and is made without any support other than the 
statement that “considering those individuals with whom I have been 
brought in contact by the ordinary circumstances of life ... I am still led 
to the conclusions that . . . there must be a distinct percentage which may 
sometimes be . . . slightly over 2 per cent.” As a matter of fact, Ellis never 
made any sort of systematic survey of any aspect of sex in any segment of 
the population. He had a minimum of face to face contact with his subjects, 
and depended largely upon information which was supplied him by cor- 
respondents. It is, of course, only a very select portion of the population 
that will send sex histories through the mails, and such histories are rarely 
more than partial accounts, usually of specific episodes that have been 
high lights in the life of the individual. 

More elaborate attempts to obtain estimates of the extent of homosexual 
activity have been made by some of the Central European students. At the 
turn of the century, Romer in Holland got 595 of his fellow students to 
give written answers to questions concerning their erotic reactions to fe- 
males and to males. In 1903 and 1904, Magnus Hirschfeld conducted a 
much more extensive investigation (finally summarized in Hirschfeld 1920). 

Through the mails, Hirschfeld distributed forms to 3000 students at the 
Charlottenburg Institute of Technology, and to 5721 metal workers in 
Berlin, asking each recipient to indicate whether his “libido had always 
been directed only to females , . . only to males ... or to both males and 
females.” Of the 7481 persons who apparently received the letters, about 
49 per cent answered. On the basis of these replies, Hirschfeld concluded 
that 94.3 per cent of the males were exclusively heterosexual, 2.3 per cent 
homosexual, and the remainder bisexual. The survey is open to the very 
severe criticism that it involved only a highly selected sample of the total 
population. What is more serious, one is left guessing as to the histories of 
the 51 per cent that failed to answer the questionnaire. 

In a more elaborate attempt to secure estimates of the incidence of the 
homosexual, Hirschfeld contacted persons who, because they had homo- 
sexual histories, could supply some information concerning the extent of 
such activity in the business or professional groups in which they moved. 
Persons in the Army and Navy were asked to estimate how many in their 
whole company or among the officers in their group were known as homo- 
sexual. College students were asked to estimate how many of the men in 



620 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


their fraternities were known to have homosexual histories. Similar reports 
were obtained from groups of Protestant clergymen and from Catholic 
priests, from postal employees, railroad employees, a group of court 
judges, bakers, bank employees, draftsmen, butchers, actors, hotel em- 
ployees, the recorded histories of English kings, etc. — from a total of 34 
different groups. Hirschfeld concluded that 525 out of the 23,771 persons 
in these groups were “homosexual.” Calculations give an incidence figure 
of approximately 2.2 per cent, and this is the figure on which Hirschfeld 
subsequently depended. 

Obviously this method of sampling falls far short of the demands of a 
scientific population analysis. It depends upon the ability of an informant 
to know the sexual histories of all the persons in a group, without collecting 
actual histories from any of them. It depends upon the informant’s ability 
to recognize homosexual males (other than those with whom he has had 
actual contact) on the basis of their physical characters and mannerisms, 
or of their public reputations. Very often such reputations are' nothing 
more than mere gossip. Moreover, there are many persons in any group 
whose homosexual histories are never known publicly. In brief, such 
sources of information are little better than the gossip and general impres- 
sions on which many persons depended before public opinion polls showed 
what can be accomplished in a statistically well-organized survey. 

Hirschfeld deserves considerable credit for having tried on a larger scale 
than anyone had before to ascertain the facts on a matter that has always 
been difficult to survey. Down to the beginning of the present study, no 
more serious attempt has been made. Nevertheless, the uncritical accept- 
ance of these inadequate calculations has delayed the recognition of the 
magnitude of the medical, psychiatric, social, and legal problems involved 
in homosexuality, and delayed scientific interpretations of the bases of 
such behavior. 

In later years, Hirschfeld had the opportunity to obtain the histories of 
persons who visited his Sex Institute at Berhn, some of them as patients, 
some of them merely as visitors who filled out the questionnaire supplied 
by the Institute. Some 10,000 of these were accumulated in the course of 
the years; but the data were uninterpretable because they were derived 
from such a select portion of the total population. Moreover, all of the 
Hirschfeld conclusions were biased by his opinion that a person is really 
homosexual only when his psychic or overt contacts are more or less ex- 
clusively so, and consequently his estimates may come nearer representing 
the incidence of certain degrees of homosexuality, rather than the totahty 
of homosexual activity. 

There have been other European studies that have been modelled on the 
Hirschfeld techniques, but all of them were based on smaller populations, 
and none of them has had as great influence on the thinking of clinicians. 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


621 


In this country, three investigators have obtained data on the incidence 
of the homosexual in our American male population. It is notable that ail 
three of them have secured figures which are remarkably higher than the 
European studies have given — not because there is any likelihood that the 
American picture is particularly different from that in Europe, but because 
all of these studies have come nearer satisfying the demands of a population 
survey. All of them involved a more thorough coverage of particular 
groups, and all of them were based on direct interviews with persons with 
whom the interviewer had had enough contact to have developed some 
rapport. Hamilton (1929) found that 17 per cent of the hundred men in 
his study had had homosexual experience after they were eighteen years 
old. Ramsey (1943), in a study of 291 younger boys, one-half of whom 
constituted a hundred percent sample of a seventh and eighth grade group 
in a junior high school, found that 30 per cent had had adolescent homo- 
sexual experience to the point of orgasm. More recently (1947), Finger has 
reported 27 per cent of a college class of 1 1 1 males admitting “at least one 
overt homosexual episode involving orgasm.” These figures come remark- 
ably close to those which we have obtained in the present study. 

One other source of data on the extent of “homosexuality” among 
American males has recently become available through statistics gathered 
by Selective Service Boards and at induction centers during the last war. 
Theoretically, this should have been a splendid opportunity to gain infor- 
mation that would have been of considerable scientific use and of consider- 
able practical use to the armed forces. From these sources, the over-all 
figures show that about one-tenth of 1 per cent of all the men were rejected 
by draft boards (Selective Service Bull. 1-4), about 0.4 per cent were turned 
down at induction centers (e.g,, Hohman and Schaffner 1947), and about 
as many more were subsequently discharged for homosexual activity while 
they were in active service. The total gives about 1 per cent officially 
identified as “homosexual.” These figures are so much lower than any 
fvhich case history studies have obtained that they need critical examination. 

The most obvious explanation of these very low figures lies in the fact 
that both the Army and Navy had precluded the possibility of getting 
accurate data on these matters by announcing at the beginning of the war 
that they intended to exclude all persons with homosexual histories. The 
American Army and Navy have always been traditionally opposed to 
homosexual activity, and in the last war, for the first time, they turned to 
psychiatrists to provide expert help in eliminating individuals with such 
histories. 

Physicians on draft boards and psychiatrists at induction centers were 
charged with the responsibility of detecting and eliminating men with such 
records, and many of the psychiatrists at induction centers paid especial 
attention to identifying these men. While the reasons for elimination of 



622 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


any man were supposed to be kept confidential, they were in actuality not 
infrequently known to the whole community in which he lived. The mere 
fact that he was rejected under a particular classification, or discharged 
from the Army or Navy on a particular discharge form, often made him a 
subject for suspicion, and in a large number of instances practically pre- 
cluded the possibility of his securing employment as a civilian. Conse- 
sequently, few men with any common sense would admit their homosexual 
experience to draft boards or to psychiatrists at induction centers or in the 
services. 

It is amazing that some of the psychiatrists (e.g., Hohman and Schaffner 
1947) apparently believed that they were getting a true record under these 
circumstances. Only a naive individual, one who was badly neurotic and 
upset over his experience, or an effeminate type of male who freely ex- 
hibited his homosexual interests, was ordinarily detected through the 
ojSicial channels. Many of the psychiatrists were less experienced in identify- 
ing the obviously homosexual male than several million untrained persons 
who had had actual contact with homosexual activities. Many psychiatrists 
realized this, and some of them recognized the fact that the incidence of 
homosexual activity in the armed forces must have been high — even 
involving as many as 10 per cent or more of the men. 

It is also to be noted that at induction centers the average interview was 
limited to less than three minutes. Considering that the psychoanalysts and 
many of the other psychiatrists have heretofore insisted that one could not 
expect to obtain data on socially taboo items of sexual behavior in any- 
thing less that a hundred hours of analysis, it is the more surprising that 
the results of these short interviews at induction centers should have been 
taken seriously. 

Discharges from the Army and Navy similarly have not provided any 
adequate source of information on the actual incidence of homosexual 
activity. Many psychiatrists in the armed forces were aware of the great 
social damage done to an individual who was discharged for such reasons, 
and they considered it desirable to help him by showing fiat feet, stomach 
ulcers, shock, or some other non-sexual item as the immediate cause of 
the discharge. Consequently, no one anywhere in official circles in the 
Army and the Navy will ever be able to obtain any adequate estimate of 
the number of men with homosexual activity who were identified and 
discharged from the services during the war. 

The estimates on the incidence of the homosexual, range, then, from 
these Selective Service figures of one-tenth of 1 per cent to the 100 per 
cent estimates of some of the psychoanalysts and of some promiscuous 
homosexual males. It has, therefore, been especially important in our pres- 
ent study to apply all of the techniques of a statistically sound popula- 
tion survey to obtaining data on this particular matter. 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


623 


INCIDENCE DATA IN PRESENT STUDY 

The statistics given throughout this volume on the incidence of homo- 
sexual activity, and the statistics to be given in the present section of this 
chapter, are based on those persons who have had physical contacts with 
other males, and who were brought to orgasm as a result of such contacts. 
By any strict definition such contacts are homosexual, irrespective of the 
extent of the psychic stimulation involved, of the techniques employed, 
or of the relative importance of the homosexual and the heterosexual in 
the history of such an individual. These are not data on the number of 
persons who are “homosexual,” but on the number of persons who have 
had at least some homosexual experience — even though sometimes not more 
than one experience— up to the ages shown in the tables and curves. The 
incidences of persons who have had various amounts of homosexual 
experience are presented in a later section of this chapter. 

An individual who engages in a sexual relation with another male 
without, however, coming to chmax, or an individual who is erotically 
aroused by a homosexual stimulus without ever having overt relations, has 
certainly had a homosexual experience. Such relations and reactions are, 
however, not included in the incidence data given here nor in most other 
places in this volume, because the volume as a whole has been concerned 
with the number and sources of male orgasms. On the other hand, the 
data on the heterosexual-homosexual ratings which are presented later 
in the present chapter, do take into account these homosexual contacts 
in which the subject fails to reach climax. Accumulative incidence curves 
based upon heterosexual-homosexual ratings may, therefore, be somewhat 
higher than the accumulative incidence curves based upon overt contacts 
carried through to the point of actual orgasm. 

Data on the homosexual activity of the pre-adolescent boy have been 
presented in another chapter (Chapter 5) and no male is included in any 
of the calculations shown in the present chapter unless he has had homo- 
sexual experience beyond the onset of adolescence. 

In these terms (of physical contact to the point of orgasm), the data in 
the present study indicate that at least 37 per cent of the male population 
has some homosexual experience between the beginning of adolescence 
and old age (U. S. Corrections. See Table 139, Figure 156). This is more 
than one male in three of the persons that one may meet as he passes along 
a city street. Among the males who remain unmarried until the age of 35, 
almost exactly 50 per cent have homosexual experience between the 
beginning of adolescence and that age. Some of these persons have but a 
single experience, and some of them have much more or even a lifetime 
of “experience; but all of them have at least some experience to the point 
of orgasm. 

21 



624 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Total Homosexual Outlet: Accumulative Incidence 


AGE 

total population 

u. s. corrections 

EDUC. level 

0-8 j 

EDUC. LEVEL 

9-12 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13+ 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

%with 

Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 

3969 

0.0 

662 

0.0 

490 

0.0 

2817 

0.0 

9 

3969 

0.1 

662 

0.0 

490 

0.2 

2817 

0.1 

10 

3969 

0.5 

662 

0.2 

490 

0.6 

2817 

0.5 

11 

3968 

1.7 

661 

1.2 

490 

2.0 

2817 

1.8 

12 

3968 

6.1 

661 

5,6 

490 

6.3 

2817 

6.2 

13 

3968 

12.6 

661 

11.0 

490 

13.7 

2817 

11.6 

14 

3965 

21.3 

658 

17.8 

490 

24.1 

2817 

18.0 

15 

3957 

27.7 

652 

24.8 

488 

31.1 

2817 

' 21.1 

16 

3934 

31.6 

635 

27.7 

483 

36.0 

2816 

23.0 

17 

3874 

34.5 

598 

27.8 

462 

40.9 

2814 

24.1 

18 

3738 

36.7 

574 

29.3 

426 

43.7 

2738 

25.6 

19 

3507 

37.5 

544 

29.0 

389 

45.0 

2574 

26.7 

20 

3203 

36.7 

516 

28.9 

348 

43.4 

2339 

27.6 

21 

2830 

37.0 

492 

29.1 

305 

43.6 

2033 

28.6 

22 

2428 

37.1 

473 

29.0 

283 

43.5 

1672 

29.8 

23 

2113 

37.3 

458 

29.0 

258 

43.4 

1397 

31.5 

24 

1822 

36.5 

438 

29.2 i 

232 

41.8 

1152 

32.1 

25 

1636 

35.4 ! 

418 

28.0 i 

216 

42.1 

1002 

33.0 

26 

1493 

35.6 

407 

28.0 ! 

202 

42.6 

884 

32.9 

27 

1358 ! 

35.6 

393 

28.5 

191 

41.9 

774 

33.7 

28 

1252 

35.5 

379 

28.2 

174 

42.0 

699 

33.9 

29 

1143 

33.7 

355 

27.3 

154 

39.0 

634 

33.6 

30 

1049 

32.4 

339 

26.5 

137 

38.7 

573 

33.7 

31 

973 

31.3 

319 

25.4 

125 

36.8 

529 

34.2 

32 

915 

30.5 

307 

26.1 

116 

34.5 

492 

32.9 

33 

856 

31.0 

295 

25.4 

113 

36.3 

448 

33.9 

34 

804 

29.9 

287 

23.7 

105 

35.2 

412 

34.7 

35 

747 

27.5 

273 

22.3 

92 

33.7 

382 

34.0 

36 

703 

27.2 

260 

22.7 

87 

32.2 

356 

33.7 

37 

641 

26.1 

242 

21.9 

76 

30.3 

323 

33.4 

38 

611 

25.4 

234 

20.9 i 

70 

30.0 

307 

33.2 

39 

556 

25.3 

212 

20.8 

64 

29.7 

280 

33.6 

40 

509 

25.0 

194 

21.6 

58 

29.3 

257 

32.7 

41 

474 

23.3 

183 

20.2 

53 

26.4 

238 

31.9 

42 

445 

23.3 

174 

1 19.5 

50 

28.0 

221 

31.2 

43 

399 

22.9 

' 159 

20.1 



192 

32.8 

44 

369 

23.5 

146 

21.9 



177 

31.1 

45 

340 

22.9 

135 

21.5 



161 

32.9 


Table 139. Accumulative incidence data on total homosexual outlet 

Covering the life span, including pre-marital, extra-marital, and post-marital exper- 
ience. In three educational levels, and in the total population corrected for the U. S. 
Census of 1940. 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


625 


These figures are, of course, considerably higher than any which have 
previously been estimated; but as already shown (Chapter 4) they must be 
understatements, if they are anything other than the fact. 

We ourselves were totally unprepared to find such incidence data when 
this research was originally undertaken. Over a period of several years we 
were repeatedly assailed with doubts as to whether we were getting a fair 
cross section of the total population or whether a selection of cases was 
biasing the results. It has been our experience, however, that each new 
group into which we have gone has provided substantially the same data. 



ASE 

Figure 156. Homosexual outlet: accumulative incidence in total U. S. population 
and in single population alone 

Black line shows percent of total population which has ever had homosexual experi- 
ence by each of the indicated ages. Hollow line shows percent of the population of 
single males which has ever had experience. All data corrected for U. S. Census distribu- 
tion. The incidence for the total population is lower than the incidence for the single 
population because the males who marry have less homosexual experience and bring 
down the averages when they are included in the calculations with the single males. 

Whether the histories were taken in one large city or another, whether 
they were taken in large cities, in small towns, or in rural areas, whether 
they came from one college or from another, a church school or a state 
university or some private institution, whether they came from one part of 
the country or from another, the incidence data on the homosexual have 
been more or less the same. 

While the validity of the data on all of the sexual outlets has been 
tested and retested throughout the study (Chapters 3 and 4), especial 
attention has been given to testing the material on the homosexual. This 
means, specifically, that we have checked these homosexual data in the 
following ways: 


626 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


1. By comparing samples of various size, taken by a strict randomiza- 
tion out of the whole of the accumulation of histories (Tables 2, 155-162, 
Figure 4). 

2. By carefully providing cross-checks and other techniques in the 
interviewing which would check memory and the accuracy of the data 
(Chapters 2, 4). 

3. By comparing data obtained from hundred percent and partial 
samples (Tables 3, 6, Figure 13). 

4. By comparing data on originals and re-takes of histories (Table 13). 

5. By comparing data obtained by three different interviewers (Tables 
16, 20, Figure 21). 

6. By comparing data obtained by the same interviewer in two suc- 
cessive four-year periods (Tables 21, 23, Figure 24). 

7. By measuring the trends shown by data calculated for successive 
age periods (Tables 58, 66, Figures 83-88). 

8. By comparing data on groups of males who became adolescent at 
different age periods (Tables 68, 77-78, Figures 91, 94). 

9. By comparing data obtained from males of different educational 
levels and occupational classes (Tables 90, 94, 96-97, 114-115, 141-146, 
Figures 105-106). 

10. By comparing homosexual incidences in two generations for which 
the median age difference was 22 years (Table 100, Figure 114). 

11. By comparing the incidences in rural and in urban groups (Table 
123). 

12. By comparing the data on various religious groups (Table 131). 

If we had arrived at the present incidence figures by a single calculation 
based on a single population, one might well question their validity. But 
the determination of the extent of the homosexual in the population is too 
important a matter to be settled on anything but an elaborately devised 
system of samples. When twelve ways of obtaining data give results that 
are as consistent as those which are to be found in the tables and charts 
listed above, there can be no question that the actual incidence of the 
homosexual is at least 37 and 50 per cent as given above. The tests show 
that the actual figures may be as much as 5 per cent higher, or still higher. 

Those who have been best acquainted with the extent of homosexual 
activity in the population, whether through clinical contacts with homo- 
sexual patients, through homosexual acquaintances, or through their own 
firsthand homosexual experience, will not find it too diflacult to accept 
the accumulative incidence figures which are arrived at here. There are 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


627 


many who have been aware of the fact that persons with homosexual 
histories are to be found in every age group, in every social level, in every 
conceivable occupation, in cities and on farms, and in the most remote 
areas in the country. They have known the homosexual in young adoles- 
cents and in persons of every other age. They have known it in single 
persons and in the histories of males who were married. In large city 
communities they know that an experienced observer may identify hun- 
dreds of persons in a day whose homosexual interests are certain. They 
have known the homosexuality of many persons whose histories were 
utterly unknown to most of their friends and acquaintances. They have 
repeatedly had the experience of discovering homosexual histories among 


ioo 

. 80 

0 

-J 

1 60 

o. 

-i 

^ 40 

u. 

O 

H 

X 

lU 

2 to 







M0SEXU> 

ALL h 

\l OUTL 

4ALES 

PT 





HU 

C 1 


] 


£DUC LE 

© 


— > f P 

VBU I5+- 


-J 


_ jh ji 

• S 






^ 5 S ® ( 

> 0 0 r> 0 ( 



^ U LC? 



^ 

1 






■ 


10 


15 


20 


25 

AGE 


30 


35 


40 


45 


Figure 157. Homosexual outlet: accumulative incidence in total U. S. population 
for three educational levels 


Showing percent of each population that has ever had homosexual experience by 
each of the indicated ages. All data based on total population, irrespective of marital 
status. 


persons whom they had known for years before they realized that they had 
had anything except heterosexual experience. 

On the other hand, the incidence of the homosexual is not 100 per cent, 
as some persons would have it. There is no doubt that there are males who 
have never been involved in any sexual contact with any other male, and 
who have never been conscious of any erotic arousal by another male. 
For while some of the psychoanalysts will contend to the contrary, it is 
to be pointed out that there are several dozen psychoanalysts who have 
contributed histories to this study who have insisted that they have never 
identified homosexual experience or reactions in their own histories. 

The number of males who have any homosexual experience after the 
onset of adolescence (the accumulative incidence) is highest in the group 
that enters high school but never goes beyond in its educational career. 


628 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Pre-marital Homosexual Outlet; Accumulative Incidence 


age 

TOTAL POPULATION 

U. S. CORRECTIONS 

educ. level 

0-8 

EDUC. LEVEL 

9-12 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13+ 

j 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

Cases 

% with 
Exper. 

8 ! 

4301 

0.0 

814 

0.0 

632 

0.0 

2855 

0.0 

9 

4301 

0,1 

814 

0.0 

632 

0.2 

2855 

0.1 

10 

4301 

0.4 

814 

0.2 

632 

0.5 

2855 

0.5 

11 

4300 

1.8 

813 

1.2 

632 

2.1 

2855 

, 1.8 

12 

4300 

6.4 

813 

5.5 

632 

7.0 

2855 

6.1 

13 

4299 

13.1 

812 

11.0 

632 

14.6 

2855 

11.6 

14 , 

4296 

21.5 

809 

17.8 

632 

24.5 

2855 

18.0 

15 

4289 

28.0 

802 

24.7 

632 

31.6 

2855 

21.1 

16 

4261 

32.1 

781 

28.0 

626 

36.7 

2854 

23.0 

11 

4177 

35.8 

731 

28.7 

596 

42.8 

2850 

24.1 

18 

3981 

37.8 

674 

30.1 

535 

45.4 

2772 

25.5 

19 

3657 

39.8 

598 

30.1 

457 

48.6 

2602 

26.6 

20 

3238 

40 3 

518 

31.7 

376 

48.4 

2344 

27.4 

21 

2782 

40.4 

456 

32.0 

312 

48.1 

2014 

28.5 

22 i 

2233 

40.6 

367 

32.7 

251 

47.8 

1615 

29.8 

23 

1795 

42.1 

298 

35.9 

215 

48.4 

1282 

31.2 

24 

1433 

44.1 1 

260 

36.5 

178 

51.7 

995 

31.2 

25 

1157 

44.4 

221 

38.0 

154 

53.2 

782 

33.0 

26 

945 

46,9 

189 

42.3 

135 

54.8 

621 

33.7 

27 

736 

48.1 

171 

45.0 

119 

54.6 

446 

35.0 

28 

593 

48.9 

150 

47.3 

105 

53.3 

338 

38.8 

29 

491 

48.0 

131 

45.8 

91 

52.7 

269 

i 38.7 

30 

397 

48.1 

117 

45.3 

72 

54.2 

208 

40.4 

31 

324 

48.6 

99 

45.5 

64 

54.7 

161 

41.6 

32 

281 

50.2 

93 

46.2 

56 

57.1 

132 

43.9 

33 

242 

49.7 

80 

45.0 



113 

44.2 

34 

207 

50,9 

75 

42.7 



90 

46.7 

35 

180 

49.8 

70 

40.0 



76 

47.4 

36 

163 

50.5 

69 

40.6 



66 

50.0 

37 

141 

48.8 

62 

40.3 



56 

55.4 

38 

132 

53.7 

58 

41.4 



54 

53.7 

39 

114 

50.8 

51 

37.3 






Table 140. Accumulative incidence data on homosexual outlet in single males 

In three educational levels, and in the total population corrected for the U. S. Census 
of 1940. 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


629 


In that group 55 per cent of the males who are still single by 30 years of 
age have had the experience of being brought to climax through a physical 
contact with another male (Table 90). Among the boys who never go 
beyond grade school the corresponding figure is 45 per cent, and for the 
males who belong to the college level, 40 per cent. The accumulative 
incidence figures for the whole of the life span (Table 140, Figure 157) are a 
bit higher for all of these groups, inasmuch as there are some males who 
do not have their first homosexual experience until after they are 30 years 
of age. 

Among single males in the population, the highest active incidence 
figures occur in the older age groups. Between adolescence and 15 years of 
age about 1 male in 4 (27%) has some homosexual experience (Table 58). 



Figure 158. Flomosexual outlet: accumulative incidence among single males, in 

three educational levels 


The figures rise to nearly 1 male in 3 in the later teens and appear to drop 
a bit in the early twenties. Among those who are not married by the latter 
part of their twenties, the incidence is about 1 male in 3, and the figures 
increase slightly among older unmarried males (39%). There are some 
minor differences in the trends in the different social levels. 

The drop in the active incidence figures between 21 and 25 appears so 
consistently through all of the calculations, that there is reason for believ- 
ing that it represents an actual fact in the behavior of the population. 
During their late teens, many males experience considerable personal 
conflict over their homosexual activities, because they have become more 
conscious of social reactions to such contacts. Particularly in that period, 
many individuals attempt to stop their homosexual relations, and try to 
make the heterosexual adjustments which society demands. Some of these 
individuals are, of course, successful, but in a certain number of cases they 




630 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


finally reach the point, somewhere in their middle twenties, where they 
conclude that it is too costly to attempt to avoid the homosexual, and 
consciously, deliberately and sometimes publicly decide to renew such 
activities. Another factor which certainly contributes to the decrease in 
active incidence in the early twenties is the fact that heterosexually oriented 
males are then marrying in great numbers, and this leaves an increas- 
ingly select group at older ages in the single population. 

The active incidence figures are highest among single males of the high 
school level (Table 90). In the late teens nearly every other male of this 
level (41%) is having some homosexual contact, and between the ages of 
26 and 30 it is had by 46 per cent of the group. Among the males of the 
grade school level about 1 in 4 (22 to 27%) has any homosexual experience 
in any age period of the pre-marital years. Among the males who belong 
to the college level only about 1 in 5 has homosexual experience between 
adolescence and 15 (22%), 1 in 6 (16%) has such relations in the later teens, 
and less than 1 in 10 (10%) has homosexual relations between the ages of 
21 and 25. Among males who never go beyond grade school, about the 
same number of individuals is involved while they are actually in grade 
school, during their late teens when they are out of school, and in all the 
subsequent years until they marry. Among the males who stop their school- 
ing at high school levels a larger number is involved after they have left 
school. For the males who belong to the college level, the largest number 
is involved while they are in high school, but the number steadily decreases 
in later years. 

Homosexual activities occur in a much higher percentage of the males 
who became adolescent at an early age; and in a definitely smaller per- 
centage of those who became adolescent at later ages (Tables 77, 78, 
Figure 94). For instance, at the college level, during early adolescence 
about 28 per cent of the early-adolescent boys are involved, and only 14 
per cent of the boys who were late in becoming adolescent. This difference 
is narrowed in successive age periods, but the boys who became adolescent 
first are more often involved even ten and fifteen years later. It is to be 
recalled (Chapter 9) that these early-adolescent boys are the same ones 
who have the highest incidences and frequencies in masturbation and in 
heterosexual contacts. It is the group which possesses on the whole the 
greatest sex drive, both in early adolescence and throughout most of the 
subsequent periods of their lives. 

Homosexual activities occur less frequently among rural groups and 
more frequently among those who live in towns or cities (Table 123). 
On the other hand, it has already been pointed out (Chapter 12) that this 
is a product not only of the greater opportunity which the city may provide 
for certain types of homosexual contacts, but also of the generally lower 
rate of total outlet among males raised on the farm. It has also been pointed 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


631 


out that in certain of the most remote rural areas there is considerable 
homosexual activity among lumbermen, cattlemen, prospectors, miners, 
hunters, and others engaged in out-of-door occupations. The homosexual 
activity rarely conflicts with their heterosexual relations, and is quite 
without the argot, physical manifestations, and other affectations so often 
found in urban groups. There is a minimum of personal disturbance or 
social conflict over such activity. It is the type of homosexual experience 
which the explorer and pioneer may have had in their histories. 

On the whole, homosexual contacts occur most frequently among the 
males who are not particularly active in their church connections. They 
occur less frequently among devout Catholics, Orthodox Jewish groups, 
and Protestants who are active in the church. The differences are not always 
great, but lie constantly in the same direction. 

Among married males the highest incidences of homosexual activity 
appear to occur between the ages of 16 and 25, when nearly 10 per cent 
of the total population of married males (U.S. Correction) is involved 
(Table 66, Figure 85). The available data seem to indicate that the per- 
centage steadily drops with advancing age, but we have already suggested 
that these figures are probably unreliable. Younger, unmarried males have 
regularly given us some record of sexual contacts with older, married males. 

Many married males with homosexual experience currently in their 
histories have, undoubtedly, avoided us, and it has usually been impossible 
to secure hundred percent groups of older married males, especially from 
males of assured social position, primarily because of the extra-marital 
intercourse which they often have, and sometimes because some of them 
have active homosexual histories. About 10 per cent of the lower level 
married males have admitted homosexual experience between the ages of 
16 and 20. About 13 per cent of the high school level has admitted such 
experience after marriage and between the ages of 21 and 25. Only 3 per 
cent of the married males of college level have admitted homosexual 
experience after marriage — mostly between the ages of 31 and 35. It has 
been impossible to calculate accumulative incidence figures for these 
several groups, but they must lie well above the active incidence figures 
just cited. 

Finally, it should be noted that there is no evidence that the homosexual 
involves more males or, for that matter, fewer males today than it did 
among older generations, at least as far back as the specific record in the 
present study goes (Chapter 11, Tables 100, 104, Figure 112). 

FREQUENCIES 

Since the incidence of the homosexual is high, and since it accounts for 
only 8 to 16 per cent of the total orgasms of the unmarried males (Tables 
66, 96, 97, Figures 84, 126, 128-130) and for a rather insignificant portion 



632 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


of the outlet of the married males (Figures 131-133), it is obvious that the 
mean frequencies must be low in the population as a whole. Even when the 
calculations are confined to those males who are having actual experience, 
the average frequencies are never high. 

These low rates are in striking discord with the fact that homosexual 
contacts could in actuality be had more abundantly than heterosexual 
contacts, if there were no social restraints or personal conflicts involved. 
The sexual possibilities of the average male in his teens or twenties are 
probably more often assayed by males than by females, and younger males 
who are attractive physically or who have attractive personalities may be 
approached for homosexual relations more often than they themselves 
would ever approach females for heterosexual relations. A homosexually 
experienced male could undoubtedly find a larger number of sexual partners 
among males than a heterosexually experienced male could find among 
females. It is, of course, only the experienced male who understands that 
homosexual contacts are so freely available. The considerable taboo which 
society places upon these activities and upon their open discussion leaves 
most people in ignorance of the channels through which homosexual 
contacts are made; and even among males who desire homosexual rela- 
tions, there are only a relatively few who have any knowledge of how to 
find them in abundance. Consequently, many homosexual individuals may 
go for months and even for years at a stretch without a single contact 
which is carried through to orgasm. 

The heterosexual male finds a regular outlet if he locates a single female 
who is acceptable as a wife in marriage. The homosexual male is more 
often concerned with finding a succession of partners, no one of whom will 
provide more than a few contacts, or perhaps not more than a single 
contact. Some promiscuous males with homosexual histories become so 
interested in the thrill of conquest, and in the variety of partners and in the 
variety of genital experiences that may be had, that they deliberately turn 
down opportunities for repetitions of contacts with any one person. This 
necessity for finding new partners may result in their going for some days 
or weeks without sexual relations. 

Even the most experienced homosexual males may be inhibited from 
making all the contacts that are available because of preferences for par- 
ticular sorts of partners. A male who has highly developed esthetic tastes, 
one who is emotionally very sensitive, one who. over-reacts to situations 
which do not entirely please him, one who develops a preference for a 
partner of a particular age or a particular social level, of a particular height 
or weight, with hair of a particular color, with particular genital qualities, 
or with other particular physical aspects — ^a male who refuses to have 
sexual relations except under particular circumstances, at particular hours 
of the day, and in particular sorts of environments — may turn down hun- 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 633 

dreds of opportunities for contacts before he finds the one individual with 
whom he accepts a relation. 

Many of the males who have homosexual histories are acutely aware 
that they are transgressing social custom and engaging in activity which 
has a certain amount of peril attached to it if it becomes known to the 
society in which they live. Consequently, many such males become over- 
sensitive to the precise situations under which they accept relationships. 
All of these handicaps make for discord between homosexual partners, 
and this lessens the number of opportunities for successful relations. 

Long-time relationships between two males are notably few. Long-time 
relationships in the heterosexual would probably be less frequent than 
they are, if there were no social custom or legal restraints to enforce 
continued relationships in marriage. But without such outside pressures 
to preserve homosexual relations, and with personal and social conflicts 
continually disturbing them, relationships between two males rarely sur- 
vive the first disagreements. 

There are some males whose homosexuality is undoubtedly the product 
of inherent or acquired timidity or other personality traits which make it 
difficult for them to approach other persons for any sort of social contact. 
Such males find it easier to make contacts with individuals of their own 
sex. Their homosexuality may be the direct outcome of their social inade- 
quacies. Even with their own sex, however, these timid individuals may 
find it very difficult to approach strangers. They may resort to taverns, 
clubs, and other places where they know that homosexual contacts may be 
easily obtained, but are likely to go alone, and may go regularly for weeks 
and months without speaking to anyone in the assemblage. The low rates 
of outlet of some of these individuals are as extreme as any in the whole 
male population. 

There are some males who are primarily or even exclusively homosexual 
in their psychic responses, but who may completely abstain from overt 
relations for moral reasons or for fear of social difficulties. Left without 
any socio-sexual contacts, some of these persons have essentially no outlet, 
and some of them are, therefore, very badly upset. 

For these several reasons, average frequencies among males with homo- 
sexual histories are usually low, and there are very few high frequencies. 
In any particular age group, in any segment of the population, it is never 
more than about 5.5 per cent of the males who are having homosexual 
relations that average more than once every other day (3.5 per week). 
Calculating only for the males who actually have homosexual experience, 
there are never more than 5.2 per cent that have frequencies averaging more 
than 6.0 per week during their most active years. Considering that it is 
25 per. cent of the entire population which has total sexual outlets which 




Figure 159, Homosexual outlet: individual variation in frequencies, among 
single males, at ages adolescent-15 and 16-20, for three educational levels 


Showing percent of each population (vertical line) which has homosexual experience 
with each type of frequency (horizontal line). 

634 








Figure 160. Homosexual outlet: individual variation in frequencies among single 
males, at ages 21-25 and 26-30, for three educational levels 


Showing percent of each population (vertical line) which has homosexual experience 
with each type of frequency (horizontal line). 

635 




636 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


average more than 3.5 per week, and considering that 24 per cent of the 
married males have outlets that average more than 6.0 per week in then- 
most active period, it is apparent that outlets from the homosexual are 
definitely low. 

Among single males who are having homosexual experience the average 
frequencies rise from 0.8 per week in early adolescence to about 1.3 per 
week at age 25 and 1.7 per week by age 35 (Table 58). Since the frequencies 
of total sexual outlet steadily decrease with advancing age (Chapter 7), it 
is to be noted that the homosexual supplies an increasing proportion of 
the orgasms for the single males who are having such contacts: 17.5 per 
cent of the orgasms in early adolescence, 30.3 per cent in the early twenties, 
40.4 per cent by age 40 (Table 58). This increased dependence of this older 
male upon his homosexual outlet parallels the increased dependence which 
the heterosexual male places upon coitus as a source of outlet (Chapter 7). 
The situation is, however, accentuated in the case of the homosexual be- 
cause the younger male may be restrained by considerable doubts as to 
the advisability of continuing in a socially taboo activity (Figures 162- 
167). See the discussion in this chapter on Incidences, 

The frequencies of homosexual contacts differ considerably at different 
social levels (Tables 90, 114, Figure 105). The least frequent activity is to 
be found in the college level. Comparing active populations of college and 
high school levels, there is 50 to 100 per cent more frequent activity among 
the males of the high school group. The grade school level stands inter- 
mediate between the other two groups. The differences between the social 
levels are most marked in the early age periods. 

The considerable amount of homosexual experienc'=‘ among males of 
the high school level is a matter for especial note. See the discussion in 
Chapter 10 (p. 384). 

Between rural and urban groups the frequencies of homosexual contacts 
differ in the same way that the incidences differ (c/. above). The contacts 
are less frequent among the farm boys of grade school and high school 
level (Table 123); but for the college level there are almost no differences 
between the two groups (Chapter 12). 

Homosexual contacts, among those males who are having such relations, 
occur less frequently (in most groups) among persons who are actively 
interested in the church (Table 131), more frequently among those who 
have least to do with rehgious activities (Chapter 13). 

THE HETEROSEXUAL-HOMOSEXUAL BALANCE 

Concerning patterns of sexual behavior, a great deal of the thinking 
done by scientists and laymen alike stems from the assumption that there 
are persons who are “heterosexual” and persons who are “homosexual,” 
that these two types represent antitheses in the sexual world, and that 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


637 


there is only an insignificant class of ^‘bisexuals” who occupy an inter- 
.mediate position between the other groups. It is implied that every in- 
dividual is innately — inherently — either heterosexual or homosexual. It is 
further implied that from the time of birth one is fated to be one thing or 
the other, and that there is little chance for one to change his pattern in the 
course of a lifetime. 

It is quite generally believed that one’s preference for a sexual partner 
of one or the other sex is correlated with various physical and mental 
qualities, and with the total personality which makes a homosexual male 
or female physically, psychically, and perhaps spiritually distinct from a 
heterosexual individual. It is generally thought that these qualities make 
a homosexual person obvious and recognizable to any one who has a 
sufficient understanding of such matters. Even psychiatrists discuss “the 
homosexual personality” and many of them believe that preferences for 
sexual partners of a particular sex are merely secondary manifestations of 
something that lies much deeper in the totality of that intangible which 
they call the personality. 

It is commonly believed, for instance, that homosexual males are rarely 
robust physically, are uncoordinated or delicate in their movements, or 
perhaps graceful enough but not strong and vigorous in their physical 
expression. Fine skins, high-pitched voices, obvious hand movements, a 
feminine carriage of the hips, and peculiarities of walking gaits are sup- 
posed accompaniments of a preference for a male as a sexual partner. It is 
commonly believed that the homosexual male is artistically sensitive, 
emotionally unbalanced, temperamental to the point of being unpredict- 
able, difficult to get along with, and undependable in meeting specific 
obligations. In physical characters there have been attempts to show that 
the homosexual male has a considerable crop of hair and less often be- 
comes bald, has teeth which are more like those of the female, a broader 
pelvis, larger genitalia, and a tendency toward being fat, and that he lacks 
a linea alba. The homosexual male is supposed to be less interested in 
athletics, more often interested in music and the arts, more often engaged 
in such occupations as bookkeeping, dress design, window display, hair- 
dressing, acting, radio work, nursing, religious service, and social work. 
The converse to all of these is supposed to represent the typical hetero- 
sexual male. Many a clinician attaches considerable weight to these things 
in diagnosing the basic heterosexuality or homosexuality of his patients. 
The characterizations are so distinct that they seem to leave little room for 
doubt that homosexual and heterosexual represent two very distinct types 
of males. 

The Terman-Miles scale for determining the degree of masculinity or 
femininity of an individual (Terman and Miles 1936) is largely based upon 
these preconceptions. Some other psychology scales have utilized very 



638 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


much the same principles. While these scales have made it more apparent 
that there may be gradations between exclusively heterosexual and ex- 
clusively homosexual individuals, or between the extremes of masculinity 
and the extremes of femininity, the implication is always present that an 
individuars choice of a sexual partner is closely related to the masculinity 
or femininity of his personality. 

It should be pointed out that scientific judgments on this point have been 
based on little more than the same sorts of impressions which the general 
public has had concerning homosexual persons. But before any suflacient 



RATINGS 


< 

X 


O 

:s 

o 

X 


Figure 161. Heterosexual-homosexual rating scale 


Based on both psychologic reactions and overt experience, individuals rate as follows: 

0. Exclusively heterosexual with no homosexual 

1 . Predominantly heterosexual, only incidentally homosexual 

2. Predominantly heterosexual, but more than incidentally homosexual 

3. Equally heterosexual and homosexual 

4. Predominantly homosexual, but more than incidentally heterosexual 

5. Predominantly homosexual, but incidentally heterosexual 

6. Exclusively homosexual 


study can be made of such possible correlations between patterns of sexual 
behavior and other qualities in the individual, it is necessary to understand 
the incidences and frequencies of the homosexual in the population as a 
whole, and the relation of the homosexual activity to the rest of the sexual 
pattern in each individual’s history. 

The histories which have been available in the present study make it 
apparent that the heterosexuality or homosexuality of many individuals 
is not an all-or-none proposition. It is true that there are persons in the 





HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


639 


population whose histories are exclusively heterosexual, both in regard to 
their overt experience and in regard to their psychicreactions. And there are 
individuals in the population whose histories are exclusively homosexual, 
both in experience and in psychic reactions. But the record also shows that 
there is a considerable portion of the population whose members have 
combined, within their individual histories, both homosexual and hetero- 
sexual experience and/or psychic responses. There are some whose hetero- 
sexual experiences predominate, there are some whose homosexual ex- 
periences predominate, there are some who have had quite equal amounts 
of both types of experience. 

Some of the males who are involved in one type of relation at one period 
in their lives, may have only the other type of relation at some later period. 
There may be considerable fluctuation of patterns from time to time. Some 
males may be involved in both heterosexual and homosexual activities 
within the same period of time. For instance, there are some who engage 
in both heterosexual and homosexual activities in the same year, or in the 
same month or week, or even in the same day. There are not a few indi- 
viduals who engage in group activities in which they may make simulta- 
neous contact with partners of both sexes. 

Males do not represent two discrete populations, heterosexual and 
homosexual. The world is not to be divided into sheep and goats. Not all 
things are black nor all things white. It is a fundamental of taxonomy that 
nature rarely deals with discrete categories. Only the human mind invents 
categories and tries to force facts into separated pigeon-holes. The living 
world is a continuum in each and every one of its aspects. The sooner we 
learn this concerning human sexual behavior the sooner we shall reach a 
sound understanding of the realities of sex. 

While emphasizing the continuity of the gradations between exclusively 
heterosexual and exclusively homosexual histories, it has seemed desirable 
to develop some sort of classification which could be based on the relative 
amounts of heterosexual and of homosexual experience or response in 
each history. Such a heterosexual-homosexual rating scale is shown in 
Figure 161. An individual may be assigned a position on this scale, for each 
age period in his fife, in accordance with the following definitions of the 
various points on the scale: 

0. Individuals are rated as O’s if they make no physical contacts which r^ult 
in erotic arousal or orgasm, and make no psychic responses to individuals ot 
their own sex. Their socio-sexual contacts and responses are exclusively with 
individuals of the opposite sex. 

1. Individuals are rated as I’s if they have only incidental homosexual con- 
tacts which have involved physical or psychic response, or incidental psyeme 
responses without physical contact. The great preponderance of their socio- 
sexual experience and reactions is directed toward individuals of the opposite sex. 
Such homosexual experiences as these individuals have may occur only u sing e 



640 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

CASES 

Heterosexual-Homosexual Rating : Active Incidence 

Single White Males — Educ. Level 0-8 



X 

0 

1 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 



% 

/o 

% 

% 

% 

% 

/o 

% 

5 

820 

92.6 

2.3 

0.1 

0.4 

1.5 

0.2 

0.1 

2.8 

6 

820 

87.1 

2,9 

0.5 

1.2 

2.8 

0.2 

0.1 

5.2 

7 

819 

81 8 

4.2 

1.5 

2.2 

3.2 

0.9 

0.2 

6.0 

8 

819 

73.7 

6.2 

1.8 

3.5 

4.3 

0.7 

0.4 

1 9.4 

9 

819 

70.5 

6.6 

1.8 

4.3 

4.6 

0.7 

0.5 

11.0 

10 

819 

62.9 

8.5 

2.2 

5.1 

5.9 

1.0 

0.5 

1 13.9 

11 

818 

59.9 

10.0 

2.3 

5.4 

6.4 

1.1 

0.5 

! 14.4 

12 

815 

54.1 

13.3 

2.8 

6.5 

7.1 

1.2 

0.6 

14.4 

13 

814 

49.8 

20.8 

2,3 

5.7 

6.0 

1.6 

0.9 

‘ 12.9 

14 

811 

41.2 

31.4 

1.7 

5.3 

6.3 

2.1 

1.4 

10.6 

15 

803 

32.0 

42.1 i 

2.7 

6.2 

5.1 

2.7 

1.5 

7.7 

16 

774 

24.1 

50.1 

3,7 

7.6 

5.0 

3.1 

1.4 

5.0 

17 

708 

17.5 

56.4 i 

4.0 

8.1 

5.1 

3.1 

1.4 

4.4 

18 

635 

11.5 

61.6 

4.6 

7.9 

5.7 

2.4 

1 1.7 

4.6 

19 

550 

10.7 

62.5 

4.5 

8.0 

6.0 

! 2.5 

1.3 

4.5 

20 

473 

9.3 

65.1 

3.2 

7.6 

5.7 

2.5 

1.5 

5.1 

21 

386 

8.8 

63.0 

4.1 

7.8 

6.7 

2.9 

2.3 

4.4 

22 

306 

7.8 

63.9 

4.2 

6.9 

7.8 

2.6 

2.6 

4.2 

23 

269 

8.2 

61.7 

4.5 

8.2 

7.4 

3.3 

1 2.6 

4.1 

24 

231 

6,1 

62.3 

4.3 

9.1 

8.2 

3.5 

1.3 

5.2 

25 

187 

4.8 i 

59.9 1 

7.0 

9.1 

9.1 

3.2 

1.6 

5.3 

26 

172 

2.3 

60.5 

5.8 

1 9.9 

10.5 

3.5 

1.7 

5.8 

27 

152 

2.6 

57.3 

7.2 

10.5 

11.2 

3.3 

2.6 

5.3 

28 

134 

2.2 

55.3 

6.0 

11.2 

13.4 

3.7 

1.5 

6.7 

29 

121 

2,5 

54.5 

5.8 

i 12.4 

13.2 

4.1 

1.7 

5.8 

30 

107 

1.9 

53,2 

6,5 

11.2 

13.1 

4.7 

1.9 

7.5 

31 

95 

2.1 

54.7 

5.3 

8.4 ' 

11.6 

7.4 

4.2 

6.3 

32 

87 

2.3 

54.2 

5.7 

8.0 

11.5 

5.7 

4.6 

8.0 

33 

78 

2.6 

56.4 

5.1 

10.3 

11.5 

2.6 

5.1 

6.4 

34 

72 

2.8 

58.2 

1.4 

11.1 

12.5 

2.8 

5.6 

5.6 

35 

68 

2.9 

58.8 

1.5 

11.8 

13.2 

1.5 

5.9 

4.4 

36 

65 

3.1 

58.5 

1.5 

10.8 

13.8 

1.5 

4.6 

6.2 

37 

59 

1.7 

57.5 

1.7 

11.9 

15.3 

1.7 

3.4 

6.8 

38 

57 

1.8 

57.8 

1.8 

12.3 

15.8 

0.0 

3.5 

7.0 

39 

50 

2.0 

64.0 

2.0 

8.0 

18.0 

0.0 

0.0 

6.0 


Table 141. Heterosexual-homosexual ratings for single white males of grade 

school level (0-8) 


These are active incidence figures for each rating at each age. For an explanation of 
the meanings of the ratings X, 0, 1, etc., see the accompanying text. If the percentages 
are added from the right-hand side of each line, the cumulated percents will show the 
portion of the population which rates 1 or more, or 2 or more, etc., in each age period. 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


641 


time or two, or at least infrequently in comparison to the amount of their hetero- 
sexual experience. Their homosexual experiences never involve as specific psychic 
reactions as they make to heterosexual stimuli. Sometimes the homosexual 
activities in which they engage may be inspired by curiosity, or may be more 
or less forced upon them by other individuals, perhaps when they are asleep or 
when they are drunk, or under some other peculiar circumstance. 

2. Individuals are rated as 2’s if they have more than incidental homosexual 
experience, and /or if they respond rather definitely to homosexual stimuli. Their 
heterosexual experiences and /or reactions still surpass their homosexual experi- 
ences and/or reactions. These individuals may have only a small amount of 
homosexual experience or they may have a considerable amount of it, but in 
every case it is surpassed by the amount of heterosexual experience that they 
have within the same period of time. They usually recognize their quite specific 
arousal by homosexual stimuli, but their responses to the opposite sex are still 
stronger. A few of these individuals may even have all of their overt experience 
in the homosexual, but their psychic reactions to persons of the opposite sex 
indicate that they are still predom'nantly heterosexual. This latter situation is 
most often found among younger males who have not yet ventured to have 
actual intercourse with girls, while their orientation is definitely heterosexual. 
On the other hand, there are some males who should be rated as 2’s because of 
their strong reactions to individuals of their own sex, even though they have 
never had overt relations with them. 

3. Individuals who are rated 3’s stand midway on the heterosexual-homo- 
sexual scale. They are about equally homosexual and heterosexual in their overt 
experience and/or their psychic reactions. In general, they accept and equally 
enjoy both types of contacts, and have no strong preferences for one or the other. 
Some persons are rated 3’s, even though they may have a larger amount of 
experience of one sort, because they respond psychically to partners of both 
sexes, and it is only a matter of circumstance that brings them into more frequent 
contact with one of the sexes. Such a situation is not unusual among single 
males, for male contacts are often more available to them than female contacts. 
Married males, on the other hand, find it simpler to secure a sexual outlet 
through intercourse with their wives, even though some of them may be as 
interested in males as they are in females. 

4. Individuals are rated as 4’s if they have more overt activity and/or psychic 
reactions in the homosexual, while still maintaining a fair amount of hetero- 
sexual activity and /or responding rather definitely to heterosexual stimuli. 

5. Individuals are rated 5’s if they are almost entirely homosexual in their 
overt activities and /or reactions. They do have incidental experience with the 
opposite sex and sometimes react psychically to individuals of the opposite sex. 

6. Individuals are rated as 6’s if they are exclusively homosexual, both in 
regard to their overt experience and in regard to their psychic reactions. 

It will be observed that this is a seven-point scale, with 0 and 6 as the 
extreme points, and with 3 as the midpoint in the classification. On opposite 
sides of the midpoint the following relations hold: 

0 is the opposite of 6 

1 is the opposite of 5 

2 is the opposite of 4 



642 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Heterosexual-Homosexual Rating : Active Incidence 
Single Whete Males — ^Educ. Level 9-12 


age 

CASES 


X 

0 

1 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

5 

631 

% 

90.8 

/o 

4.4 

% 

0.3 

% 

0.3 

% 

0.8 

% 

0.3 

% 

0.2 

% 

2.9 

6 

631 

83.0 

7.4 

0.8 

0.8 

2.2 

0.5 

0.2 

5.1 

7 

631 

77.7 

9.0 

1.1 

1.7 

3.0 

1.1 

0.2 

6.2 

8 

631 

72.0 

10.9 

1.0 

2.2 

3.6 

1.1 

0.3 

. 8.9 

9 

631 

68.7 

9.7 

1.1 

2.7 

4.8 

1.1 

0.3 

11.6 

10 

631 

59.3 

11.9 

1.7 

3.2 

5.5 

1.4 

0.5 

16.5 

11 

631 

56.0 

12.7 

3.2 

4.1 

5.5 

1.4 

1.1 

16.0 

12 

631 1 

46.1 

18.9 

4.0 

5.5 

6.0 

2.2 

1.1 

16.2 

13 

631 

40.3 

26.1 

4.0 

5.2 

6.0 

2.2 

1.6 

14.6 

14 

631 

27.9 

40.5 

3.6 

5.5 

4.8 

3.0 

3.0 

11.7 

15 

629 

19.9 

48.6 

4.3 

6.5 

4.8 

4.6 

3.5 

7.8 

16 

619 

9.7 

56.6 

6.5 

8.1 

4.7 

4.7 

3.6 

6.5 

17 

577 

4.9 

56.2 

7.3 

11.1 

4.5 

5.2 

4.0 

6.8 

18 

502 

3.0 

57.0 

6.6 

10.8 

6.0 

5.4 

4.6 

7.0 

19 

420 

2.4 

55.2 

6.9 

11.7 

5.0 

5.2 

6.0 

7.6 

20 

350 

2.0 

57.0 

5.4 

10.9 

4.6 

4.9 

6.6 

8.6 

21 

274 

1.5 

56.9 

6.6 

8.8 

3.6 

5.8 

7.3 

9.5 

22 

232 

1.7 

56.5 

6.9 

8.2 

3.9 

5.2 

7.3 

10.3 

23 

197 

1.5 

55.4 1 

7.1 

6.1 

5.1 

7.1 

9.1 

8.6 

24 

166 

1.8 

51.4 

7.8 

6.6 

6.0 

8.4 

8.4 

9.6 

25 

140 

0.7 

50 8 1 

6.4 

5.7 

5.7 

9.3 

10.7 

10.7 

26 

125 

1.6 

50.4 

6.4 

5.6 

2.4 

10.4 

9.6 

13.6 

27 

113 

0.9 

52.3 

6.2 

4.4 

3.5 

8.8 

9.7 

14.2 

28 

97 

1.0 

52.5 i 

5.2 

2.1 

3.1 

12.4 

9.3 

14.4 

29 

82 

1.2 

48.8 

7.3 

2.4 

3.7 

11.0 

8.5 

17.1 

30 

67 

1.5 

46.2 

9.0 

3.0 

3.0 

10.4 

9.0 

17.9 

31 

58 

0.0 

48.3 

6.9 

5.2 

3.4 

6.9 

8.6 

20.7 


Table 142. Heterosexual-homosexual ratings for single white males of high 

school level (9-12) 

These are active incidence figures for each rating at each age. For an explanation of 
the meanings of the ratings X, 0, 1, etc., see the accompanying text. If the percentages 
are added from the right-hand side of each line, the cumulated percents will show the 
portion of the population which rates 1 or more, or 2 or more, etc., in each age period. 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


643 


AGE 

CASES 

Heterosexual-Homosexual Rating: Active Incidence 
Single White Males — ^Educ. Level 13+ 



X 

0 

1 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

5 

2846 

% 

85.8 

■% 

7.3 

% 

0.2 

% 

0.2 

% 

2.2 

% 

0.6 

% 

0.2 

% 

3.5 

6 

2846 

82.5 

7.7 

0.3 

0.6 

3.0 

0.7 

0.2 

5.0 

7 

2846 

79.5 

8.0 

0.3 

0.7 

3.1 

1.0 

0.5 

6.9 

8 

2846 

73.1 

9.5 

0.5 

1.2 

4.1 

1.2 

0.6 

9.8 

9 

2846 

72.2 

9.1 

0.6 

1.3 

4.3 

1.3 

0.6 

10.6 

10 

2846 

63.9 

11.3 

0.9 

1.9 

5.5 

1.7 

0.7 

14.1 

11 

2846 

61.7 

12.4 

i 1.1 

2.1 

5.0 

1.9 

0.8 

15.0 

12 . 

2846 

51.5 

19.7 

1.4 

2.5 

5.2 

2.4 

1.0 

16.3 

13 

2846 

42.5 

31.8 

1.9 

2.5 

5.1 

3.0 

1.4 

11.8 

14 

2846 

30.0 

47.6 

3.0 

3.0 

3.7 

3.0 

1.5 

8.2 

15 

2846 

22.3 

59.1 

3.3 

2.9 

2.8 

2.7 

1.4 

5.5 

16 ‘ 

2843 

13.8 ' 

68.9 

4.0 

3.0 

2.5 

2.5 

1.7 

1 3.6 

17 

2839 

8.9 

75.7 

3.9 

2.8 

2.0 

2.2 

1.7 

2.8 

18 

2755 

5.7 

79.5 

4.8 

2.5 

1.6 

1.8 

1.8 

2.3 

19 

2579 

4.3 

81.0 

5.2 

2.3 

1.6 

1.8 

1.8 

2.2 

20 

2306 

3.9 

80.6 

5.6 

2.6 

1.3 

1.8 ’ 

1.9 

2.3 

21 

1961 

3.6 

81.4 

5.2 

2.5 

1.0 

1.9 

2.0 

2.4 

22 

1527 

3.2 

80.4 

5.4 

2.3 

1.0 

2.2 

2.4 

3.1 

23 

1201 

2.9 

80.5 

4.5 

2.2 

0.9 

2.4 

2.9 

3.7 

24 

895 

3.2 

77.5 

5.1 

2.1 

1.3 

3.4 

3.0 

4.4 

25 

687 

3.3 

75.5 

5.5 

2.3 i 

1.3 

3.3 

4.1 

4.7 

26 

517 

3.7 

71.9 

5.8 

2.9 

1.4 

3.1 

5.0 

6.2 

27 

381 

3.7 

68.3 

7.1 

1.8 

1.3 

3.9 

5.5 

8.4 

28 

303 

4.3 

67.0 

6.6 

0.3 

1.7 

3.6 

6.6 

9.9 

29 

240 

4.2 

67.4 

5.0 

0.4 

1.3 

2.1 

7.5 

12.1 

30 

179 

4.5 

64.7 

4.5 

0.6 

0.6 

2.8 

9.5 

12.8 

31 

140 

5.7 

64.9 

3.6 

0.7 

0.0 

2.9 

7.9 

14.3 

32 

119 

6.7 

63.1 

4,2 

0.8 

0.0 

3.4 

8.4 

13.4 

33 

100 

7.0 

59.0 

5.0 

1.0 

0.0 

4.0 

7.0 

17.0 

34 

80 

6.3 

56.2 

5.0 

0.0 

0.0 

5.0 

7.5 

20.0 

35 

71 

7.0 

55.0 

5.6 

0.0 

1.4 

2.8 

8.5 

19.7 

36 

58 

6.9 

48.4 

5.2 

0.0 

1.7 

3.4 

10.3 

24.1 

37 

56 

7.1 

48.2 

5.4 

0.0 

1.8 

3.6 

10.7 

23.2 

38 

51 

5.9 

51.0 

3.9 

0.0 

0.0 

3.9 

9.8 

25.5 


Table 143. Heterosexual-homosexual ratings for single white males of college 

level (13 +) 

These are active incidence figures for each rating at each age. For an explanation of 
the meanings of the ratings X, 0, 1, etc., see the accompanying text. If the percentages 
are added from the right-hand side of each line, the cumulated percents will show the 
portion of the population which rates 1 or more, or 2 or more, etc., in each age period. 




AGE 



5 10 (5 20 25 30 35 40 


AGE 

Figures 162-164. Active incidence curves: heterosexual-homosexual ratings, 
by age and educational level, among single males 
Top figure, 162, shows percent of single males who have at least incidental (or more) 
homosexual reactions or experience (ratings 1-6) in each year. Middle figure, 163, shows 
percent of single males who have more than incidental homosexual reactions or experi- 
ence (ratings 2-6). Bottom figure, 164, shows percent of single males who have as much 
as or more homosexual than heterosexual reactions or experience (ratings 3-6), in each 
year. 








Figures 165-167. Active incidence curves: heterosexual-homosexual ratings, by 
age and educational level, among single males 
Top figure, 165, shows percent of single males who have more homosexual than 
heterosexual reactions or experience (ratings 4-6) in each year. Middle figure, 166, 
shows percent of single males who have more or less exclusively homosexual reactions 
or experience (ratings 5-6). Bottom figure, 167, shows percent of single males who are 
exclusively homosexual (rating 6) in each year. 

645 



646 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

CASES 

Heterosexual - Homosexual Rating : Active Incidence 
Married White Males — Educ . Level 0-8 

X 

0 

1 

2 

3 

4 



% 

% 

% 

% 

% 

% 

18 

68 

0.0 

85.3 

4.4 

7.4 

2.9 

0.0 

19 

103 

0.0 

91.3 

1,9 

3.9 

2.9 

0.0 

20 

134 

0.0 

90.3 

2.2 

3.0 

4.5 

0.0 

21 

181 

0.0 

90.1 

2.2 

5.5 

2.2 

0.0 

22 

223 

0.0 

88.8 

3.6 

5.8 

1.8 

0.0 

23 

222 

0.0 

91.8 

2.3 

4.1 

1.8 

0.0 

24 

228 

0.0 

91.2 

3.1 

3.5 

2.2 

0.0 

25 

238 

0.0 

92.0 

1.7 

4.2 

1.7 

0.4 

26 

223 

0.0 

96.0 

1.8 

1.8 

0.4 

0.0 

27 

232 

0.0 

92.7 

3.4 

3.0 

0.9 

0.0 

28 

232 

0.0 

92.7 

2.2 

4.7 

0.4 

0.0 

29 

209 

0.0 

93.3 

1.4 

4.3 

1.0 

0.0 

30 

207 

0.0 

94.7 

1.4 

2.9 

1.0 

0.0 

31 

194 

0.0 

92.8 

3.1 

3.1 

1.0 

0.0 

32 

192 

0.0 

91.2 

3.6 

3.6 

1.6 

0.0 

33 

179 

0.0 

92.7 

1.7 

3.9 

1.7 

0.0 

34 

177 

0.0 

93.8 

1.7 

4.5 

0.0 

0.0 

35 

162 

0.0 

95.7 

0.6 

3.7 

0.0 

0.0 

36 

157 

i 0.6 

93.0 

3.2 

3.2 

0.0 

0.0 

37 

146 

0.7 

93.8 

1.4 

3.4 

0.0 

0.7 

38 

139 

0.7 

94,3 

1.4 

2.9 

0.0 

0.7 

39 

126 

0.8 

92.8 

1.6 

4.0 

0.0 

0.8 

40 

125 

0.8 

92.8 

1.6 

4.0 

0.0 

0.8 

41 

113 

0.9 

94.6 

1.8 

1.8 

0.0 

0.9 

42 

111 

0.9 

94.6 

1.8 

1.8 

0.0 

0.9 

43 

99 

1,0 

93.8 

2.0 

2.0 

0.0 

1.0 

44 

84 

1.2 

95.2 

1.2 

1.2 

0.0 

1.2 

45 

75 

1.3 

96.1 

1.3 

1.3 

0.0 

0.0 

46 

75 

1.3 

96.1 

1.3 

1.3 

0.0 

0.0 

47 

74 

1.4 

95.9 

0.0 

2.7 

0.0 

0.0 

48 

71 

1.4 

97.2 

0.0 

1.4 

0.0 

0.0 

49 

65 

1.5 

97.0 

0.0 

1.5 

0.0 

0.0 


Table 144. Heterosexual-homosexual ratings for married white males of grade 

school level (0-8) 


These are active incidence figures for each rating at each age. For an explanation of 
the meanings of the ratings X, 0, 1, etc., see the accompanying text. If the percentages 
are added from the right-hand side of each line, the cumulated percents will show the 
portion of the population which rates 1 or more, or 2 or more, etc., in each age period. 




HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


647 


It will be observed that the rating which an individual receives has a 
dual basis. It takes into account his overt sexual experience and/or his 
psychosexual reactions. In the majority of instances the two aspects of 
the history parallel, but sometimes they are not in accord. In the latter 
case, the rating of an individual must be based upon an evaluation of the 
relative importance of the overt and the psychic in his history. 

In each classification there are persons who have had no experience or a 
minimum of overt sexual experience, but in the same classification there 
may also be persons who have had hundreds of sexual contacts. In every 
case, however, all of the individuals in each classification show the same 
balance between the heterosexual and homosexual elements in their his- 
tories. The position of an individual on this scale is always based upon the 
relation of the heterosexual to the homosexual in his history, rather than 
upon the actual amount of overt experience or psychic reaction. 

Finally, it should be emphasized again that the reality is a continuum, 
with individuals in the population occupying not only the seven categories 
which are recognized here, but every gradation between each of the cate- 
gories, as well. Nevertheless, it does no great injustice to the fact to group 
the population as indicated above. 

From all of this, it should be evident that one is not warranted in recog- 
nizing merely two types of individuals, heterosexual and homosexual, and 
that the characterization of the homosexual as a third sex fails to describe 
any actuality. 

It is imperative that one understand the relative amounts of the hetero- 
sexual and homosexual in an individual’s history if one is to make any 
significant analysis of him. Army and Navy officials and administrators in 
schools, prisons, and other institutions should be more concerned with the 
degree of heterosexuality or homosexuality in an individual than they are 
with the question of whether he has ever had an experience of either sort. 
It is obvious that the clinician must determine the balance that exists 
between the heterosexual and homosexual experience and reactions of his 
patient, before he can begin to help him. Even courts of law might well 
consider the totahty of the individual’s history, before passing judgment 
on the particular instance that has brought him into the hands of the 
law. 

Everywhere in our society there is a tendency to consider an individual 
“homosexual” if he is known to have had a single experience with another 
individual of his own sex. Under the law an individual may receive the 
same penalty for a single homosexual experience that he would for a 
continuous record of experiences. In penal and mental institutions a male 
is likely to be rated “homosexual” if he is discovered to have had a single 
contact with another male. In society at large, a male who has worked out 



648 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

CASES 

Heterosexual-Homosexual Rating: Active Incidence 
Married White Males — Educ. Level 9-12 



0 

1 

2 

3 

4 

5 



/o 

% 

% 

% 

% 

/o 

18 

51 

84.3 

3.9 

9.8 

2.0 

0.0 

0.0 

19 

73 

84.9 

4.1 

4.1 

4.1 

1.4 

. 1.4 

20 

84 

83.2 

4.8 

4.8 

4.8 

1.2 

1.2 

21 

99 

80.0 

4.0 

9.0 

6.0 

1.0 

0.0 

22 

101 

85.1 

1.0 

7.9 

5.0 

1.0 

0.0 

23 

104 

84.7 

1.9 

9.6 

3.8 

0.0 

0.0 

24 

102 

90.2 

2.0 

4.9 

2.9 

0.0 

0.0 

25 

107 

92.5 

1.9 

4.7 

0.9 

0.0 

0.0 

26 

107 

90.6 

1.9 

5.6 

1.9 

0.0 

0.0 

27 

99 

91.0 

2.0 

5.0 

0.0 

2.0 

0.0 

28 

96 

88.5 

4.2 

5.2 

0.0 

2.1 

0.0 

29 

90 

89.0 

4.4 

3.3 

0.0 

3.3 

0.0 

30 

80 

88.7 

3.8 

2.5 

0.0 

5.0 

0.0 

31 

79 

91.2 

2.5 

2.5 

0.0 

3.8 

0.0 

32 

74 

91.7 

1.4 

1.4 

1.4 

4.1 

0.0 

33 

69 

92.9 

1.4 

1.4 

1.4 

2.9 

0.0 

34 

63 

93.6 

3.2 

0.0 

0.0 

3.2 

0.0 

35 

58 

91.5 

3.4 

0.0 

1.7 

3.4 

0.0 

36 

58 

93.2 

3.4 

0.0 

0.0 

3.4 

0.0 

37 

53 

90.5 

3.8 

0.0 

0.0 

3.8 

0.0 

38 

50 

90.0 

4.0 

0.0 

0.0 

4.0 

0.0 


Table 145. Heterosexual-homosexual ratings for married white males of high 

school level (9-12) 


These are active incidence figures for each rating at each age. For an explanation of 
the meanings of the ratings 0, 1, etc., see the accompanying text. If the percentages 
are added from the right-hand side of each line, the cumulated percents will show the 
portion of thd population which rates 1 or more, or 2 or more, etc., in each age period. 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


649 


AGE 

CASES 

Heterosexual-Homosexual Rating : Active Incidence 
Married White Males — Educ. Level 13 + 

X 

0 

1 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

20 

59 

% 

0.0 

% 

96.6 

% 

0.0 

% 

3.4 

% 

0.0 

% 

0.0 

% 

0.0 

% 

0.0 

21 

97 

0.0 

91.7 

5.2 

2.1 

1.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

22 

172 

0.0 

90.1 

5.8 

2.3 

0 6 

0.0 

1.2 

0.0 

23 

221 

0.0 

90.9 

4.5 

1.8 

1.4 

0.9 

0.5 

0.0 

24 

278 

0.4 

91.9 

3.6 

2.2 

0.7 

0.4 

0.4 

0.4 

25 

‘ 330 

0.0 

91.3 

4.8 

1.8 

0.9 

0.6 

0.3 

0.3 

26 

378 

0.0 

93.4 

4.2 

0.8 

0.5 

0.5 

0.3 

0.3 

27 

391 

0.0 

91.5 

5.9 

0.5 

0.8 

0.3 

0.5 

0.5 

28 

392 

0.0 

90.0 

5.9 

1.5 

1.3 

0.3 

0.5 

0.5 

29 

385 

0.0 

90.4 

6.2 

1.6 

1.0 

0.3 

0.5 

0.0 

30 

386 

0.0 

89.9 

7.3 

1.0 

0.8 

0.5 

0.5 

0.0 

31 

387 

0.0 

91.0 

5.9 

1.1 

0.8 

0.8 

0.5 

0.0 

32 

366 

0.0 

90,7 

6.6 

1.1 

0.8 

0.5 

0.0 

0.0 

33 

340 

0.0 

91.1 

5.9 

1.8 

0.6 

0.6 

0.0 

0.0 

34 

323 

0.0 

89.8 

7.1 

1.9 

0.6 

0.6 

0.0 

0.0 

35 

300 

0.0 

90.2 1 

5.7 

2.7 

0.7 

0.7 

0.0 

0.0 

36 

283 

0.0 

90.5 

5.3 

2.8 

0.7 

0.7 

0.0 

0.0 

37 

248 

0.0 

90 8 

4.8 

2.8 

0.8 

0.8 

0.0 

0.0 

38 

235 

0,0 

90.1 

5.1 

3.0 

0.9 

0.9 

0.0 

0.0 

39 

218 

0.0 

89,0 

5.5 

3.2 

1.4 

0.9 

0.0 

0.0 

40 

205 

0.0 

89.2 

4.9 

3.4 

1.5 

1.0 

0.0 

0.0 

41 

190 

0.0 

90.5 

4.7 

2.6 

1.1 

1.1 

0.0 

0.0 

42 

182 

0.0 

90.2 

5.5 

2.7 

0.5 

1.1 

0.0 

0.0 

43 

154 

0.6 

89.7 

5.8 

2.6 

0.0 

1.3 

0.0 

0.0 

44 

145 

0.7 

91.0 

4.1 

2.8 

0.0 

1.4 

0.0 

0.0 

45 

128 

0.8 

91.3 

4.7 

1.6 

0.0 

1.6 

0.0 

0.0 

46 

117 

0.9 

92.2 

4.3 

1.7 

0.0 

0.9 

0.0 

0.0 

47 

113 

0.9 

92.0 

'4.4 

1.8 

0.0 

0.9 

0.0 

0.0 

48 

97 

1.0 

90.7 

6.2 

2.1 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

49 

93 

2.2 

90.2 

5.4 

2.2 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 


Table 146. Heterosexual-homosexual ratings for married white males of the 

college level (13+) 

These are active incidence figures for each rating at each age. For an explanation of 
the meanmgs of the ratings X, 0, 1, etc., see the accompanying text. If the percentages 
are added from the right-hand side of each line, the cumulated percents will show the 
portion of the population which rates 1 or more, or 2 or more, etc., in each age period. 



650 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


a highly successful marital adjustment is likely to be rated “homosexual” 
if the community learns about a single contact that he has had with another 
male. All such misjudgments are the product of the tendency to categorize 
sexual activities under only two heads, and of a failure to recognize the 
endless gradations that actually exist. 

From all of this, it becomes obvious that any question as to the number of 
persons in the world who are homosexual and the number who are hetero- 
sexual is unanswerable. It is only possible to record the number of those 
who belong to each of the positions on such a heterosexual-homosexual 
scale as is given above. Summarizing our data on the incidence of overt 
homosexual experience in the white male population (Tables 139 - 140 , and 
Figures 156 - 1 58 ) and the distribution of various degrees of heterosexual- 
homosexual balance in that population (Tables 141 - 150 , Figures 162 - 170 ), 
the following generalizations may be made: 

37 per cent of the total male population has at least some overt homosexual 
experience to the point of orgasm between adolescence and old age (Figure 156). 
This accounts for nearly 2 males out of every 5 that one may meet. 

50 per cent of the males who remain single until age 35 have had overt homo- 
sexual experience to the point of orgasm, since the onset of adolescence 
(Figure 156). 

58 per cent of the males who belong to the group that goes into high school but 
not beyond, 50 per cent of the grade school level, and 47 per cent of the college 
level have had homosexual experience to the point of orgasm if they remain 
single to the age of 35 (Figure 158). 

63 per cent of all males never have overt homosexual experience to the point 
of orgasm after the onset of adolescence (Figure 156). 

50 per cent of all males (approximately) have neither overt nor psychic experi- 
ence in the homosexual after the onset of adolescence (Figures 162-167). 

13 per cent of the males (approximately) react erotically to other males without 
having overt homosexual contacts after the onset of adolescence. 

30 per cent of all males have at least incidental homosexual experience or 
reactions (Le., rate 1 to 6) over at least a three-year period between the ages of 
16 and 55. This accounts for one male out of every three in the population who 
is past the early years of adolescence (Table 150, Figure 168). 

25 per cent of the male population has more than incidental homosexual experi- 
ence or reactions {Le., rates 2-6) for at least three years between the ages of 16 
and 55. In terms of averages, one male out of approximately every four has had 
or will have such distinct and continued homosexual experience. 

18 per cent of the males have at least as much of the homosexual as the hetero- 
sexual in their histories {Le,, rate 3-6) for at least three years between the ages 
of 16 and 55. This is more than one in six of the white male population. 

13 per cent of the population has more of the homosexual than the heterosexual 
(Le., rates 4-6) for at least three years between the ages of 16 and 55. This is one 
in eight of the white male population. 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


651 


10 per cent of the males are more or less exclusively homosexual {Le., rate 5 
or 6) for at least three years between the ages of 16 and 55. This is one male in 
ten in the white male population. 

8 per cent of the males are exclusively homosexual rate a 6) for at least 
three years between the ages of 16 and 55. This is one male in every 13. 

4 per cent of the white males are exclusively homosexual throughout their lives, 
after the onset of adolescence (Table 150, Figure 168). 

None of those who have previously attempted to estimate the incidence 
of the homosexual have made any clear-cut definition of the degree of 
homosexuality which they were including in their statistics. As a matter 
of fact, it seems fairly certain that none of them had any clear-cut concep- 


AGE 

CASES 

Heterosexual-Homosexual Rating : Active Incidence 
Total Population—U. S. Corrections 

X 

0 

1 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

5 1 

4297 

% 

90.6 

% 

4.2 

% 

0.2 

% 

0.3 

% 

1.2 

% 

0.3 

7o 

0.2 

% 

3.0 

10 

4296 

61.1 

10.8 

1.7 

3.6 

5.6 

1.3 

0 5 

15.4 

15 

4284 

23.6 

48.4 

3.6 

6.0 

4.7 

3.7 

2.6 

7.4 

20 

3467 

3.3 

69.3 

4.4 

7.4 

4.4 

2.9 

3.4 

4.9 

25 

1835 

1.0 

79.2 

3.9 

5.1 

3.2 

2.4 

2.3 

2.9 

30 

1192 

0.5 

83.1 

4.0 

3.4 

2.1 

3.0 

1.3 

2.6 

35 

844 

0.4 

86.7 

2.4 

3.4 1 

1.9 

1.7 

0.9 

2.6 

40 

576 

1.3 

86.8 

3.0 

3.6 1 

2.0 

0.7 

0.3 

2.3 

45 

382 

2.7 

88.8 

2.3 

2.0 

1.3 

0.9 

0.2 

1.8 


Table 147. Heterosexual-homosexual ratings for all white males 


These are active incidence figures for the entire white male population, including 
single, married, and post-marital histories, the final figure corrected for the distribution 
of the population in the U. S. Census of 1940. For further explanations see the legend 
for Table 141. 

tion of what they intended, other than their assurance that they were in- 
cluding only those “who were really homosexual.” For that reason it is 
useless to compare the 2 or 3 per cent figure of Havelock Ellis, or the 2 to 
5 per cent figure of Hirschfeld, or the 0.1 per cent figure of the Army 
induction centers with any of the data given above. The persons who are 
identified as “homosexuals” in much of the legal and social practice have 
rated anything between 1 and 6 on the above scale. On the other hand, there 
are some persons who would not rate an individual as “really homosexual” 
if he were anything less than a 5 or 6. Nevertheless, it should be emphasized 
again that there are persons who rate 2’s or 3’s who, in terms of the 
number of contacts they have made, may have had more homosexual 
experience than many persons who rate 6, and the clinician, the social 



652 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


AGE 

CASES 

Heterosexual-Homosexual Rating: Active Incidence 
Cumulated Percents 

X 1 0 1+ 

2+ 1 3+ 4+ 

1 5+ 1 6 

Single Males — ^Educ. Level 0-8 




% 

% 

% 

% 

% 

% 

% 

°/ 

/o 

15 i 

803 

32.0 

42.1 

25.9 

23.2 

17.0 

11.9 

9.2 

7.7 

20 

473 

9.3 

65.1 

25.6 

22.4 

14.8 

9.1 

6.6 

5.1 

25 

187 

4.8 

59,9 

35.3 

28.3 

19.2 

10.1 

6.9 

5.3 

30 

107 

1.9 

53.2 

44.9 

38.4 1 

27.2 ' 

14.1 

9.4 

7.5 

35 

68 

2.9 

58.8 

38.3 

36.8 

25.0 

11.8 

10.3 

4.4 


Single Males — ^Educ. Level 9-12 


15 

629 

19.9 j 

48.6 

31.5 

27.2 

20.7 

15.9 

11.3 

7.8 

20 

350 

2.0 

57.0 

41.0 

35.6 

24.7 

20.1 

15.2 

8.6 

25 

140 

0.7 

50.8 

48.5 

42.1 

36.4 

30.7 

21.4 

10.7 

30 

67 

1.5 

46.2 

52.3 

43.3 

40.3 

37.3 

26.9 

17.9 


Single Males — Educ. Level 13+ 


15 

2846 

22.3 i 

59.1 

18.6 

15.3 

12.4 

9.6 

6.9 

5.5 

20 

2306 

3.9 

80.6 

15.5 

9.9 

7.3 

6.0 

4.2 

2.3 

25 

687 

3.3 

75.5 

21.2 

15.7 

13.4 

12.1 

8.8 

4.7 

30 

179 

4.5 

64.7 

30.8 

26.3 

25.7 

25.1 

22.3 

12.8 

35 

71 

7.0 

55.0 

38.0 

32.4 

32.4 

31.0 

28.2 

19.7 


Married Males — ^Educ. Level 0-8 


20 

134 

0.0 

90.3 

9.7 

7.5 

4.5 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

25 

238 

0.0 

92.0 

8.0 

6.3 

2.1 

0.4 

0.0 

0.0 

30 

207 

0.0 

94.7 

5.3 

3.9 

1.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

35 

162 

0.0 

95.7 

4.3 

3.7 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

40 

125 

0.8 

92.8 

6.4 

4.8 

0.8 

0.8 

0.0 

0.0 

45 

75 

1.3 

96.1 

2.6 

1.3 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 


Married Males — ^Educ. Level 13 + 


20 

59 

0.0 

96.6 

3.4 

3.4 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

25 

330 

0.0 

91.3 

8.7 

3.9 

2.1 

1.2 

0.6 

0.3 

30 

386 

0.0 

89.9 

10.1 

2.8 

1.8 

1.0 

0.5 

0.0 

35 

300 

0.0 

90.2 

9.8 

4.1 

1.4 

0.7 

0.0 

0.0 

40 

205 

0.0 

89.2 

10.8 

5.9 

2.5 

1.0 

0.0 

0.0 

45 

128 

0.8 

91.3 

1 

7.9 

3.2 

1.6 

1.6 

0.0 

0.0 


Total Population: Single, Married — All Educ. Levels 


15 

4284 

23.6 

48.4 

28.0 

24.4 

18.4 

13.7 

10.0 

7.4 

20 

3467 

3.3 

69.3 

27.4 

23.0 

15.6 

11.2 

8.3 

4.9 

25 

1835 

1.0 

79.2 

19.8 

15.9 

10.8 

7.6 

5.2 

2.9 

30 

1192 

0 5 

83.1 

16.4 

12.4 

9.0 

6.9 

3.9 

2.6 

35 

844 

0.4 

86.7 

12.9 

10.5 

7.1 

5.2 

3.5 

2.6 

40 

576 

1.3 

86.8 

11.9 

8.9 ’ 

5.3 ! 

3.3 

2.6 

2.3 

45 

382 

2.7 

88.8 

8.5 

6.2 

4.2 

2.9 1 

2.0 

1.8 


Table 148. Cumulated percents of heterosexual-homosexual ratings 


Based on Tables 141-147, cumulated from the right-hand end of each line. See 
accompanying text for definitions of ratings X, 0, 1, 2, etc. Data for adult males (at 
ages 15, 20, 25, 30, etc.), for single and married groups of each educational level and of 
total population. Shows percent of each group which rates 1 or more, 2 or more, 3 or 
more, etc., in each of the given years. 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


653 


Heterosexual-Homosexual Ratings: Accumulative Incidence 
All Males, Single and Married 


CURRENT RATING 

HIGHEST RATINGS FOR ANY 3 YEARS SINCE 1 6 

X 

0 

1 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 


Educ. Level 0-8 



1 

% 

7o 

% 

% 

% 

% 

% 

% 

16-20 

712 

11.2 

64.4 

2.9 

8.2 

5.2 

2.2 

1.7 

4.2 

16-25 

557 

3.8 

73.7 

2.9 

7.5 

5.9 

1.4 

1.4 

3.4 

16-30 

462 

1.1 

77.7 

2.8 

7,6 

4.5 

1.3 

1.1 

3.9 

16-35 

357 

0.8 

80.1 

2.8 

6.2 

4.2 

1.4 

1.1 

3.4 

16^0 

288 

0.3 

83.7 

1.4 

5.9 

3.5 

1.0 

0.7 

3.5 

16^5 

205 

0.5 

84.8 

1.0 

5.4 

2.4 

1.5 

0.5 

3.9 

16-50 

147 

0.7 

84.9 

1.4 

4.8 

2.0 

0.7 

0.7 

4.8 

16-55 

98 

1.0 

84.8 

0.0 

5.1 

2.0 

1.0 

1.0 

5.1 


Educ. Level 9-12 


16-20 

584 

2.7 

61,2 

5.3 

! 10.4 

4.8 

5.0 

3.4 

7.2 

16-25 

347 

0.9 

63.4 

4.9 

8.1 

4.9 

4.9 

4.0 

8.9 

16-30 

238 

0.4 

67.2 

4.6 

5.9 

3.4 

4.2 

3.4 

10.9 

16-35 

148 

0.0 

70.3 

4.7 

6.1 

3.4 

2.7 

2.0 

10.8 

16-40 

92 

0.0 

72.8 

4.3 

4.3 

3.3 

3.3 

1.1 

10.9 

16-45 

61 

0.0 

77.1 

3.3 

3.3 

1.6 

3 3 

1.6 

9.8 


Educ. Level 13+ 


16-20 

2979 

5.3 

80.3 

3.8 

2,4 

1.7 

2.1 

1.9 

2.5 

16-25 

1635 

2.1 

80.1 

4.3 

2.5 

1.3 

2.9 

2.7 

4.1 

16-30 

876 

1.4 

77.8 

4.6 

2.1 , 

1.7 

2.6 

3.4 

6.4 

16-35 

552 

1.1 

80.7 

4.2 

2,4 

1.1 

2.5 

2.0 

6.0 

16-40 

375 

0.5 

81.4 

4.5 

1.3 

0.8 

2.1 

2.2 

7.2 

16-45 

218 

0.5 

82.1 

6.0 

1.8 

0.9 

0.9 

1.8 

6.0 

16-50 

132 

0.0 

87.1 

4.5 

2.3 

1.5 

0.8 

0.0 

3.8 

16-55 

74 

0.0 

86.5 

4.1 

2.7 

! 

1.3 

1.3 

0.0 

4.1 


Total Population — U. S. Correction 


16-20 

4275 

5.7 

64.8 

4.4 

8.6 

4.5 

3.7 

2.7 

5.6 

16-25 

2539 

2.0 

69.0 

4.2 

7.1 

4.7 

3.5 

3.0 

6.5 

16-30 

1576 

0.8 

72.7 

3.9 

6.0 

3.6 

2.9 

2.5 

7.6 

16-35 

1057 

0.5 

76.3 

3.8 

5.6 

3.4 

2.1 

1.6 

6.7 

16-40 

755 

0.2 

79.9 

2.7 

4.8 

3.1 

1.9 

1.0 

6.4 

16-45 

484 

0.4 

82.3 

2.2 

4.4 

2.0 

1.9 

1.0 

5.8 

16-50 

318 

0.5 

83.8 

2.6 

4.0 

2.1 

1.1 

0.5 

5.4 

16-55 

191 

0.7 

85.0 

1.4 

4.9 

1.5 

0.8 

0.7 

5.0 


Table 149. Heterosexual-homosexual ratings: accumulative incidence for each 

rating 

Showing percents of each population which have ever held the shown rating for at 
least three years between 16 and the age shown in column 1. By eliminatmg both pre- 
adolescent and early adolescent years from the calculations, and by limiting the table 
to cases which have had at least three years of homosexual ratings, the table has been 
limited to defimtely adult and pronouncedly homosexual experience of the degree 
shown by each rating. See Table 150 for cumulated percents derived from this table. 



AGE 

PERIOD 


CASES 


Heterosexual-Homosexual Ratings: Accumulative Incidence 
All Males, Single or Married 


current rating 

cumulated percents 

highest ratings for any 3 YEARS SINCE 16 

X 

0 

1-6 

2-6 

3-6 

4-6 

5-6 

6 


Educ. Level 0-8 




% 

7o 

% 

% 

0/ 

/o 

% 

% 

y 

/o 

16-20 

712 

11.2 

64.4 

24.4 

21.5 

13.3 

8.1 

5.9 

4.2 

16-25 

557 

3.8 

73.7 

22.5 

19.6 

12.1 

6.2 

4.8 

3.4 

16-30 

462 

1.1 

77.7 

21.2 

18.4 

10.8 

6.3 

5.0 

3.9 

16-35 

357 

0.8 

80.1 

19.1 

16.3 

10.1 

5.9 

4.5 

3.4 

16-40 

288 

0.3 

83.7 

16.0 

14.6 

8.7 

5.2 

4.2 

3.5 

16-45 

205 

0.5 

84.8 

14.7 

13.7 

8.3 

5.9 

4.4 

3.9 

16-50 

147 

0.7 

84.9 

14.4 

13.0 

8.2 

6.2 

5.5 

4.8 

16-55 

98 

1.0 

84.8 

14.2 

14.2 

9.1 

7.1 

6.1 

5.1 


Educ. Level 9-12 


16-20 

584 

2.7 

61.2 

36.1 

30 8 

20.4 

15 6 

10.3 

7.2 

16-25 ! 

347 

0.9 

63.4 

35.7 1 

30.8 ! 

22.7 

17.8 : 

12.9 

8.9 

16-30 i 

238 

0.4 

67.2 

32.4 

27.8 i 

21.9 

18.5 

14.3 

10.9 

16-35 

148 

0.0 1 

70.3 

29,7 1 

25.0 

18.9 

15.5 i 

12.8 

10.8 

16-40 

92 

0.0 

72.8 

27.2 

22.9 i 

18.6 

15.3 1 

12.0 

10.9 

16-45 

61 

0.0 1 

77.1 

22,9 1 

19.6 i 

16.3 

14.7 1 

11.4 

9.8 


Educ. Level li-f 


16-20 

2979 

5.3 

80 3 

14.4 

10.6 

8.2 

6.5 

4.4 

2.5 

16-25 

1635 

2.1 

80.1 

17.8 

13.5 

11.0 

9.7 

6.8 

4.1 

16-30 

876 

1.4 

77.8 

20.8 

16.2 

14.1 

12.4 

9.8 

6.4 

16-35 

552 

1.1 

80.7 

18.2 

14.0 

11.6 

10.5 

8.0 

6.0 

16-40 

375 

0.5 

81.4 

18.1 

13.6 

12.3 

11.5 

9.4 

7.2 

16-45 

218 

0.5 

82.1 

17.4 

11.4 

i 9.6 

8.7 

7.8 

6.0 

16-50 

132 

0.0 

87.1 

12.9 

8.4 

6.1 

4.6 

3.8 

3.8 

16-55 

74 

0.0 

86.5 

13.5 

9.4 

6.7 

5.4 

4.1 

4.1 


Total Population — ^U. S. Corrections 


16-20 

4275 

5.7 

64.8 i 

29.5 

25.1 

16.5 

12.0 

8.3 

5.6 

16-25 

2539 

2.0 

69.0 

29.0 

24.8 

17.7 

13.0 

9.5 

6.5 

16-30 

1576 

0.8 

72.7 

26.5 

22.6 

16.6 

13.0 

10.1 

7.6 

16-35 

1057 

0.5 

76.3 

23.2 

19.4 

13.8 

10.4 

8.3 

6.7 

16-40 

755 

0.2 

79.9 

19.9 

17.2 

12.4 

9.3 

7.4 

6.4 

16-45 

484 

0.4 

82.3 

17.3 

15.1 

10.7 

8.7 

6.8 

5.8 

16-50 

318 

0.5 1 

83.8 

15.7 

13.1 

9.1 

7.0 

5.9 

5.4 

16-55 

191 

0.7 i 

85.0 

14.3 

12.9 

8.0 

6.5 

5.7 

5.0 

Total U. S. Population — Corrected for All Ages 

16-55 

4275 

1.5 

75.6 

22.9 

19.6 

13.7 

10.4 

8.0 

6.2 


Table 150. Heterosexual-homosexual rating: cumulated percents on each rating 


Derived from Table 149. Showing percents of each population which have rated 
'*at least a I,’’ “at least a 2,” “at least a 3,” etc., for at least 3 years between 16 and 
the age shown in column 1. The final line of figures, for the total U. S. population 
corrected for all ages, shows how many males in any population may be expected to 
have ratings of each sort. 


654 


HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


655 


worker, court officials, and society in general are not infrequently con- 
cerned with persons who rate no more than 2’s or 3’s. Many who rate only 
1 or 2 are much disturbed over their homosexual experience, and they are 
frequently among those who go to clinicians for help. 

Finally, it should be emphasized that the social significance of an indi- 
vidual’s history may or may not have any relation to his rating on the above 
scale. An older male who has never before had homosexual contact, may 



Figure 168. Accumulative incidence of heterosexual-homosexual ratings in 
total male population (single and married), by age periods 

Based on U. S. Corrected data. Based only on ratings held by each individual for a 
period of at least three years. All accumulative incidence curves should rise; these drop 
in older age periods because (1) yoxmger males today may be more often involved in 
homosexual activity, or (2) older males forget their earlier experience, or (3) older males 
deliberately cover up their homosexual experience. Certainly the data for the earlier 
age periods are the most reliable. 


force a sexual relation with a small boy; and although he rates only a 1, 
he may so outrage the community that the full force of the law may be 
stirred up against him. On the contrary, most persons who rate I’s have 
histories which do not disturb anybody. At the other end of the scale, 
some of the exclusively homosexual males may so confine their overt 
contacts that no social problems are raised, while others who also rate 6 
are active wolves who are in continual trouble because of their open 
affronts to social conventions. 

22 


656 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


BISEXUALITY 

Since only 50 per cent of the population is exclusively heterosexual 
throughout its adult life, and since only 4 per cent of the population is 
exclusively homosexual throughout its hfe, it appears that nearly half 
(46%) of the population engages in both heterosexual and homosexual 
activities, or reacts to persons of both sexes, in the course of their adult 
lives. The term bisexual has been applied to at least some portion of this 
group. Unfortunately, the term as it has been used has never been strictly 
delimited, and consequently it is impossible to know whether it refers to 
all individuals who rate anything from 1 to 5, or whether it is being limited 



X 0 1-6 2-6 3-6 4-6 5-6 6 

Figure 169. Heterosexual-homosexual ratings in total male population (single 
and married) in any single year 


Based on U. S. Corrected data (last line of Table 150). Passing experiences eliminated 
from data by showing only ratings which have involved a period of at least three years 
after the males turned 16. Percent shown as “X” have no socio-sexual contacts or 
reactions. 

to some smaller number of categories, perhaps centering around group 3. 
If the latter is intended, it should be emphasized that the Ts, 2’s, 4’s, and 
5’s have not yet been accounted for, and they constitute a considerable 
portion of the population. 

In any event, such a scheme provides only a three-point scale (hetero- 
sexual, bisexual, and homosexual), and such a limited scale does not 
adequately describe the continuum which is the reality in nature. A seven- 
point scale comes nearer to showing the many gradations that actually 
exist. 

As previously pointed out, it is rather unfoftimate that the word bisexual 
should have been chosen to describe this intermediate group. The term is 
used as a substantive, designating individuals — ^persons; and the root mean- 







658 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


typical female in the population. Where a single individual combines 
in its one person the primary sex characters of two sexes (namely, the 
ovaries a;id the testes), it is recognized as a hermaphrodite. Where the 
secondary sexual characters of an individual are in part the unmodified 
characters of one sex, and in part the characters of the other sex, the 
individual is known as a gynandromorph, A gynandromorphic insect may 
have the head coloration that is typical of one sex and the thoracic colora- 
tion that is typical of the other sex. An intersex, on the contrary, has a 
portion or the whole of its structures intermediate in character between 
the structures of the typical male or the female of the species. In the case 
of Goldschmidt’s gypsy moths, the females are typically large, the males 



Figure 170. Development of heterosexuality and homosexuality by age periods 

Active incidence curves, corrected for U. S. population. Males with no socio-sexual 
response (rating X) rapidly disappear between the ages of 5 and 20. Males whose 
responses are chiefly heterosexual (rating 0 or 1) rapidly increase in number until they 
ultimately account for 90 per cent of the whole population. Males who are more than 
incidentally homosexual in response or overt activity (ratings 2-6) are most abundant 
in pre-adolescence and through the teens, gradually becoming less abundant with 
advancing age. 

typically smaller. The intersexual individuals show gradations in size 
between the larger female and smaller male. The typical female of the 
gypsy moth is buff yellow, the male is white. The intersexes show various 
grades of color between yellow and white. A gynandromorph might have 
one wing yellow and one wing white, one wing large and one wing small, 
but the intersexes have wings that are intermediate in size and color. 

In spite of the fact that Goldschmidt himself (1916) accepted the idea 
that the homosexual human male or female was an intersex, there is no 
adequate basis for reaching any such conclusion. Those who have accepted 
this interpretation have assumed without asking for specific evidence that 




HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


659 


an individual’s choice of a sexual partner is affected by some basic physi- 
ologic capacity. No work that has been done on hormones or on any other 
physiologic capacities of the human animal justifies such a conclusion 
(Kinsey 1941). Goldschmidt and others who have thought of the homo- 
sexual individual as an intersex have relied upon incidence figures which 
were pure guesses and which, as the data in the present chapter will show, 
bear little relation to the fact as it has now been ascertained. 

There are a few males in whom the urethra opens on the under surface 
of the penis. Such a condition is known as a hypospadia. The most extreme 
confusion of biological ideas has come from the identification of these 
hypospadiac males as intersexes who are predisposed to be homosexual 
in their behavior. However, an investigation of the embryonic develop- 
ment of the male penis (Arey 1924, 1946, Patten 1946) will show that a 
hypospadia is nothing more than a failure in the closure of the urethra at 
the end of normal embryonic development, and has no relation whatsoever 
to the genetic maleness or femaleness of the individual, nor to the endo- 
crine constitution of the individual. As our own histories of hypospadiac 
individuals definitely show, such malformations have nothing to do with 
their choice of sexual partners unless, as in some extreme cases among 
ignorant and uneducated persons, the sexual identity of the individual is 
confused and he is raised in the clothing and the traditions of the opposite 
sex. In popular parlance such individuals are commonly called “mor- 
phodites,” but the designation is incorrect, for a true hermaphrodite, as 
we have already pointed out, has functioning gonads of both sexes within 
its one body. It is, of course, in the same way that a female with a large 
clitoris is sometimes called a hermaphrodite. Sometimes the term intersex 
has been apphed to such females (Dickinson 1933); but until more is 
known about the biological basis of this situation, it is not certain that 
the term intersex should be apphed even in these cases. 

SCIENTIFIC AND SOCIAL IMPLICATIONS 

In view of the data which we now have on the incidence and frequency 
of the homosexual, and in particular on its co-existence with the hetero- 
sexual in the fives of a considerable portion of the male population, it is 
diflScult to maintain the view that psychosexual reactions between indi- 
viduals of the same sex are rare and therefore abnormal or unnatural, or 
that they constitute within themselves evidence of neuroses or even 
psychoses. 

If homosexual activity persists on as large a scale as it does, in the face 
of the very considerable public sentiment against it and in spite of the 
severity of the penalties that our Anglo-American culture has placed upon 
it through the centuries, there seems some reason for believing that such 
activity would appear in the histories of a much larger portion of the 



660 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


population if there were no social restraints. The very general occurrence 
of the homosexual in ancient Greece (Licht 1925, 1926, 1928, 1932), and 
its wide occurrence today in some cultures in which such activity is not as 
taboo as it is in our own, suggests that the capacity of an individual to 
respond erotically to any sort of stimulus, whether it is provided by another 
person of the same or of the opposite sex, is basic in the species. That 
patterns of heterosexuality and patterns of homosexuality represent 
learned behavior which depends, to a considerable degree, upon the mores 
of the particular culture in which the individual is raised, is a possibility 
that must be thoroughly considered before there can be any acceptance of 
the idea that homosexuality is inherited, and that the pattern for each 
individual is so innately fixed that no modification of it may be expected 
within his lifetime. 

The opinion that homosexual activity in itself provides evidence of a 
psychopathic personahty is materially challenged by these incidence and 
frequency data. Of the 40 or 50 per cent of the male population which has 
homosexual experience, certainly a high proportion would not be con- 
sidered psychopathic personalities on the basis of anything else in their 
histories. It is argued that an individual who is so obtuse to social reactions 
as to continue his homosexual activity and make it any material portion 
of his life, therein evidences some social incapacity; but psychiatrists and 
clinicians in general might very well re-examine their justification for 
demanding that aU persons conform to particular patterns of behavior. 
As a matter of fact, there is an increasing proportion of the most skilled 
psychiatrists who make no attempt to re-direct behavior, but who devote 
their attention to helping an individual accept himself, and to conduct 
himself in such a manner that he does not come into open conflict with 
society. 

There are, of course, some persons with homosexual histories who are 
neurotic and in constant difficulty with themselves and not infrequently 
with society. That is also true of some persons with heterosexual histories. 
Some homosexual individuals are so upset that they have difficulty in the 
accomplishment of their business or professional obhgations and reach 
the point where they find it difficult to make the simplest sort of social 
contact without friction. It is, however, a considerable question whether 
these persons have homosexual histories because they are neurotic, or 
whether their neurotic disturbances are the product of their homosexual 
activities and of society’s reaction to them. These are matters that must be 
investigated in more detail in a later volume; but they are questions that 
become more significant when one realizes the actual extent of homosexual 
behavior. 

Factors Accounting for the Homosexual. Attempts to identify the biologic 
bases of homosexual activity, must take into account the large number of 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


661 


males who have demonstrated their capacity to respond to stimuli pro- 
vided by other persons of the same sex. It must also be taken into account 
that many males combine in their single histories, and very often in exactly 
the same period of time, or even simultaneously in the same moment, 
reactions to both heterosexual and homosexual stimuli. They must take 
into account that in these combinations of heterosexual and homosexual 
experience, there is every conceivable gradation between exclusively hetero- 
sexual histories and exclusively homosexual histories. It must be shown 
that the fluctuations in preferences for female or male partners are related 
to fluctuations in the hormones, the genes, or the other biologic factors 
which are assumed to be operating (Kinsey 1941). It must be shown that 
there is a definite correlation between the degree in which the biologic 
factor operates, and the degree of the heterosexual-homosexual balance 
in the history of each individual. 

If psychologic or social forces are considered as agents in the origin of 
the homosexual, the same sorts of correlations must be shown before any 
causal relationship is established. An infrequent phenomenon might be 
accounted for by factors of one sort, but the factors which account for the 
homosexual must be of such an order as the incidence and frequency data 
show this phenomenon to be in our culture. Moreover, it should be em- 
phasized that it is one thing to account for an all-or-none proposition, as 
heterosexuality and homosexuality have ordinarily been taken to be. But 
it is a totally different matter to recognize factors which will account for 
the continuum which we find existing between the exclusively heterosexual 
and the exclusively homosexual history. 

Whatever factors are considered, it must not be forgotten that the basic 
phenomenon to be explained is an individual’s preference for a partner 
of one sex, or for a partner of the other sex, or his acceptance of a partner 
of either sex. This problem, is after all, part of the broader problem of 
choices in general: the choice of the road that one takes, of the clothes 
that one wears, of the food that one eats, of the place in which one sleeps, 
and of the endless other things that one is constantly choosing. A choice 
of a partner in a sexual relation becomes more significant only because 
society demands that there be a particular choice in this matter, and does 
not so often dictate one’s choice of food or of clothing. 

Hereditary Bases of Homosexuality. Through a brilliant series of studies, 
Goldschmidt showed the hereditary bases of intersexes among insects. It 
is unfortunate, however, that he identified homosexual males and females 
in the human species as intersexes, and thereby reached the conclusion 
that there must be a hereditary basis for homosexuality (Goldschmidt 
1916). The argument in his original paper was based on nothing more 
than an analogy between the intermediate secondary sexual characters 
which he found in the insects, and what he assumed to be intermediate 



662 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


characters in the psychology of the homosexual human individual. From 
this analogy he reasoned that there must be an inheritance of the human 
behavior phenomenon, just as there is inheritance of the morphologic 
structures which constitute the intersexuality of moths. With this idea of 
the inheritance of heterosexuality or homosexuality, a number of other 
workers have agreed. We are not ready at this time to discuss these data 
in detail, but we may point out that the incidence data and the record of 
gradations between exclusively heterosexual and exclusively homosexual 
histories which have been presented in the present chapter, have consider- 
able significance in this question of heredity. 

In order to prove that homosexual patterns of behavior are inherited 
in the human animal, the following conditions would need to be ful- 
filled: 

1. It would be necessary to define strictly what is meant in the study by 
the term homosexual. The term should be limited to persons of particular 
position on the heterosexual-homosexual scale; but whatever the restric- 
tions of the original study the conclusions should finally be applicable to 
all persons who have ever had any homosexual experience. 

2. There should be a determination of the incidence of the phenomenon 
in groups of siblings in which the complete sexual history of every indi- 
vidual in each family is known. It would be very desirable to secure com- 
plete histories of all the sibhngs in each family for at least two successive 
generations. As far as we are aware, such an accumulation of complete 
histories has never been available in any study of the inheritance of the 
homosexual. 

3. Especial attention should be paid to the balance between the homo- 
sexual and the heterosexual behavior in the histories of each of the siblings 
in such a study. 

4. The recognition of homosexuality in any individual should not be 
considered sufficient unless a complete sexual history is available. In 
considering the histories of relatives and ancestors, the pubhshed studies 
have put too much reliance upon suspicion, gossip, or the accidental 
pubhc disclosure of homosexual activity. In no instance has there been 
any sufficient regard for the fact that these relatives, who may, indeed, have 
had homosexual experience, may also have had heterosexual experience 
and rated anything between 1 and 6 on the heterosexual-homosexual 
scale. 

5. Similarly, the heterosexuality of any individual who enters into the 
calculations should be determined through complete sex histories. In 
nearly all studies to date, heterosexuality has been assumed where there 
was marriage or other known relations with the opposite sex, and when 
there was no public knowledge of homosexuality. These are, of course, 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 663 

untrustworthy sources of information on such a socially taboo item of 
behavior as is involved here. 

6. There should be data on enough cases of siblings to be statistically 
significant. In view of the experience in the present study (Chapter 3), it 
may be necessary to have histories from several hundred individuals in 
order to obtain satisfactory results. 

7. The incidence of the homosexual, as it is defined in the study, should 
be shown to be higher among siblings than it is in the histories of the non- 
siblings in the study. Inasmuch as our present data indicate that more 
than a third (37%) of the white males in any population (or probably, for 
that matter, among anyone’s ancestors) have had at least some homosexual 
experience, and inasmuch as the data indicate that a quarter of the males 
in the population (and a quarter of the males in anyone’s ancestry) may 
have more than incidental homosexual experience in the course of their 
lives, it would be necessary to show that the incidence of the homosexual 
in groups of siblings is higher than that. This, of course, has never been 
shown in any study on the inheritance of the homosexual. 

8. Whatever the hereditary mechanisms which are proposed, they must 
allow for the fact that some individuals change from exclusively hetero- 
sexual to exclusively homosexual patterns in the course of their lives, or 
vice versa, and they must allow for frequent changes in ratings of individ- 
uals on the heterosexual-homosexual scale. 

Social Applications. It is obvious that social interpretations of the homo- 
sexual behavior of any individual may be materially affected by a considera- 
tion of what is now known about the behavior of the population as a whole. 
Social reactions to the homosexual have obviously been based on the 
general belief that a deviant individual is unique and as such needs special 
consideration. When it is recognized that the particular boy who is dis- 
covered in homosexual relations in school, the business man who is having 
such activity, and the institutional inmate with a homosexual record, are 
involved in behavior that is not fundamentally different from that had by 
a fourth to a third of all of the rest of the population, the activity of the 
single individual acquires a somewhat different social significance. 

One of the factors that materially contributes to the development of 
exclusively homosexual histories, is the ostracism which society imposes 
upon one who is discovered to have had perhaps no more than a lone 
experience. The high school boy is likely to be expelled from school and, 
if it is in a small town, he is almost certain to be driven from the com- 
munity. His chances for making heterosexual contacts are tremendously 
reduced after the public disclosure, and he is forced into the company of 
other homosexual individuals among whom he finally develops an exclu- 
sively homosexual pattern for himself. Every school teacher and principal 



664 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


who is faced with the problem of the individual boy should realize that 
something between a quarter and a third of all the other boys in the same 
high school have had at least some homosexual experience since they turned 
adolescent. 

Community gossip and reactions to rumors of homosexual activity in 
the history of some member of the community would probably be modified 
if it were kept in mind that the same individual may have a considerable 
heterosexual element in his history as well. The social worker who is 
inchned to label a particular boy or older male in her case load as homo- 
sexual, because he is known to have had some such activity, should keep 
in mind that there is every gradation between complete homosexuality 
and complete heterosexuality. Administrators in institutions, officials in 
the Army and Navy, and many other persons in charge of groups of males 
may profitably consider the balance between the heterosexual and homo- 
sexual in an individual’s history, rather than the homosexual aspects 
alone. 


Administrators of penal and mental institutions are often much disturbed 
over the problem presented by a male who is committed for a homosexual 
offense. Such an individual is likely to receive especially severe treatment 
from the officials in the institution, and he may be segregated as a potential 
menace to the rest of the inmate body. If it is an institution in which trained 
psychologists or psychiatrists are employed, they are likely to give especial 
attention to the half dozen cases who are sent to the institution each year, 
on such charges. Our surveys in institutions, however, indicate that 25 or 
30 per cent of all the inmates have had homosexual experience before 
admission. It is obvious that the male who happens to be sent in on a 
homosexual charge may present no more special problem to the institution 
in this regard than the other quarter or third of the inmate body, who 
might just as well have been sent in on such a charge. As far as the admin- 
istration of a custodial institution is concerned, the problem of discipline 
does not depend upon the control of individuals who have some homo- 
sexual experience in their history, as much as it does upon the control of 
men who are particularly aggressive in forcing other individuals into homo- 
sexual relations. 


The judge who is considering the case of the male who has been arrested 
for homosexual activity, should keep in mind that nearly 40 per cent of all 
the other males in the town could be arrested at some time in their lives 
for similar activity, and that 20 to 30 per cent of the unmarried males in 
that town could have been arrested for homosexual activity that had taken 
place within that same year. The court might also keep in mind that the 
penal or mental institution to which he may send the male has something 
between 30 and 85 per cent of its inmates engaging in the sort of homo- 
sexual activity which may be involved in the individual case before him. 



HOMOSEXUAL OUTLET 


665 


On the other hand, the judge who dismisses the homosexual case that 
has come before him, or places the boy or adult on probation, may find 
himself the subject of attack from the local press which charges him with 
releasing dangerous “perverts’’ upon the community. Law enforcement 
officers can utilize the findings of scientific studies of human behavior 
only to the extent that the community will back them. Until the whole 
community understands the realities of human homosexual behavior, there 
is not likely to be much change in the -official handling of individual 
cases. 

The difficulty of the situation becomes still more apparent when it is 
realized that these generalizations concerning the incidence and frequency 
of homosexual activity apply in varying degrees to every social level, to 
persons in every occupation, and of every age in the community. The 
police force and court officials who attempt to enforce the sex laws, the 
clergymen and business men and every other group in the city which peri- 
odically calls for enforcement of the laws — particularly the laws against 
sexual “perversion” — have given a record of incidences and frequencies 
in the homosexual which are as high as those of the rest of the social level 
to which they belong. It is not a matter of individual hypocrisy which 
leads officials with homosexual histories to become prosecutors of the 
homosexual activity in the community. They themselves are the victims 
of the mores, and the public demand that they protect those mores. As 
long as there are such gaps between the traditional custom and the actual 
behavior of the population, such inconsistencies will continue to exist. 

There are those who will contend that the immorality of homosexual 
behavior calls for its suppression no matter what the facts are concerning 
the incidence and frequency of such activity in the population. Some have 
demanded that homosexuality be completely eliminated from society by a 
concentrated attack upon it at every point, and the “treatment” or isola- 
tion of all individuals with any homosexual tendencies. Whether such a 
program is morally desirable is a matter on which a scientist is not qualified 
to pass judgment; but whether such a program is physically feasible is a 
matter for scientific determination. 

The evidence that we now have on the incidence and frequency of homo- 
sexual activity indicates that at least a third of the male population would 
have to be isolated from the rest of the community, if all those with any 
homosexual capacities were to be so treated. It means that at least 13 per 
cent of the male population (rating 4 to 6 on the heterosexual-homosexual 
scale), would have to be institutionalized and isolated, if all persons who 
were predominantly homosexual were to be handled in that way. Since 
about 34 per cent of the total population of the United States are adult 
males, this means that there are about six and a third million males in the 
country who would need such isolation. 



666 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


If all persons with any trace of homosexual history, or those who were 
predominantly homosexual, were eliminated from the population today, 
there is no reason for believing that the incidence of the homosexual in 
the next generation would be materially reduced. The homosexual has 
been a significant part of human sexual activity ever since the dawn of 
history, primarily because it is an expression of capacities that are basic 
in the human animal. 



Chapter 22 


ANIMAL CONTACTS 

To many persons it will seem almost axiomatic that two mating animals 
should be individuals of the same species. This is so often true, from one 
end of the animal kingdom to the other, that exceptions to the rule seem 
especially worthy of note. To those who believe, as children do, that 
conformance should be universal, any departure from the rule becomes an 
immorality. The immorality seems particularly gross to ah individual who 
is unaware of the frequency with which exceptions to the supposed rule 
actually occur. 

No biologist exactly understands why males of a species are attracted 
primarily, even if not exclusively, to females of the same species. What is 
there to prevent insects of one species from mating with insects of many 
other species? What is there to prevent a frog from mating with frogs of 
other species? Why should mammals mate only with mammals of their 
own kind? In the animal kingdom as a whole, is it to be believed that 
the sources of sexual attraction are of such a nature that they provide 
stimuli only for other individuals of the same species? For the scientist it 
does not suffice to be told that nature allows nothing else but intraspecific 
mating because she considers reproduction to be the objective of all sexual 
activities, and because the production of offspring is supposed to be im- 
possible as a product of an interspecific cross. It does not suffice to think 
of inner forces which draw individuals together in their sexual relations. 
Such concepts concern intangibles with which science can have no dealing 
and, in the last analysis, the biologist and psychologist must look for 
material stimuli which, originating in one individual, may so affect other 
individuals that mating is the inevitable consequence. 

For instance, such specific stimuli are recognized in the odors emitted 
by female moths; and they have been demonstrated to be the sources of 
attraction for the males who come from considerable distances to congre- 
gate around the female that emits the odors. Such specific stimuli are 
recognized in the mating calls of toads and frogs, probably in the songs of 
birds, in the odors which some female mammals produce and, among 
the higher mammals, in the visual stimuli which the mere presence of an 
individual may provide. There is a considerable literature on this subject, 
but it needs to be analyzed with caution because so much of it is anthro- 
pomorphic, arriving at the sort of interpretation that a human intelligence 

667 



66S 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


would expect to find if intraspecific mating were the only possibility in 
nature. 

Even the scientists have been considerably biased in their investigations 
in this field, for they too have accepted the traditions. Even they have 
believed that matings between individuals of different species occur only 
rarely. Within the last few decades, however, students of taxonomy, 
genetics, and evolution have had the existence of interspecific hybrids 
increasingly drawn to their attention. These, of course, predicate the 
existence of interspecific matings. Some biologists are clearly uncomfortable 
in the face of these data, and are inclined to argue them away as they 
would argue away blots on their philosophy or theology. Even among the 
higher animals, interspecific crosses, or crosses between distinct varieties, 
have increasingly become known. The bird banding work has shown that 
birds respect the limits of their own species much less often than the old- 
time naturahsts would have insisted. And, finally, the students of sexual 
behavior among the higher mammals are beginning to report an increasing 
number of instances of animals mating, or trying to mate, with individuals 
of totally distinct and sometimes quite remote species (Nat. Res. Council 
Conf on Mammalian Sex Behav. 1943, Beach 1947). 

Fertile crosses between very distinct species are limited by microscopic 
mechanisms which the students of genetics and of cell structure have 
investigated in considerable detail. There is, however, no comparable 
knowledge of factors which might prevent matings between specifically 
distinct individuals. When one examines the observed cases of such crosses, 
and especially the rather considerable number of instances in which pri- 
mates, including man, have been involved, one begins to suspect that the 
rules about intraspecific matings are not so universal as tradition would 
have it. Indeed, one is struck anew with the necessity for better reasons 
than biologists and psychologists have yet found, for expecting that animal 
matings should invariably be limited to individuals of the same species. 

In light of the above, it is particularly interesting to note the degree of 
abhorrence with which intercourse between the human and animals of 
other species is viewed by most persons who have not had such experience. 
The biologist and the psychologist, and the anthropologist and the student 
of history, will have made a significant contribution when they can expound 
the development of our taboos on such contacts. 

It is known, of course, that these taboos were weU-established in the 
Old Testament and in the Talmud. Attention is also to be drawn to the 
fact that in the older Hittite code (Barton 1925), which may have had 
some influence on the Hebrew codes, the taboos on animal intercourse 
were not clearly the moral issues that they subsequently came to be. 
Specifically, in the Hittite code it is decreed that “if a man lie with a cow 
the punishment is death.” “If a man lies with a hog or dog, he shall die.” 



ANIMAL CONTACTS 


669 


“If a man . . . lies [with another, unidentified animal] the punishment is 
death.” “If a bull rear upon a man, the bull shall die, but the man shall 
not die.” “If a boar rear upon a man, there is no penalty.” “If a man lies 
with a horse or mule, there is no penalty, but he shall not come near the 
king, and he shall not become a priest.” These are proscriptions against 
contacts with certain animals, while contacts with certain other animals 
are more or less accepted. Such distinctions are strikingly paralleled by 
the taboos which made certain foods clean and other foods unclean. The 
student of human folkways is inclined to’see a considerable body of super- 
stition in the origins of all such taboos, even though they may ultimately 
become religious and moral issues for whole nations and whole races 
of people. 

In any event, it is certain that human contacts with animals of other 
species have been known since the dawn of history, they are known among 
all races of people today, and they are not uncommon in our own culture, 
as the data in the present chapter will show. Far from being a matter for 
surprise, the record simply substantiates our present understanding that 
the forces which bring individuals of the same species together in sexual 
relations, may sometimes serve to bring individuals of different species 
together in the same types of sexual relations. 

REFERENCES 

Data on the incidence and frequency of sexual contacts between the 
human male and animals of other species have already been detailed in 
this volume in tables and charts, and in discussions in the text, as follows : 


PAGE 

174 

TABLE 

FIGURE 

NATURE OF DATA 

Pre-adolescent experience with animals 

234 

49 


Range of variation in frequency of animal 
contacts 

260-262 

59 


Age affecting animal contacts 

362-363 

91 

172 

Educational level and animal contacts 

378, 

488-491 

96 

126, 128-130 

Importance of animal intercourse as one 
source of sexual outlet for single males 

459-463 

512 

124 

125 

Relation of rural-urban background to 
animal contacts 

Masturbation, self and animal 

669-673 

151 

171 

Accumulative incidence of animal con- 
tacts among single, rural males 

670-674 


173 

Individual variation in frequencies of 
animal contacts among rural males 


INCIDENCES AND FREQUENCIES 

The significance of animal contacts in the history of the human male 
lies largely in the field of social values ; for there is no other type of sexual 
activity which accounts for a smaller proportion of the total outlet of the 
total population (Figures 126, 128-130). For that population, including both 



670 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


single and married males, only a fraction of 1 per cent of the total number 
of orgasms is derived from animal intercourse. In the period when such 
contacts are most frequent, namely between adolescence and 20 years of 
age, a little less than 1 per cent of the total outlet is so derived; but the 
figure drops rapidly in successive age groups, and it amounts to only 0.04 
per cent among those males who remain single after the age of 25 (Table 59). 

In the total population, only one male in twelve or fourteen (estimated 
at about 8%) ever has sexual experience with animals (Table 59). In this 
total population, it is not more than 6 per cent which is involved in the 



A6E 

Figure 171. Animal contacts: accumulative incidence in single rural population 

Showing percent of single rural males who have ever had sexual contacts with animals 
to the point of orgasm, by each of the indicated ages. All data corrected for U. S. Census 
distribution. 

most active period (between adolescence and 20). The percentage drops in 
successive age groups to a little more than 1 per cent in the early twenties, 
and to a still lower figure at older ages. 

Frequencies of animal contacts are similarly low in the population 
taken as a whole. For most individuals, they do not occur more than once 
or twice, or a few times in a lifetime. 

On the other hand, the significance of such interspecific relationships 
becomes more apparent if we confine the calculations simply to that 
segment of the population which has access to animals, namely to the 
males who are raised on farms. For that group, the incidences and fre- 
quencies of animal contacts are more nearly comparable to the incidences 
and frequencies of contacts with prostitutes, or of homosexual contacts, 
in the population. There are a number of city-bred boys (4% between 
adolescence and age 15 alone) who have animal contacts in their histories 
(Table 124), and the fact that most of their experiences occur when they 



ANIMAL CONTACTS 


671 


visit on farms suggests that the entire human male population might have 
animal contacts as frequently as farm boys do if animals were available 
to all of them. 

Among boys raised on farms, about 17 per cent experience orgasm as 
the product of animal contacts which occur sometime after the onset of 
adolescence (Table 151, Figure 171). As many more have contacts which 
do not result in orgasm, and there are still others who have pre-adolescent 
experience which is not included in the above calculations. It is, in con- 
sequence, something between 40 and 50 per cent of all farm boys who have 
some sort of animal contact, either with or without orgasm, in their pre- 
adolescent, adolescent, and/or later histories. These must be minimum 
data, for there has undoubtedly been some cover-up in the reports of these 
activities. The data given in the remainder of this chapter are confined to 
those contacts which have resulted in orgasm for the human subject; but 
all of these figures may be doubled if one wishes to determine the total 
number of persons involved in any sort of relation, whether with or with- 
out orgasm. Such data begin to show what the significance of animal 
intercourse might be if conditions were more favorable for such activity. 

In fact, in certain Western areas of the United States, where animals 
are most readily available and social restraints on this matter are less 
stringent, we have secured incidence figures of as high as 65 per cent in 
some communities, and there are indications of still higher incidences in 
some other areas. The cases, however, are still too few to warrant a 
specific statement on these regional differences. 

Ultimately, 14 to 16 per cent of the rural males of the grade school 
level, 20 per cent of the rural males of the high school level, and 26 to 28 
per cent of the rural males of the college level have some animal expe- 
rience to the point of orgasm (Tables 91, 151). In this upper educational 
level, nearly one rural male in three has such contacts to the point of 
orgasm, and well over half of these upper level males have some kind of 
sexual contact with animals. 

Frequencies of animal contacts vary from once or twice in a lifetime to 
regular rates of several times a week over a considerable period of years. 
Maximum regular frequencies for a few individuals may go as high as 8 
per week between adolescence and 15, and 4 per week between 16 and 20, 
but not above once per week between ages 21 and 25 (Table 49). For most 
males, however, the frequencies come nearer averaging once in 2 or 3 
weeks, in that portion of the population which is having any contacts 
at all (Table 124). In most histories the contacts with animals are Umited 
to a matter of 2 or 3 years, but in some cases they’ may extend over a 10- 
or 15-year period or even throughout the whole of a life span. 

Animal contacts are most frequent during the late pre-adolescent years 
(Chapter 5) while ejaculation is stiU impossible for the human subject and 



672 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Animal Contacts: Accumulative Incidence Data 


AGE 

TOTAL POPULATION 

U. S. CORRECTIONS 

EDUC. LEVEL 

0-8 

EDUC. LEVEL 

9-12 

EDUC. LEVEL 

13+ 


Cases 

% with 

Cases 

%with 

Cases 

% with 

Cases 

%with 



Exper. 


Exper. 


Exper. 


Exper. 

10 

749 

0.04 

203 


110 


436 

0 7 

11 

749 

0.4 

203 

0.5 

110 


436 

1.1 

12 

749 

3.7 

203 

4.4 

110 

2.7 

436 

4.8 

13 

749 

5.7 

203 

5.9 

110 

4.5 

436 

10.8 

14 

749 

8.1 

203 

8.4 

110 

6.4 

436 

17.2 

15 

749 

10.4 

203 

9.9 

110 

9.1 

436 

22.7 

16 

745 

12.2 

201 

10.0 

109 

12.8 

435 

25.5 

17 

728 

12.8 

190 

10.0 

105 

14.3 

433 

26.1 

18 

700 

15.4 

179 

13.4 

93 

16.1 

428 

26.6 

19 

646 

15.9 

166 

13.9 

79 

16.5 

401 

26.4 

20 

567 

16.5 

145 

14.5 

63 

17.5 

359 ‘ 

26.2 

21 

500 

14.0 

128 

14.1 

51 

11.8 

321 

28.0 

22 

366 

15.0 

101 

12.9 



265 

29.4 

23 

290 

16.7 

76 

15.8 



214 

29.0 

24 

253 

17.2 

67 

14.9 



186 

28.0 

25 

217 


60 

13.3 



157 

27.4 

26 

176 


50 

12.0 



126 

25.4 

27 

96 






96 

24.0 

28 

72 






72 

23.6 

29 

59 






59 

23.7 


Table 151. Accumulative incidence data on animal contacts among rural males 

Data confined to single males who lived on farms for some period between 12 and 
18 years of age, or for longer periods. Data shown for three educational levels, and for 
the total farm population corrected for the U. S. Census of 1940. 



ANIMAL CONTACTS 


673 


when, in actuality, the boy almost never reaches orgasm in his animal 
contacts. One-third of the males who will ever have such contacts have had 
them by the age of 9. Between 10 and 12 there is a rapid increase in the 
number of boys involved and the peak of such activity is reached just 
before the onset of adolescence. In a third of the cases, boys with pre- 
adolescent experience continue their activities with animals into adolescent 
years. In terms of actual orgasms achieved by the human participant, the 
highest frequencies occur in the earliest adolescent years ; but in some rural 
areas, especially in the West, there is a considerable amount of regular 
activity in the later teens and even through the early twenties. Cases become 
relatively rare among single males in later years. There are, nevertheless, 
occasional individuals who have regular contacts from adolescence into 



ACE 


Figure 172. Animal contacts: accumulative incidence in three educational 
levels of single rural population 


Showing percent of each level of single rural males who have ever had sexual contacts 
with animals to the point of orgasm, by each of the indicated ages. 


their fifties, and there is one case of a male past 80 years of age who had 
had such contacts regularly throughout the whole of his life. 

In most parts of the country animal intercourse is extremely rare among 
married males; but, again, such experiences are not unknown among 
married adults in some rural areas of the West. 

Younger adolescent boys who are having animal contacts, derive, on an 
average, 7 per cent of their total outlet from that source. The males who 
are still having such contacts in their early twenties may derive as much as 
15 per cent of their outlet from that source (Table 59). 

As already indicated, a fair number of city boys have sexual relations 
with animals (Table 124). Some of this is had with household pets, par- 
ticularly with dogs, and some of it is had with ponies or with animals in 




674 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


the city stockyards. More of it, however, is had with animals with which 
the city boy comes into contact when he visits a farm during a vacation 
period. Since their opportunities for contacts are infrequent, city-bred 
males may not have more than a few experiences, and the frequencies of 
contacts among farm boys may average thirty to seventy times as high as 
the frequencies among city-bred groups. 



Figure 173. Aiiijtnal contacts: individual variation in frequencies, in two age 
periods, at three educational levels 


NATURE OF CONTACTS 

The animals that are involved in these human contacts include practi- 
cally all of the species that are domesticated on the farm or kept as pets in 
the household. Because of their convenient size, animals hke calves or, 
in the West, burros and sheep are most often involved. Practically every 
other mammal that has ever been kept on a farm enters into the record, 
and a few of the larger birds, hke chickens, ducks, and geese. Vaginal coitus 




ANIMAL CONTACTS 


675 


is the most frequent technique in the relations, but in at least parts of the 
country the fellation of the boy by the calf is not uncommon, and occasion- 
ally the household pels, particularly the dog and even the cat, may be 
induced to so perform. There is some anal intercourse. In some cases the 
boy masturbates by frictation against the body of the animal. There is an 
occasional record of the human male fellating the male animal. 

Masturbation of the animal by the human subject is almost as frequent 
as vaginal coitus. Masturbation may be either on the male or female 
animal, but it is most common with the male animal, particularly with 
the male dog. Very often whole groups of boys may be involved in such 
activities. If a boy is alone, he may masturbate himself while he masturbates 
the animal, and there may be considerable erotic stimulation to the boy 
involved in such a performance. 

SOCIAL SIGNIFICANCE 

A considerable portion of the animal activity of the farm boy is the 
product of his erotic arousal upon contemplating the coitus that occurs 
among the animals themselves, and of his constant association with 
animals that he knows have been recently involved in sexual activity. Such 
sympathetic emotional responses are natural enough and not fundament- 
ally different from those which would be expected if the boy were to observe 
coitus among human subjects. His attempt to replace the male animal in 
such relations is the obvious outcome of an identification of his own capac- 
ities with those of the animal he has observed. His initial attempts are 
sometimes inspired by a quite understandable curiosity to try what he has 
discovered to be a possible sort of activity. Whatever moral issues may be 
involved, and however long-standing the social condemnation of animal 
contacts may have been throughout the history of Western European 
civilizations, the easy dismissal of such behavior by characterizing it as 
abnormal shows little capacity for making objective analyses of the basic 
psychology that is involved. 

In a considerable number of instances the farm boy’s initiation into 
animal contacts is inspired by his knowledge of similar activity among 
his companions. This is particularly true in Western areas where adults 
as well as adolescents are not infrequently engaged in animal intercourse, 
and where there may be frequent conversation in the community about 
such activities. It is not unusual in some rural areas to find individuals 
who openly adnudt that such contacts have provided them with some erotic 
satisfaction. 

To a considerable extent contacts with animals are substitutes for 
heterosexual relations with human females. In rural areas where both 
social and sexual relations with girls may be more or less limited, the boy 
is often left alone or with his brothers, his male cousins, or the adult 



676 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


males who are working on the farm. We share the general impression, 
although we have no significant data to establish it, that rural communities 
are on the whole more traditional in their moral condemnation of pre- 
marital sexual relations, and the boy on the farm is often strictly forbidden 
to associate with girls. This cannot help but encourage substitutional 
behavior of the sort which the animals may afford. There are histories of 
extremely religious males who, even in their twenties and in later years, 
continue to derive practically the whole of their outlet from animals 
because of their conviction that heterosexual coitus with a human female 
is morally unacceptable. 

In not a few cases the animal contacts become homosexual activities. 
Masturbating the male animal, whether it is a dog, horse, bull, or some 
other species, may provide considerable erotic excitement for the boy or 
older adult. He senses the genital similarities between the male animal 
and himself, and he recognizes the relationship between the animal’s 
performance and reactions and his own capacities. His enjoyment of the 
relationship is enhanced by the fact that the male animal responds to the 
point of orgasm, and in at least some cases he is disappointed that the 
female animal (with rare exceptions) shows no erotic arousal and fails to 
experience orgasm. For these reasons, many a farm boy has as much con- 
tact with male animals as he does with female animals. There is consider- 
able basis for calling such activity homosexual, but since it is not recog- 
nized as such by most of the boys who are involved, they are in no conflict 
over that fact. 

Psychically, animal relations may become of considerable significance 
to the boy who is having regular experience. While his initial contacts may 
involve little more than the satisfaction which is to be obtained from 
physical stimulation, the situation becomes quite different for the boy who 
is having frequent contacts with particular animals. The depth of the 
boy’s psychic response is evidenced by his quick erection and by the ease 
with which he may reach orgasm in his relations with the animal. The 
psychic significance of his experience is particularly evidenced by the fact 
that animal contacts may become a regular part of his nocturnal dreams. 
Moreover, many a farm boy, while masturbating, develops erotic fantasies 
of himself in contact with some animal. In some cases the boy may develop 
an affectional relation with the particular animal with which he has his 
contacts, and there are males who are quite upset emotionally, when 
situations force them to sever connections with the particular animal. If 
this seems a strange perversion of human affection, it should be recalled 
that exactly the same sort of affectional relationship is developed in many 
a household where there are pets; and it is not uncommon for persons, 
everywhere in our society, to become considerably upset at the loss of a 
pet dog or cat which has been in the home for some period of time. The 



ANIMAL CONTACTS 


677 


elements that are involved in sexual contacts between the human and 
animals of other species are at no point basically different from those that 
are involved in erotic responses to human situations. 

On the other side of the record, it is to be noted that male dogs who 
have been masturbated may become considerably attached to the persons 
who provide the stimulation; and there are records of male dogs who 
completely forsake the females of their own species in preference for the 
sexual contacts that may be had with a human partner. 

With most males, animal contacts represent a passing chapter in the 
sexual history. They are replaced by coitus with human females as soon 
as that is available. On the other hand, the male who has had any con- 
siderable amount of animal experience may become so conditioned that 
he still finds himself erotically aroused by contemplating such possibihties, 
even years after he has stopped having actual contacts. 

Anglo-American legal codes rate sexual relations between the human 
and animals of other species as sodomy, punishable under the same laws 
which penalize homosexual and mouth-genital contacts. The city-bred 
judge who hears such a case is likely to be unusually severe in his condem- 
nation, and is likely to give the maximum sentence that is possible. Males 
who are sent to penal institutions on such charges are likely to receive 
unusually severe treatment both from the administrations and from the 
inmates of the institutions. All in all, there is probably no type of human 
sexual behavior which has been more severely condemned by that segment 
of the population which happens not to have had such experience, and 
which accepts the age-old judgment that animal intercourse must evidence 
a mental abnormality, as well as an immorality. 

On the other hand, in rural communities where animal contacts are not 
infrequent, and where there is some general knowledge that they do 
commonly occur, there seem to be few personal conflicts growing out of 
such activity, and very few social difficulties. It is only when the farm-bred 
male migrates to a city community and comes in contact with city-bred 
reactions to these activities, that he becomes upset over the contemplation 
of what he has done. This is particularly true if he learns through some 
psychology course or through books that such behavior is considered 
abnormal. There are histories of farm-bred males who have risen to 
positions of importance in the business, academic, or political world in 
some large urban center, and who have lived for years in constant fear 
that their early histories will be discovered. The chnician who can reassure 
these individuals that such activities are biologically and psychologically 
part of the normal mammahan picture, and that such contacts occur in as 
high a percentage of the farm population as we have already indicated, 
may contribute materially toward the resolution of these conflicts. 



678 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Viewed objectively, human sexual behavior, in spite of its diversity, is 
more easily comprehended than most people, even scientists, have pre- 
viously realized. The six types of sexual activity, masturbation, spontaneous 
nocturnal emissions, petting, heterosexual intercourse, homosexual con- 
tacts, and animal contacts, may seem to fall into categories that are as 
far apart as right and wrong, licit and illicit, normal and abnormal, 
acceptable and unacceptable in our social organization. In actuality, they 
all prove to originate in the relatively simple mechanisms which provide 
for erotic response when there are sufficient physical or psychic stimuli. 

To each individual, the significance of any particular type of sexual 
activity depends very largely upon his previous experience. Ultimately, 
certain activities may seem to him to be the only things that have value, 
that are right, that are socially acceptable; and all departures from his 
own particular pattern may seem to him to be enormous abnormalities. 
But the scientific data which are accumulating make it appear that;, if 
circumstances had been propitious, most individuals might have become 
conditioned in any direction, even into activities which they now consider 
quite unacceptable. There is little evidence of the existence of such a thing 
as innate perversity, even among those individuals whose sexual activities 
society has been least inclined to accept. There is an abundance of evidence 
that most human sexual activities would become comprehensible to most 
individuals, if they could know the background of each other individuaTs 
behavior. 

The social values of human activities must be measured by many scales 
other than those which are available to the scientist. Individual responsi- 
bilities toward others in the social organization, and the long-range out- 
come of behavior which represents the individual’s response to the stimuli 
of the immediate moment, are things that persons other than scientists 
must evaluate. As scientists, we have explored, and we have performed 
our function when we have published the record of what we have found 
the human male doing sexually, as far as we have been able to ascertain 
that fact. 



CLINICAL TABLES 


TABLE 152 . SINGLE WHITE MALES 
TABLE 153 . MARRIED WHITE MALES 
TABLE 154 . PREVIOUSLY MARRIED WHITE MALES 




Chapter 23 


CLINICAL TABLES 

This chapter is primarily designed for use by the clinician and some 
others concerned with directing human behavior. It will be of particu- 
lar use to psychiatrists, physicians, chnical psychologists, personnel offi- 
cers, counselors in schools, marriage counselors, court judges, probation 
and parole officers, institutional directors, social workers, clergymen, 
teachers, and parents. By means of the tables included in this chapter, it 
should be possible to compare the sexual history of the individual who 
comes for help, with averages for other persons of the same age group, 
educational level, and religious or rural-urban background. Many persons 
should be interested in comparing their histories with the group patterns. 

It is often important to know how far an individual’s sexual behavior de- 
parts from the pattern of the group in which he has been raised or in which 
he may now live. Personality conflicts more often depend upon the indi- 
vidual’s departure from the pattern of the social group to which he belongs, 
less often upon his failure to conform to the publicly pretended social code 
or to the formulated laws. Many clinicians feel that any re-direction of be- 
havior should be limited to fitting the individual into the pattern of the 
particular group to which he belongs, rather than trying to place him in a 
pattern which the upper social level considers socially or morally desir- 
able. An increasing number of clinicians have come to realize that 
attempts to re-direct behavior into patterns which are foreign to the 
background of the individual may introduce even more conflicts. 

Many persons who are disturbed over items in their sexual histories may 
be put at ease when they learn what the patterns of the rest of the popula- 
tion are, and when they realize that their own behavior has not departed 
fundamentally from the behavior of most persons in their social group. 

It may well be questioned how far an individual is responsible for his be- 
havior when he conforms to the pattern of his social level, even though he 
may thereby be involved in a transgression of the law. Court officers and 
other law enforcement officials, administrators of penal, mental, and other 
institutions, and social workers might well distinguish between departures 
from the law and departures from the pattern which is common in a whole 
social group. The chnician who is interested in modifying the pattern of a 
particular individual should realize that this must ultimately involve a 
transformation of the pattern of the whole group from which the individual 
comes. 


681 



682 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


DEFINITIONS 

Terms in these tables have been used with the same meanings that have 
been applied to them throughout the present volume. Specifically, the fol- 
lowing definitions have been used: 

1. White males. These tables are based entirely upon white males. The 
data for Negro males are not included. 

2. Marital status. There are three sets of tables (Tables 152, 153, 154) 
covering single males, married males, and previously married males. Per- 
sons who have lived openly as man and wife for a full year in a common law 
relationship are treated as married persons. 

3. Age. Summaries are given for each age group, beginning with a period 
which extends from adolescence through 15, and continuing with groups 
that extend beyond that by 5-year periods. 

4. Educational level. This represents the amount of schooling which the 
subject ultimately attains before leaving school. There are three classifi- 
cations : 0-8 are the males who never have more than grade school edu- 
cation; 9-12 are the males who go into high school but not beyond; 13+ 
are the males who at least start to college. For those individuals who are 
still in grade school or high school, the ratings must not be taken to repre- 
sent the grades in which they are currently located. For a male who is still 
in grade school or in high school, the clinician may sometimes predict, on 
the basis of his home background, the amount of his future schooling. All 
males who are still in college may be considered of college level and located 
in the tables along with the males who have completed college. 

5. Urban. Persons who have lived in cities or towns for the major portion 
of the period between 12 and 18 years of age. 

6. Rural. Persons who have lived on operating farms for an appreciable 
portion of the time between ages 12 and 18. 

7. Protest., active. Persons regularly attendant or actively involved in 
organizational work in a Protestant church, during the age period pre- 
sented on the tables. 

8. Protest., inactive. Persons with Protestant church background, but 
not regular attendants or active participants in church activities, during the 
period shown. 

9. Cath., devout. Persons who are members and frequent attendants in 
the Catholic church, and/or who regularly attend confession. 

10. Cath., inactive. Persons with Catholic backgrounds but not regular 
attendants and not regularly going to confession during the age period 
shown in the tables. 

11. Jevrish, Orthod. Persons who are regular attendants at the synagogue 
and/or more or less strict in their observance of the Orthodox customs. 

12. JevFish, inactive. Persons with Jewish backgrounds but not regular 
attendants at the synagogue or strict in their observance of the Orthodox 
customs. 



CLINICAL TABLES 


683 


13. Source of outlet. 

Total. The total number of orgasms (given as averages per week) 
which result from any and all types of sexual activity, added together 
Mast. Masturbation. Orgasms experienced from deliberate self- 
stimulation by any technique, whether manual, frictional, or other. 
Emiss. Nocturnal emissions. Orgasms experienced during sleep, 
whether with or without accompanying dreams. 

Pet. Petting to climax. Orgasms experienced as a result of heterosexual 
contacts which do not include the actual union of genitalia. 

Interc. Intercourse. Orgasms resulting from heterosexual coitus. For 
single males this represents the total of all pre-marital intercourse, ir- 
respective of whether it is had with companions or with prostitutes, or 
with both. For married males this represents only that portion of the 
heterosexual intercourse which is had with the wife. In post-marital 
histories this is again the total of all heterosexual intercourse, whether 
with companions or with prostitutes or with both. 

Extra. Extra-marital intercourse. The total number of orgasms se- 
cured by the married male from coitus with females other than his 
wife, whether the partners are companions or prostitutes. 

Prost. Intercourse with prostitutes. The record of orgasms derived 
from heterosexual intercourse with prostitutes, in the single, extra- 
marital, or post-marital histories. 

Homo. Homosexual outlet. Orgasms resulting from physical contacts 
with other males, irrespective of the techniques employed, and irre- 
spective of whether the relation is had with companions or with male 
prostitutes. 

Anim. Animal contacts. Orgasms resulting from sexual contacts with 
animals other than human, and irrespective of techniques employed. 

14. Cases. Showing the size of the population on which the data in the 
tables are based. It is to be recalled (Chapter 3) that a sample of 50 has 
proven adequate for establishing incidence data, that samples of 100 or 200 
are fairly adequate for means and medians, and that samples of 300 or 
more are quite adequate for determining means and medians. The larger 
the sample, the more adequate the data on the range of variation. The 
clinician may make some allowance for error in data which are shown here 
when they are based on samples which are smaller than those prescribed 
above; but such smaller samples may still be taken as indicative of results 
that may be obtained from larger samples. 

15. Incid. %. Showing the percentage of that particular group (the 
incidence) which has any experience with that particular outlet. This pro- 
vides a figure for calculating the size of the active population. 

16. Freq. per week. Data showing the average frequencies per week from 
each particular source of outlet, for each segment of the population. These 
data are all based on “active populationsV’ as the term has been used 



684 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


throughout the present volume. They are averages for those persons who 
derive any of their outlet from that particular source, and they are not 
averaged with data from those who do not draw an outlet from that source. 

17. Range. The ranges given under Freq. per week represent the maxi- 
mum frequencies which have been recorded for the individuals available 
in the present study, except that the one most extreme individual in each 
population has not been included. An individual whose rates of outlet av- 
erage higher than the rates shown in this column, represents a more ex- 
treme case than the present investigators have found in a population of the 
size used here. How far these most extreme individuals depart from the 
average for the particular population may be seen by comparing the size 
of the range with the size of the means and the medians. 

18. Mean. The means shown under Freq. per week represent the average 
number of orgasms from each particular source in the particular popula- 
tion. They represent one sort of average which should be taken into account 
in judging the particular individual, but they should be compared with the 
averages shown as medians in the next column. 

19. Median. The medians shown under Freq. per week represent the av- 
erage number of orgasms derived by the average individual (the individual 
who stands mid-way) in the particular population. In many instances this 
is the most important sort of average for comparisons. The figures for the 
median become meaningless, however, when they become too small, in 
which case the data for the means will have to be relied upon. 

20. % of total outlet. These data show the proportion of the total sexual 
outlet which is derived from each source, in each active population. These 
do not total 100% for the whole population, because each calculation is 
based only on those individuals whose histories include the particular out- 
let (see Chapter 3). 

21. Range. The range under % of total outlet shows the maximum per- 
centage which any individual in the available sample has derived from a 
particular source, except that the one most extreme individual is not in- 
cluded in the calculations. 

22. Mean. The means under % of total outlet show the proportion of the 
total number of orgasms which, on an average, is derived from the par- 
ticular source. This is one kind of average, but it should be compared with 
the averages shown as medians in the next column. 

23. Median. The medians under % of total outlet show the percentages of 
the total number of orgasms which are derived from each particular source 
by the average individual in the active population (the individual who 
stands mid-way in the whole population). 

HOW TO USE THE TABLES 

1. After the sexual history of an individual is secured by the clinician, 
the extent to which the history agrees or disagrees with averages for the 
particular social group to which he belongs, and the question of whether 



CLINICAL TABLES 


685 


he falls within or outside of the range of variation now known within that 
group, may be determined by the use of these clinical tables. Data which 
the clinician needs for such comparisons should include average frequen- 
cies of orgasms experienced by the individual (averaged per week) from 
each of the possible sources (masturbation, nocturnal emissions, hetero- 
sexual petting, total heterosexual intercourse, heterosexual intercourse 
with prostitutes, marital intercourse, homosexual outlets, and animal con- 
tacts), during each of the 5-year periods of the history. The total sexual 
outlet for the individual in each of the 5-year periods may then be deter- 
mined by an addition of the above outlets. It is then possible to compute 
the percentages of the total outlet which the individual has derived from 
each of these sources, during each of these 5-year periods. 

2. The group to which a particular individual belongs may be deter- 
mined as follows : 

A. Locate the individual in Table 152, 153, or 154, depending on 
whether he is single, married, or previously married. 

B. LFnder one of these Tables, pick the age group to which the indi- 
vidual now belongs. 

C. Under each of the ages, locate the section which covers groups 
whose maximum amount of schooling corresponds with that of 
the subject. 

D. Under the particular educational levels, locate the individual as 
urban or rural, and belonging to one or another of the religious 
groups shown. If the proper rural-urban or religious group is not 
shown, the history of the particular individual will have to be com- 
pared with the data for that educational level taken as a whole. 

E. For further information, examine the data given under each of the 
age groups which the individual has previously passed through. 

3. It is then possible to consider the extent to which the individuafs pat- 
tern agrees with or departs from the pattern shown for his group in the 
tables. Remember that in any group there is always considerable individual 
variation, and that no individual may be expected to conform to averages 
for the group (see Figures 136 to 160). The extent to which the chnician 
allows a departure from any average will depend upon his interpretation 
of the significance of a strict conformance to the group pattern, and his ac- 
ceptance of some latitude of individual variation. In any event, the judg- 
ment of the individual’s behavior will have been based upon an under- 
standing of the patterns of sexual behavior which lie in the background 
from which the individual comes. 

4. A generalized statement concerning the nature of the sexual pattern 
in each of these social groups is given on the right-hand page and directly 
opposite the corresponding data in the tables. If statistical calculations on 
the individual’s sexual history are not available, or do not seem sufficiently 
important to secure, his history can at least be compared with the general- 
ized statements given on the right-hand pages throughout this chapter. 



Table 152. Clinical Tables 
SINGLE WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 



1 FREQ. PER WEEK 

1 % OF TOTAL OUTLET 




INCID. 







AGE: ADOL.-15 

OF 

CASES 

% 








OUTLET 



Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 











1. Whole group 

Total 

712 

91 

25.0 

3.3 

2.1 







Mast 

712 

85 

14.0 

1.8 

1.1 

100 

66.9 

75.8 


Emiss. 

712 

25 

2.5 

0,2 

0.1 

100 

17.1 

2.7 


Pet. 

712 

13 

3.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

2.5 

0.6 


Interc. 

630 

48 

22.0 

2.2 

1.2 

100 

50.8 

48.5 


Prost. 

712 

9 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

85 

12.3 

2.5 


Homo. 

712 

24 

7.0 

1.0 

0.4 

100 

19.4 

9.3 


Amm. 

712 

6 

2.0 

0.5 

0.1 

25 

5.2 

0.9 

2. Urban 

Total 

401 

92 

25.0 

3.7 

2.3 






Mast. 

401 

87 

14.0 

2.0 

1.2 

100 

65.6 

74.2 


Emiss. 

401 

27 

2.5 

0.2 

0.1 

100 

14.1 

1.9 


Pet. 

401 

14 

2.5 

0.3 

0.1 

15 

2.4 

0.6 


Interc. 

414 

51 

22.0 

2.5 

1.4 

100 

50.2 

48.8 


Prost. 

401 

10 

2.0 

0.4 

0.2 

70 

11.4 

3.3 


Homo. 

401 

28 

7.0 

1.1 

0.4 

75 

17.6 

8.1 


Amm. 

401 

4 

1.5 

0.3 

0.1 

25 

4.1 

0.8 

3. Rural 

Total 

245 

89 

17.0 

2.7 

1.7 





Mast 

245 

81 

14.0 

1.6 

1.1 

100 

70.2 

84.3 


Emiss. 

245 

23 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

100 

24.4 

4.8 


Pet. 

245 

12 

1.0 

0.1 

0.1 

30 

3.1 

0.7 


Interc. 

199 

39 

14.0 

1.7 

0.9 

100 

52.0 

48.1 


Prost. 

245 

5 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

85 

20.2 

3.0 


Homo. 

245 

18 

3.5 

1.0 

0.5 

100 

24.9 

12,5 


Anim. 

245 

9 

2.0 

0.6 

0.5 

20 

7.2 

5.8 

4. Protest., active 

Total 

89 

83 

14.0 

3.2 

1.9 





Mast. 

89 

71 

10.0 

1.8 

1.1 

100 

69.4 

87.5 


Emiss. 

89 

21 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

100 

29.5 

4.0 


Pet. 

89 

11 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

4.4 

0.7 


Interc. 

83 

34 

7.0 

2.0 

0.8 

100 

51.8 

47.5 


Prost. 

89 

4 

0.5 

1.1 

0.4 

30 

28.6 

17.5 


Homo. 

89 

1 22 

3.5 

1.7 

1.7 

100 

1 37.1 

27.5 


Anim. 

78 

4 

1.0 

1.1 

1.0 

15 

13.0 

15.0 

5. Protest, inactive 

Total 

481 

94 

25.0 

3.3 

2.0 





Mast 

481 

88 

14.0 

1.8 

1.1 

100 

66.3 

73.1 


Emiss. 

481 

26 

2.5 

0.2 

0.1 

100 

13.6 

2.2 


Pet. 

422 

14 

3.0 

0.2 

0.1 

15 

2.5 

0.6 


Interc. 

431 

52 

22.0 

2.2 

1.3 

100 

51.3 

49.2 


Prost. 

422 

10 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

85 

12.2 

2.0 


Homo. 

481 

23 

6.0 

1.0 

0.4 

75 

17.3 

5.4 


Amm. 

422 

5 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

20 

4.0 

0.9 

6. Cath., inactive 

Total 

106 

91 

14.0 

3.6 

2.5 





Mast. 

106 

86 

9.0 

1.9 

1.2 

100 

64.1 

75.0 


Emiss. 

106 

24 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

100 

17.3 

3.0 


Pet. 

106 

16 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

1.7 

0.6 


Interc. 

89 

45 

9.0 

2.7 

2.1 

100 

52.6 

56.3 


Prost. 

106 

7 

0.5 

0.3 

0.3 

15 

7.1 

6.3 


Homo. 

106 

30 

3.5 

1.0 

0.4 

55 

17.3 

6.3 

Educ. level 9-12 

Amm. 

77 

8 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

4.3 

0.7 

7. Whole group 

Total 

606 

95 

25.0 

3.5 

2.5 





Mast 

606 

90 

15.0 

2.2 

1.4 

100 

70.5 

78.5 


Emiss, 

606 

40 

5.0 

0.4 

0.1 

100 

16.7 

3.7 


Pet. 

606 

20 

2.5 

0.2 

0.1 

40 

4.5 

0.7 


Interc. 

511 

43 

25.0 

1.9 

0.8 

100 

39.9 

33.9 


Prost. 

606 

8 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

60 

7.0 

0.9 


Homo. 

606 

32 

7.0 

0.9 

0.3 

100 

18.1 

4.9 



Anim. 

606 

6 

2.5 

0.5 


45 

7.8 

2.8 


m 



CLINICAL TABLES : SINGLE 


687 


Table 152 . Clinical Tables 
SINGLE WHITE MALES 

1. Adol.-15, Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Most boys (91%) immediately ac- 
tive upon onset of adolescence, mean weekly rates 3.3 but often 7 or more. 
Regular rnasturbation for 85%, but of total outlet from intercourse for 3^^ of 
group. This level accepts intercourse as the great desideratum, often begins it in 
pre-adolescence. Nocturnal emissions in only 34 of boys. Homosexual in 34, 
accounting for 1/10 of their outlet. Other outlets less frequent. 

2. Adol.-15. Educ. level 0-8. Urban. Essentially as described above (Group 1). 
City-bred boys are somewhat more often involved than rural boys in every type 
of sexual activity except animal contacts. Masturbation % of outlet for 87% of 
boys; intercourse important, providing 3^^ of outlet for of boys. Homosexual 
in 28%, accounting for 8% of their outlet. All other sources more minor. 

3. Adol.-15. Educ. level 0-8. Rural. Essentially as described under Group 1. 
In most .sexual activities, farm boys are less often involved than city boys (Group 
2). Masturbation in 81%, intercourse in 39%, homosexual in 18%. Masturbation 
accounts for 84% of total outlet of those who masturbate at all, intercourse for 
48%, homosexual for 13%. Animal contacts to orgasm in 9%, attempts in as 
many more. 

4. AdoL-15. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., active. Somewhat more restrained boys 
of lower level (Group 1). Fewer individuals involved in any type of activity; only 
a few of these upset by conflict between religious backgrounds and lower level’s 
acceptance of sex. Boys who do engage in any activity, may have as high fre- 
quencies as the rest of the community. Masturbation, intercourse, and the homo- 
sexual are the most common, in that order. 

5. Adol.-15. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. A sexually active group of 
lower level boys (Group 1). Acceptant of most types of sexual activity. Masturba- 
tion in 88%, intercourse in 52%, prostitutes in 10%, the homosexual in 23%. Fre- 
quencies highest in masturbation and intercourse. Quite aware that their com- 
panions and older boys are similarly involved, and not impressed by persons who 
interfere with such nearly universal activity. 

6. Adol.-15. Educ. level 0-8. Cath., inactive. Of Cathohc background, but 
sexual activities little affected by religious teaching. Closely duplicating the group 
pattern (Group 1), and pattern of inactive Protestant boys (Group 5). Accept 
masturbation (86% of boys), intercourse (45%), and homosexual relations 
(30%). Few conflicts. Predominantly heterosexual in psychic response, overt ac- 
tivity, and public pretense; but a full third have some homosexual relations. 

7. Adol.-15. Educ. level 9-12. Whole group. Most active of all educational 
levels, nearly every boy sexually active immediately at adolescence, with high 
frequencies — 3.5 per week, often 10 or more. Nearly all (90%) masturbate; 
nearly half (43%) have intercourse before 15, many before adolescence; a third 
(32%) ejaculate in homosexual before 15. Nocturnal emissions (40%) higher than 
among boys of grade school level; petting more often than in grade school or 
even college levels. 


23 



Table 152. Clinical Tables ( continued ) 
SINGLE WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 

OF 

OUTLET 


INCID. 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 

AGE: ADOL.-15 

Educ. level 9-12 

8. Urban 

O.A.SHS 

% ■ 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Total 

459 

96 

23.0 

3.7 

2.6 

— 

— 

— 


Mast. 

459 

90 

12.0 

2.3 

1.6 

100 

69.1 

77.2 


Emiss. 

459 

42 

5.0 

0 4 

0.1 

100 

16.7 

3.9 


Pet. 

459 

20 

2.5 

0.3 

0.1 

40 

5.0 

0 7 


Interc. 

405 

44 

25.0 

2.0 

0.8 

100 

40.0 

35.0 


Frost 

459 

8 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

5.1 

0.9 


Homo. 

459 

38 

5.0 

0.8 

0.3 

100 

18.8 

7.9 


Anim. 

459 

4 

2.0 

0.5 

0.1 

20 

6.3 

3.0 

9. Rural 

Total 

124 

94 

18.0 

3.0 

1.9 

— 

— 

— 


Mast 

124 

89 

9.0 

1.9 

1.2 

100 

77.9 

92.5 


Emiss. 

124 

35 1 

2.0 

0.3 

0.2 

100 

17.6 

3.0 


Pet. 

124 

18 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

10 

2.6 

0.6 


Interc, 

91 

38 

4.5 

1.5 

0.9 

100 1 

37.4 

32.0 


Prost. 

124 

5 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

20.0 

0.8 


Homo. 

124 

20 

7.0 

1.3 

0.1 

45 

12.6 

2.3 


Anim. 

124 

10 

1.0 

0.4 

0.1 

45 

11.5 

3.0 

10. Protest., active 

Total 

93 

92 

12.0 

2.8 

1.9 

— 

— 

— 


Mast 

93 

87 

9.0 

2.0 

1.6 

100 

72.0 

81.3 


Emiss. 

93 

41 

2.5 

0.4 

0.2 

100 

25.8 

11.3 


Pet. 

93 

16 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

10 

3.3 

0.7 


Interc. 

86 

31 

4.5 

1.4 

0.8 

80 

40.5 

40.0 


Prost. 

93 

5 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

1.0 

0.8 


Homo. 

93 

15 

2.5 

0.7 

0.1 

75 

21.9 

3.0 


Arum. 

83 

5 

0.5 

0.2 

0.2 

15 

16.8 

12.5 

11. Protest., inactive 

Total 

375 

95 

23.0 

3.6 

2.5 



— 

— 


Mast. 

375 

90 

12.0 

2.1 

1.4 

100 

69.6 

77.8 


Emiss. 

375 

40 

4.0 

0.4 

0.1 

100 

13.2 

2.7 


Pet. 

311 

21 

1.5 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

3.7 

0.7 


Interc. 

318 

48 

14.0 

2.0 

1.0 

100 

42.4 

36.1 


Prost. 

311 

8 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

15 

2.6 

0.8 


Homo. 

375 

34 

5.0 

0.8 

0.3 

95 

15.6 

4.4 


Anim. 

311 

8 

2.0 

0.5 

0.2 

55 

7.4 

2.7 

12. Cath., inactive 

Total 

103 

98 

23.0 

3.9 

2.7 





— 


Mast 

103 

95 

11.0 

2.3 

1.6 

100 

73.9 

81.0 


Emiss. 

103 

35 

1.5 

0.4 

0.2 

90 

13.1 

4.3 


Pet. 

103 

21 

2.5 

0.5 

0.1 

40 

7.1 

0.7 


Interc. 

72 

38 

3.0 

1.6 

0.5 

85 

26.1 

22.5 


Prost. 

103 

9 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

5 

6.5 

1.0 

Educ. level 13+ 

Homo. 

103 

47 

5.0 

1.1 

0.4 

75 

23.2 

12.5 

13. Whole group 

Total 

2799 

96 

29.0 

3.0 

2.3 

— 

— 

— 


Mast 

2799 

82 

29.0 

2.7 

2.1 

100 

82.2 

92.7 


Emiss. 

2799 

70 

12.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

30.0 

11.6 


Pet. 

2799 

14 

3.5 

0.3 

0.1 

80 

9.1 

0.8 


Interc. 

2421 

10 

9.5 

0.8 

0.3 

100 

23.6 

9.4 


Prost 

2799 

2 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

35 

4.2 

0.7 


Homo. 

2799 

21 

6.0 

0.4 

0.1 

90 

9.4 

1.1 


Anim. 

2799 

5 

8.0 

0.4 

0.1 

75 

6,6 

0.8 

14. Urban 

Total 

2587 

96 

29.0 

3.0 

2.3 








Mast. 

2587 

82 

29.0 

2.7 

2.1 

100 

81.8 

92.6 


Emiss. 

2587 

72 

12.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

28.9 

10.7 


Pet. 

2587 

14 

3.5 

0.3 

0.1 

70 

9.3 

0.9 


Interc. 

2126 

10 

9.5 

0.8 

0.3 

100 

21.9 

9.0 

1 

Prost. 

2587 

2 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

15 

2.6 

0.7 


Homo. 

2587 

22 

6.0 

0.4 

0.1 

85 

10.1 

1.4 


Anim. 

2587 

2 

2.0 

0.1 

0.1 

51 

0.7 

0.6 


688 



CLINICAL tables: SINGLE 


689 


SINGLE WHITE MALES 

8. AdoL-15. Educ. level 9-12. Urban. Often very energetic and socially active. 
Sexually more involved than farm boys (Group 9), especially those city boys who 
become adolescent by 12. Mean total frequencies 3.7 per week (often to 10 or 
more). Chief outlets masturbation (among 90%), intercourse (44%), nocturnal 
emissions (42%), and homosexual (38%). Early becoming proficient at socio- 
sexual contacts, relatively uninhibited except in a few of the religiously devout. 

9. Adol.-15. Educ. level 9-12. Rural. Less active than city-bred boys (Group 
8), more active than farm boys of grade school level (Group 3). Most (89%) 
masturbate, a third (38%) have coitus, another third (35%) nocturnal emissions. 
In contrast to city boys, only one in five (20%) has homosexual, but one in ten 
(10%) completes animal intercourse, as many more may attempt it. For those 
actively involved, 92% of outlet from masturbation, 32% from intercourse. 

10. Adol.-15. Educ. level 9-12. Protest., active. Slightly less active, but actually 
very close to other young adolescent boys of high school level (Group 7). Mas- 
turbation in 87% of histories, intercourse in one-third, these the chief sources of 
outlet. Nevertheless, petting and homosexual in one in seven histories. An occa- 
sional individual may be in conflict with religious backgrounds, but more af- 
fected by group pattern than by religion. 

11. Adol.-15. Educ. level 9-12, Protest., inactive. Data almost exactly as de- 
scribed for urban group of high school level (Group 8). Incidences highest in 
masturbation (90%), intercourse (48%), nocturnal emissions (40%), and the 
homosexual (34%). Chief outlets from masturbation (78% of total for mastur- 
bating males) and intercourse (36% of total for males who have any coitus). 
Group accepts sex with little conflict. 

12. AdoL-15. Educ. level 9-12. Cath., inactive. Most active of younger ado- 
lescent boys, 98% having some outlet by 15. This a reflection of community ac- 
ceptance of sex, and of minimum attention to religious teaching. Incidences 
highest in masturbation (95%), homosexual (47%), intercourse (38%). Chief 
outlets masturbation (81%), intercourse (22%). Intercourse considered highly 
desirable. Homosexual often condemned, but nearly one-half (47%) draw 12.5% 
of their outlet from it. 

13. Adol.-15. Educ. level 13+. Whole group. Boys who ultimately go to col- 
lege become active early, depend chiefly on masturbation and nocturnal emis- 
sions. Masturbation often accepted, occasionally causes worry, not as often as 
among older generations. Pre-adolescent play chiefly exhibitionistic, with little 
carry-over. Much inhibited socio-sexually, from adolescence and on. Much less 
affected than lower level boys by sexual activities of older companions, but soon 
acquire community judgment that intercourse is wrong. More often pet to 
climax. 21% have homosexual contacts (infrequently); this not often causing 
conflicts at this age. 

14. Adol.-15. Educ. level 13 -f. Urban. More or less precisely matches descrip- 
tion given above (Group 13) for whole college level at this age; but animal inter- 
course rare in this city-bred group. 



Table 152. Clinical Tables (continued) 
SE^GLE WHITE MALES 


AGE 

: ADOL.-15 

SOURCE 

OF 

OUTLET 

cases 

INCID. 

% 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 13+ 










15. 

Rural 

Total 

352 

96 

20.0 

3.1 

2.4 

— 

— 

— 



Mast. 

352 

84 

14.0 

2.7 

2.2 

100 

84.8 

94.4 



Emiss. 

352 

62 

4.0 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

28 0 

11.1 



Pet. 

352 

11 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

25 

3 6 

0.6 



Interc. 

265 

11 

4.5 

0.8 

0.3 

100 

35.7 

20.0 



Prost. 

352 

2 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

10 

3.8 

0.9 



Homo. 

352 

21 

2.5 

0.4 

0.1 

40 

6.4 

2.4 



Anim. 

352 

28 

8.0 

0.5 

0.1 

75 

9.1 

1.0 

16. 

Protest., active 

Total 

547 

95 

12.0 

2.6 

2.1 

— 

— 

— 



Mast 

547 

81 

12.0 

2.3 

1.9 

100 

81.2 

90.4 



Emiss. 

547 

71 

7.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

33.5 

17.2 



Pet. 

484 

14 

3.0 

0.4 

0.1 

60 

11.3 

0.9 



Interc. 

493 

7 

4.5 

0.8 

0.3 

100 

21.3 

8.6 



Prost 

484 

2 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

0.8 

0.6 



Homo. 

547 

24 

4.0 

0.4 

0.1 

80 

8.8 

1.7 



Anim. 

484 

9 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

30 

6,2 

0.8 

17. 

Protest, inactive 

Total 

1471 

96 

25.0 

3.2 

2.6 

— 

— 





Mast. 

1471 

85 

20.0 

2.8 

2.3 

100 

81.6 

92.1 



Emiss. 

1471 

68 

7.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

29.2 

11.5 



Pet. 

1178 

17 

2.5 

0.3 

0.1 

70 

8.2 

0.8 



Interc. 

1205 

11 

7.0 

0.8 

0.3 

100 

22.6 

10.0 



Prost. 

1178 

2 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

15 

2.5 

0.7 



Homo.' 

1471 

26 

5.0 

0.4 

0.1 

85 

7.8 

1.0 



Anim. 

1178 

7 

2.5 

0.4 

0.1 

35 

5.2 

0.8 

18. 

Cath., devout 

Total 

132 

98 

16.0 

2.5 

1.7 

— 

— 





Mast. 

132 

79 

16.0 

2.5 

1.6 

100 

83.4 

91.7 



Emiss. 

132 

75 

3.0 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

40.1 

19.5 



Pet. 

132 

8 

0.5 

0.2 

0.2 

25 

9.9 

5.5 



Interc. 

103 

5 

1.5 

0.9 

1.0 

45 

37.0 

40.0 



Prost. 

132 

2 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 

1 

4.3 

5.5 



Homo. 

132 

17 

2.5 

0.5 

0.1 

40 

14.2 

5.0 



Anim. 

98 

3 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 

1 

1.3 

1.0 

19. 

Cath., inactive 

Total 

165 

95 

15.0 

3.2 

2,3 









Mast. 

165 

81 

14.0 

2.9 

2.0 

100 

77.9 

89.4 



Emiss. 

165 

65 

3.5 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

33.9 

12.0 



Pet. 

165 

13 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

50 

17.7 

6.3 



Interc. 

117 

16 

3.0 

1.5 

0.5 

95 

33.9 

25.0 



Prost. 

165 

4 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

5 

8.0 

3.0 



Homo. 

165 

20 

2.5 

0.6 

0.2 

50 

16.6 

10.8 

20. 

Jewish, Orthod. 

Total 

58 

91 

7.0 

2.1 

1.4 

~_ 







Mast. 

58 

72 

6.0 

2.2 

1.5 

100 

76.5 

95.0 



Emiss. 

58 

78 

2.5 

0.3 

0.2 

100 

37.7 

15.0 



Pet. 

58 

21 

1.5 

0.3 

0.1 

50 

14.0 

0.9 



Interc. 

53 

11 

1.0 

0.6 

0.3 

50 

26.3 

20.0 



Prost. 

58 

5 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

0.5 

0.7 



Homo. 

58 

10 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

5 

10.9 

2.0 

21. 

Jewish, inactive 

Total 

601 

96 

29.0 

3.0 

2.3 

— 

__ 




Mast 

601 

80 

29.0 

2.9 

2.2 

100 

81.2 

93.9 



Emiss. 

601 

75 

6.5 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

36.5 

19.6 



Pet. 

377 

10 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

45 

9.1 

1.7 



Interc. 

412 

7 

3.0 

0.5 

0.1 

75 

13.2 

5.0 



Prost. 

377 

2 

0.5 

0.3 

0.1 

10 

7.7 

1.0 



Homo. 

601 

15 

* 4.5 

0.4 

0.1 

55 

9.9 

2.8 



Anim. 

377 

1 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

0.5 

1.0 


690 



CLINICAL tables: SINGLE 


691 


SESTGLE WHITE MALES 

15. Adol.-15. Educ. level 13 +. Rural. Closely matches whole college level of 
this age (Group 13). But animal intercourse with orgasm occurs among more 
than a quarter (28%), attempts with animals may occur in nearly as many more, 
so about half of these boys may be involved, with frequencies that average once 
in ten weeks. Conflicts rarely develop out of this, unless some city-bred clinician 
or other adult becomes disturbed in dealing with the boy. 

16. Adol.-15. Educ. level 13+. Protest., active. Closely similar to whole group 
of boys of college level (Group 13), with only a slightly more inhibited pattern. 
Differing chiefly in being still less often involved in intercourse (7%) and still 
more often in the homosexual (24%). Some individuals in the group, however, 
in conflict over what sexual activity they do have. 


17. Adol.~15. Educ. level 13+. Protest., inactive. Sexual pattern a close du- 
plicate of that described for whole group of young boys of college level (Group 
13). Only a bit more active in each type of sexual activity. Not so often in con- 
flict over their sexual activity, but boys of college level rarely come to consider 
pre-marital socio-sexual contacts as right. 

18. Adol.-15. Educ. level 13 +. Cath., devout. A slightly more restrained ver- 
sion of pattern typical of all college level at this age (Group 13). This identity 
emphasizes that it is group pattern rather than immediate religious background 
that is most significant (Chapter 10). Religious teaching, however, may intro- 
duce conflicts which are not infrequently observed among boys in this group. 
Emissions supply 20% of outlet for of boys. 

19. Adol.-15, Educ. level 13+« Cath., inactive. Not particularly different from 
pattern followed by all younger boys of college level (Group 13). Differing chiefly 
from devout Catholics in having 3 times as many (16%) involved in intercourse. 
Still as restrained as rest of college level, irrespective of lack of religious back- 
ground (Chapter 10). 

20. Adol.-15. Educ. level 13+. Jewish, Orthodox. Most restrained of all 
younger adolescents of college level, mean outlet only 2.1. Slower in getting 
started, depending more often on involuntary emissions; only 72% start mas- 
turbation, against which there are strong taboos. Petting more often than any 
other college level group, but intercourse in only 11% of cases. Compared with 
rest of college level, one-half as many (10%) with homosexual. The pattern lays 
groundwork for personality conflicts which are frequent in next age group of 
these males. 

21o Adol.-15. Educ. level 13+. Jewish, inactive. Closer to average for all col- 
lege level boys (Group 13), in some respects less restrained than Orthodox Jew- 
ish boys, but taboos on heterosexual petting (10%) and intercourse (7%) ap- 
parently more severe. Sometimes involved in conflicts over sexual codes, even 
though not actively connected with church. 



Table 152. Clinical Tables (continued) 
SINGLE WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 


INCID. 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 

AGE: 16-20 

OUTLET 

CASES 

% 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med 

Educ. level 0-8 

22. Whole group 

Total 

720 

97 

29 0 

3.3 

2.2 

— 





Mast. 

720 

84 

8.0 

1.1 

0.6 

100 

42.0 

34.0 


Emiss. 

720 

56 

2.5 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

12.4 

3.7 


Pet. 

720 

21 

3.0 

0.3 

0.1 

40 

3.3 

0 6 


Interc. 

635 

85 

22.0 

2.0 

1.0 

100 

57.3 

59.7 


Prost. 

720 

48 

4.0 

0 4 

0,1 

100 

15.7 

4.8 


Homo. 

720 

26 

7.0 

0.8 

0.3 

75 

16.7 

5.2 


Amm. 

720 

4 

2.5 

0.5 

0.2 

20 

4.1 

1.0 

23. Urban 

Total 

397 

98 

26.0 

3.5 

2.4 

— 

— 




Mast 

397 

85 

8.0 

1.2 

0.6 

100 

39.2 

31 0 


Emiss. 

397 

58 

2.5 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

10.4 

3.1 


Pet. 

397 

22 

3 0 

0.3 

0.1 

25 

3.0 

0.7 


Interc. 

406 

87 

22 0 

2.3 

1.3 

100 

60.2 

64.0 


Prost. 

397 

51 

4 0 

0.5 

0.2 

100 

17.3 

6.0 


Homo. 

397 

32 

5.5 

0.8 

0.3 

75 

15.6 

5.2 


Anim. 

397 

2 

1.5 

0.6 

0.1 

20 

4.9 

1.0 

24. Rural 

Total 

259 

96 

16.0 

2.9 

2.0 








Mast. 

259 

85 

7.0 

1.1 

0.7 

100 

50.7 

48.8 


Emiss. 

259 

53 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

14.1 

4.0 


Pet. 

259 

18 

1 5 

0.2 

0 1 

30 

4.1 

0.7 


Interc. 

208 

81 

14.0 

1.6 

0.7 

100 

51.1 

48.9 


Prost. 

259 

42 

3.0 

0.4 

0.1 

95 

14.1 

3.8 


Homo. 

259 

21 

7.0 

1.0 

0.4 

70 

18.1 

7.5 


Anim. 

259 

7 

2.0 

0.5 

0.3 

5 

2.2 

1.4 

25. Protest., active 

Total 

91 

93 

15.0 

2.9 

1.7 








Mast 

' 91 

81 

5.0 

1.0 

0 5 

100 

51.1 

43.3 


Emiss. 

91 

53 

2 0 

0.3 

0 1 

100 

14.2 

1.6 


Pet. 

91 

19 

1.0 

0.1 

0.1 

5 

0.9 

0.7 


Interc. 

81 

70 

14 0 

1.8 

0.6 

100 

53.0 

52.5 


Prost. 

91 

37 

1.0 

0.3 

0.1 

75 

14.8 

5.0 


Homo. 

91 

23 

3.0 

1.4 

0.8 

70 

29.6 

27.5 

26, Protest, inactive 

Total 

493 

98 

29.0 

3.4 

2.3 



__ 

_ 


Mast. 

493 

86 

8.0 

1.1 

0.6 

100 

40.9 

33.7 


Emiss. 

493 

57 

2.5 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

10.3 

2.6 


Pet. 

431 

22 

3.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

3.1 

0.7 


Interc. 

442 

90 

22.0 

2.1 

1.1 

100 

58.1 

61.1 


Prost. 

431 

49 

4 0 

0.4 

0.2 

100 

16.9 

5.2 


Homo. 

493 

24 

7.0 

0.8 

0.3 

75 

14.4 

4.1 


Anim. 

431 

4 

2.5 

0.4 

0.1 

20 

3.1 

0.9 

27. Cath., inactive 

Total 

105 

98 

12 0 

3.5 

2.6 





Mast. 

105 

85 

7.0 

1.2 

0.7 

100 

45.1 

36 9 


Emiss. 

105 

57 

1.0 

0.3 

0.2 

40 

11.6 

6.3 


Pet. 

105 

23 

1 0 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

1.9 

0.7 


Interc. 

88 

81 

10 0 

2.3 

1.3 

100 

57.5 

60.0 


Prost. 

105 

59 

3.0 

0.5 

0.2 

100 

14.1 

5.6 

Educ. level 9-12 

28. Whole group 

Homo. 

105 

35 

4.0 

1.0 

0.4 

60 

17.1 

4.6 

Total 

607 

100 

26 0 

3.5 

2.7 





Mast. 

607 

89 

15 0 

1.5 

0.8 

100 

45.4 

39.2 


Emiss. 

607 

71 

3.0 

0,3 

0.1 

100 

13.6 

3.9 


Pet. 

607 

34 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

50 

5.5 

0.7 


Interc. 

515 

76 

25,0 

1.9 

0.9 

100 

51.7 

55.8 


Prost. 

607 

41 

2.5 

0.2 

0.1 

100 

9.5 

0.9 


Homo. 

607 

41 

11.0 

0.9 

0.3 

100 

19.0 

5.3 


Anim 

607 

5 

3.5 

0.8 

0 2 

65 

15.5 

5.8 


692 



CLINICAL tables: SINGLE 


693 


SINGLE WHITE MALES 

22. Age 16-20. Ediic. level 0-8. Whole group. Most boys sexually most active 
at this age, mean frequencies 3.3 per week, often 10, or more. Masturbation still 
in 84%, becoming strongly taboo at this level, constituting only 34% of outlet. 
Intercourse expected, freely accepted (Chapter 10), in 85%, contributing 60% of 
outlet; rnales often promiscuous, often with venereal disease, often in conflict 
with society because of sexual activity. Petting in 21%, a very minor outlet. 
Homosexual in a quarter (26%), but publicly condemned and infrequent. 
Nocturnal emissions in half (56%), very minor outlet. Individuals who were ado- 
lescent by 12 most active of all single males (Chapter 9); but mentally and physi- 
cally poorer individuals often with low rates. Little psychic eroticism. Often 
strong taboos against nudity, oral eroticism, and some petting techniques. 

23. Age 16-20. Educ. level 0-8. Urban. Typical of whole grade school level 
(Group 22). More active than corresponding rural males (Group 24), more often 
involved in intercourse and homosexual, less often with animals. A third of out- 
let from solitary sources, two-thirds from socio-sexual contacts. 

24. Age 16-20. Educ. level 0-8. Rural. Typical of whole grade school level 
(Group 22), not quite so active. Not so many (81%) with intercourse, only 21% 
with homosexual. Masturbation and intercourse account for most of outlet, 
equally important. Animal intercourse still in 7%, of minor significance at this 
age. 

25. Age 16-20. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., active. Somewhat less active than 
average of grade school level (Group 22), much more active than any college 
group. Intercourse still accepted in 70%, accounting for 53% of outlet. Masturba- 
tion in 81% but taboo, accounting for only 43% of outlet. Homosexual in nearly 
a quarter (23%), accounting for 28% of their outlet. Other activities minor, 

26. Age 16-20. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. Very close to average for 
whole lower level (Group 22), in most outlets somewhat more active. Intercourse 
now in 90% for whom it provides nearly two-thirds (61%) of outlet. Masturba- 
tion still in 86%, but taboo and provides only a third of outlet. All else very 
minor. Rarely any conflict over behavior. 

27. Age 16-20. Educ. level 0-8. Cath., inactive. A rather active group of lower 
level boys, averaging 3.5 per week, often 7 or more. Intercourse in 81%, account- 
ing for 60% of outlet. Masturbation in 85%, accounting for only 37% of outlet. 
All other activities minor. Homosexual in 35%, but accounting for less than 5% 
of orgasms even for those males. Quite acceptant of this pattern, not often with 
conflicts. 

28. Age 16-20. Educ. level 9-12. Whole group. This age at high school and 
grade school levels (Group 22) more active than any other single males (especially 
if adolescent by 12). Masturbation in 89% but more or less taboo, accounting for 
39% of outlet. Intercourse in three-quarters (76%), accounting for 56% of outlet. 
Often highly promiscuous, not infrequently acquiring venereal disease. Noc- 
turnal emissions, petting, prostitutes, homosexual in many histories, but minor 
as sources of outlet. 41% experiment with homosexual, often severely condemn- 
ing it but clearly aroused by it. Otherwise little conflict over their pattern. Espe- 
cially acceptant of heterosexual activity, although this may bring difficulty with 
police. More erotic psychically than grade school level; more or less accept 
nudity and oral eroticism; involved in petting as often as college males. 



Table 152 . Clinical Tables {continued) 
SINGLE WHITE MALES 




SOURCE 

CASES 

INCID. 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 

AGE 

: 16-20 

OF 








OUTLET 


/o 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 9-12 










29. 

Urban 

Total 

520 

100 

26.0 

3.6 

2.7 

— 

— 

— 


Mast. 

520 

90 

8.0 

1.5 

0.9 

100 

45.4 

39.7 



Emiss 

520 

71 

3.0 

0.3 

0 1 

100 

13.4 

4.2 



Pet. 

458 

34 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

50 

6.0 

0.8 



Interc. 

405 

73 

25.0 

1.9 

0.9 

100 

51.6 

55.6 



Prost. 

458 

41 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

85 

9.5 

1.0 



Homo. 

520 

46 

11.0 

1 0 

0.4 

100 

20.6 

6.6 



Anim. 

458 

3 

3.0 

0.7 

0.1 

25 

7.0 

1.0 

30. 

Rural 

Total 

124 

99 

17.0 

3.2 

2.5 

— 

— 

— 



Mast. 

124 

85 

7.0 

1.3 

0.8 

100 

47.0 

39.2 



Emiss. 

124 

72 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

12.3 

2.3 



Pet. 

124 

31 

1.0 

0.1 

0.1 

30 

3.6 

0.7 



Interc. 

95 

87 

7.0 

1.7 

1.0 

100 

51.8 

56.3 



Prost. 

124 

41 

2.5 

0.3 

0.1 

25 

5.2 

0 8 



Homo. 

124 

26 

2.0 

0 4 

0.1 

55 

9.4 

0.8 



Anim. 

124 

10 

2 5 

0 9 

0.4 

65 

27. '8 

25.0 

31. 

Protest., active 

Total 

95 

100 

9.5 

2 5 

1.7 

— 

— 

— 



Mast. 

95 

88 

6.0 

1 3 

0.7 

100 

51.8 

50.0 



Emiss. 

95 

72 

2 0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

19.4 

6.4 



Pet. 

95 

32 

1.0 

0.2 

0 1 

35 

6 5 

0.8 



Interc. 

89 

67 

6 0 

1.3 

0.7 

100 

49.8 

55.8 



Prost. 

95 

32 

1.0 

0.2 

0 1 

20 

3.6 

0.8 



Homo. 

95 

31 

3 0 

0.7 

0.1 

so 

14.8 

0.9 

32. 

Protest., inactive 

Total 

315 

99 

21.0 

3.6 

2.7 

— 







Mast. 

315 

89 

7.0 

1.4 

0.8 

100 

43.4 

37.3 



Emiss. 

315 

73 

3.0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

11.5 

3.2 



Pet. 

315 

33 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

60 

6.3 

0.7 



Interc. 

322 

80 

20.0 

2 0 

0.9 

100 

52.9 

57.0 



Prost. 

315 

44 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

100 i 

8.9 

0.9 



Homo. 

315 

37 

6.0 

0.8 

0.2 

100 i 

16.4 

4.7 



Anim. 

315 

6 

3.5 

0.9 

0.4 

65 

16.2 

6.7 

33. 

Cath., inactive 

Total 

101 

100 

19.0 

4.4 

3.2 









Mast. 

101 

91 

8.0 

1.7 

1.0 

100 

44.0 

39.6 



Emiss. 

101 

60 

2.0 

0.3 

0.2 

50 

10.1 

5.0 



Pet. 

101 

36 

2.0 

0.4 

0.1 

30 

6.5 

0.8 



Interc. 

69 

68 

13.0 

2.0 

1.0 

100 

54.6 

57.5 



Prost. 

101 

45 

1.5 

0.2 

0.1 

75 

9.2 

0.9 


1 

Homo. 

101 

59 

6.0 

1.4 

0.6 

85 

29.3 

22.5 

Educ. level 134- 










34. 

Whole group 

Total 

2861 

100 

24.0 

2.7 

2.1 

— 

— 

— 


Mast. 

2861 

89 

23.0 

2.0 

1.5 

100 

68.3 

76.0 



Emiss. 

2861 

91 

6.5 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

25.4 

12.7 



Pet. 

2861 

46 

5.0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

10.7 

0.9 



Interc. 

2475 

42 

12.0 

0.6 

0.2 

100 

18.5 

4.9 



Prost. 

2861 

19 

1.5 

0.1 

0.1 

50 

2.4 

0.6 



Homo. 

2861 

16 

7.0 

0.4 

0.1 

90 

10.1 

1.1 



Anim. 

2861 

2 

2.0 

0.4 

0.1 

50 

10.1 

2.8 

35. 

Urban 

Total 

2762 

100 

24.0 

2.8 

2.2 









Mast. 

2762 

89 

23.0 

2.1 

1.6 

100 

66.2 

71.8 



Emiss. 

2762 

91 

6.5 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

25.1 

11.8 



Pet. 

2762 

48 

5.0 

0.3 

0.1 

95 

12.1 

0.9 



Interc. 

2172 

43 

12.0 

0.6 

0.2 

100 

18.9 

5.4 



Prost. 

2640 

20 

1.5 

0.1 

0.1 

40 

2.0 

0.6 



Homo. 

2762 

16 

7.0 

0.4 

0.1 

70 

10.3 

1.0 



Anim. 

2640 

1 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

10 

5.5 

0.8 


694 



CLINICAL TABLES SINGLE 


695 


SINGLE WHITE MALES 

29. Age 1^20. Educ. level 9-12. Urban. Fits description given above for high 
school level in late teens (Group 28). More often (46%) experiments with homo- 
sexual; but intercourse the accepted source, accounting for 56% of outlet. Often 
highly promiscuous, often (in 41%) with prostitutes. 

30. Age 16-20. Educ. level 9-12. Rural. Not significantly different from whole 
high school level (Group 28). Differs from corresponding urban groups in being 
involved in intercourse in more cases (87%), less often in homosexual (26%). 
Animal intercourse in 10%, this a rather material outlet (25%) for males in- 
volved. 

31. Age 16-20. Educ. level 9-12, Protest., active. Pattern typical of whole high 
school level (Group 28). Showing little effect of church connections, unless an 
occasional individual is disturbed by conflicts between religious teaching and 
group pattern which he actually follows. Intercourse in somewhat fewer histories 
(67%), homosexual in fewer (31%). 

32. Age 16-20. Educ. level 9-12. Protest., inactive. Pattern typical of whole 
high school level (Group 28). The group pattern predominates. Not materially 
different from active Protestant group, except more (80%) involved in inter- 
course; 44% with prostitutes; 37% with homosexual experience. Intercourse 
chief source (57%) of outlet, masturbation next (37%). 

33. Age 16-20. Educ. level 9-12. Cath., inactive. Most unrestrained and sexu- 
ally most active of all high school level (Group 28), with almost double the outlet 
of active Protestants. Mean rate totals 4.4, higher than in any other single males 
except individuals who become adolescent at early ages. Incidences and rates 
high in masturbation, petting, intercourse with prostitutes, and homosexual 
(59%). Incidences and rates lower for nocturnal emissions and (strangely enough) 
for intercourse (68%). Few sexual conflicts, though often social dfficulties from 
sexual activity. 

34. Age 16-20. Educ. level 13+. Whole group. Highly restrained (Chapter 10), 
largely avoiding socio-sexual contacts on moral grounds (often rationalized as 
“sensible” or “decent”). Even those who pet to climax (46%), have intercourse 
(42%), or homosexual (1 6%), derive less than 7% of total outlet from such sources, 
over 90% from solitary sources (masturbation, nocturnal emissions). Few part- 
ners in socio-sexual contacts. Much psychic eroticism that does not lead to inter- 
course. Many individuals appear to accept such a regime, but many actually 
tense in socio-sexual situations. Some much disturbed over few contacts they do 
have. Masturbation, petting, oral eroticism, nudity more often accepted. Most 
American-trained clinicians come from this group, and may therefore need to 
make special effort to recognize inhibitions and conflicts inherent in such asocial 
patterns. 

35. Age 16-20. Educ. level 13 +. Urban. Typical of whole college level (Group 
34). 



Table 152 . Clinical Tables {continued) 
SESTGLE WHITE MALES 




SOURCE 


INCID . 

FREQ 

PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 



OF 

CASES 








AGE 

: 16-20 

OUTLET 


/o 

Range 

Mean 

Med 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 13+ 










36. 

Rural 

Total 

363 

100 

13.0 

2 5 

2 0 

— 

— 

— 



Mast. 

363 

89 

8.0 

1.9 

1.4 

100 

73.3 

79 0 



Emiss. 

363 

92 

5.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

24.5 

13.7 



Pet. 

363 

35 

2.0 

0.2 

0 1 

75 

5.1 

0 7 



Interc. 

272 

36 

5.5 

0 5 

0.1 

95 

16 6 

3.0 



Frost. 

363 

13 

0.5 

0 1 

0.1 

10 

1.7 

0.6 



Homo. 

363 

17 

2 0 

0 3 

0 1 

50 

7.7 

2.0 



Anim. 

363 

16 

2.0 

0.5 

0.1 

50 

11.7 

4.2 

37. 

Protest., active 

Total 

557 

100 

11 0 

i 2.3 

1.8 

— 

_ 

_ 



Mast. 

557 

86 

10.0 

1 8 

1.3 

100 

71.3 

77.4 



Emiss. 

557 

92 

5.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

29.2 

16.7 



Pet. 

492 

40 

3.5 

0.4 

0.1 

100 

12.7 

1 0 



Interc. 

502 

27 

7.0 

0.6 

0 1 

100 

15.2 

2.4 



Prost. 

492 

11 

0.5 

0 1 

0 1 

20 

2.5 

0.6 



Homo. 

557 

17 

4.0 

0 4 

0 1 

75 

8.3 

1.3 



Anim 

492 

4 

1.0 

0.3 

0..^ 

40 

10.1 

3.4 

38. 

Protest., inactive 

Total 

1513 

100 

21.0 

2.9 

2.4 

— 

— 

— 



Mast 

1513 

91 

14.0 

2.1 

1.6 

100 

68.9 

76.2 



Emiss. 

1513 

92 

6.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

24.3 

12.3 



Pet. 

1210 

47 

4.0 

0.3 

0 1 

90 

9.3 

0.9 



Interc. 

1235 

45 

9.0 

0.6 

0.2 

100 

18.1 

6.1 



Prost. 

1210 

20 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

25 

1.4 

0.6 



Homo. 

1513 

18 

5.0 

0.4 

0.1 

90 

8 9 1 

0.9 



Anim. 

1210 

3 

2.0 

0.6 

0.1 

50 

9.7 

2.3 

39. 

Cath., devout 

Total 

136 

100 

12 0 

2.4 

1.7 





— 



Mast. 

136 

84 

12 0 

1 8 

1.0 

100 

66.4 

75.6 



Emiss. 

136 

94 

4 5 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

34 2 

24.2 



Pet. 

136 

43 

2.0 

0.4 

0.1 

60 

14 0 

5.0 



Interc. 

107 

39 

2.0 

0.5 

0.1 

100 

19.7 

1 5.4 



Prost. 

136 

16 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

2.0 

0.7 



Homo. 

136 

15 

1.0 

0.3 

1 0.1 

15 

5.5 

2.2 

40. 

Cath., inactive 

Total 

168 

100 

13.0 

2.8 

2 2 





— 



Mast. 

168 

86 

11.0 

1.9 

1.5 

100 

56.2 

58.8 



Emiss. 

168 

91 

3.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

29 2 

14.0 



Pet. 

168 

'50 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

50 

9.5 

1.0 



Interc. 

120 

59 

5.5 

0.9 

0.3 

100 

29.1 

13.8 



Prost. 

168 

30 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

3.5 

0.7 



Homo. 

168 

20 

3.0 

0.7 

0.3 

70 

19.7 

10.0 

41. 

Jewish, Orthod. 

Total 

59 

100 

6.0 

2.0 

1.6 









Mast. 

59 

: 78 

6.0 

1.5 

0.9 

100 

64.3 

72.5 



Emiss. 

59 

90 

2.0 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

37.0 

23.8 



Pet. 

59 

46 

1.5 

0.3 

0.1 

60 

11.0 

1.0 



Interc. 

54 

39 

2.5 

0.6 

0.3 

85 

28.0 

15.0 



Prost. 

59 

20 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

0.7 

0.6 



Homo. 

59 

12 

0.5 

0.2 

0,1 

15 

9.8 

0.8 

42. 

Jewish, inactive 

Total 

607 

100 

23.0 

3.0 

2.3 









Mast. 

607 

88 

23.0 

2.4 

1.7 

100 

70.4 

78,2 



Emiss. 

607 

86 

6.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

29,6 

15.7 



Pet. 

379 

48 

3.5 

0.3 

0.1 

80 

9.9 

2.6 



Interc. 

416 

46 

5.5 

0.5 

0.1 

95 

14.6 

2.8 



Prost. 

379 

22 

1.5 

0.1 

0.1 

50 

3.8 

0.6 



Homo. 

607 

11 

3.5 

0.6 

0.1 

70 

12.7 

2.0 


696 



CLINICAL TABLES *. SINGLE 


697 


SINGLE WHITE MALES 

36. Age 16-20. Educ. level 13 +. Rural. Close to pattern for whole college level 
(Group 34), a bit less active. Incidences, frequencies, and percents of total outlet 
a bit lower in petting, total intercourse, intercourse with prostitutes. Animal in- 
tercourse in 16%, attempts in still others; but provides only 4% of outlet. 

37. Age 16-20. Educ. level 13+. Protest., active. Pattern close to that for 
whole college level (Group 34), but still lower incidences, frequencies, and per- 
cents of total outlet. Intercourse in only 27%, lower than in any other group of 
this age, as against 80% to 91% incidence figures in other educational levels at 
this same age. 

38. Age 16-20. Educ. level 13+. Protest., inactive. Typical of whole college 
level (Group 34). Because they are not active in church, many of this group con- 
sider themselves emancipated sexually, and a few more (45%) do have inter- 
course, but even they derive only 6% of outlet therefrom. Actually about as re- 
strained as actively religious groups; and clinician should realize these males are 
equally capable of being disturbed over the few socio-sexual contacts they do 
have. 

39. Age 16-20. Educ. level 13+. Cath., devout. Close to pattern for whole col- 
lege level (Group 34), showing remarkably little effect of devoutly religious back- 
ground. Slightly lower incidences in most outlets, this raising importance but not 
actual frequencies of nocturnal emissions. Group pattern (Chapter 10) more im- 
portant than immediate religious influences (Chapter 13). 

40. Age 16-20. Educ. level 13+. Cath., inactive. Close to pattern for whole 
college level (Group 34), not nearly as distinct as religiously inactive groups are 
at lower educational levels. Hardly differs from devout Catholic group except in 
lower percent of outlet from masturbation and emissions, higher incidence (59%) 
of intercourse, and higher rates and percent of outlet from that source. 

41. Age 16-20. Educ. level 13+. Jewish, Orthodox. Most restrained and sex- 
ually least active among all groups of college level (Group 34). Total outlet only 
2.0 per week. Incidences lower in masturbation (78%), intercourse (39%), homo- 
sexual (12%). Total outlet largely from masturbation (73%) and nocturnal emis- 
sions (24%). Relation of sexual restraint and neuroses should be given especial 
attention by clinician handling individual cases in this group. 

42. Age 16-20. Educ. level 13+. Jewish, inactive. Very similar to pattern of 
whole college level (Group 34). Much restraint still to be recognized here. Re- 
markably close to Orthodox pattern (Group 41), even though present group not 
actively connected with synagogue. A few more (46%) have intercourse, but per- 
cent of total outlet from intercourse (3%) actually lower than in Orthodox group 
(15%); much of intercourse commercially secured from prostitutes. 



Table 152. Clinical Tables {continued) 
SINGLE WHITE MALES 




SOURCE 


INCID. 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 












AGE: 21-25 

OUTLET 


/o 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 0-8 










43. 

Whole group 

Total 

361 

97 

26.0 

3.3 

2.0 

— 

— 

— 



Mast. 

361 

62 

7.0 

1.0 

0.5 

100 

37.5 

24.8 



Emiss. 

361 

60 

2 0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

13.2 

4.0 



Pet. 

361 

16 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

25 

2.8 

0.6 



Interc. 

312 

86 

18.0 

2 3 

1.1 

100 

66.4 

76.3 



Prost. 

361 

61 

7.0 

0.6 

0.3 

100 

28.2 

16.3 



Homo. 

361 

22 

6.0 

1.1 

0.4 

90 

21.7 

9 0 



Anim. 

361 

1 

1.0 

0.4 

0.3 

Scant 

9.3 

10.5 

44. 

Urban 

Total 

188 

98 

26.0 

3.4 

2.0 



— 





Mast. 

188 

63 

7.0 

1.0 

0.5 

100 

34.2 

22.5 



Emiss. 

188 

60 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

11.8 

3.5 



Pet. 

188 

16 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

15 

2.4 

0.6 



Interc. 

195 

91 

18.0 

2.6 

1.4 

100 

68.4 

78.8 



Prost, 

188 

66 

5.5 

0.7 

0.4 

100 

29.6 

18.1 



Homo. 

188 

29 

5.0 

0.9 

0.4 

100 

20.6 

10.0 

45. 

Rural 

Total 

141 

95 

19.0 

3.0 

1.8 









Mast. 

141 

64 

5.5 

1.1 

0.6 

100 

48.0 

45.0 



Emiss. 

141 

57 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

16.4 

4.8 



Pet. 

141 

13 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

15 

5.7 

0.9 



Interc. 

106 

80 

14.0 

1.8 

0.8 

100 

62.8 

68.0 



Prost. 

141 

50 

4.5 

0.6 

0.3 

100 

27.3 

13.8 



Homo. 

141 

17 

3.5 

1.5 

0.4 

70 

32.8 

25.0 



Anim. 

141 

3 

1.0 

0.4 

0.3 

Scant 

6.3 

1.0 

46. 

Protest., inactive 

Total 

234 

98 

26.0 

3.3 

2.1 









Mast. 

234 

62 

6.0 

1.0 

0.5 

100 

38.2 

25.0 



Emiss. 

234 

59 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

95 

11.4 

3.0 



Pet. 

234 

15 

1.5 

0.3 

0.1 

40 

4.3 

0.7 



Interc. 

200 

92 

18.0 

2.4 

1.2 

100 

67.4 

77.7 



Prost. 

234 

64 

5.0 

0.6 

0.3 

100 

28.1 

16.5 



Homo. 

234 

19 

4.0 

1.1 

0.5 

70 

20.7 

11.3 

47. 

Cath., inactive 

Total 

60 

98 

12.0 

3.3 

2.3 

_ 





Mast. 

60 

67 

4.0 

0.9 

0.4 

100 

32.7 

22.5 



Emiss. 

60 

65 

1.5 

0.3 

0.2 

50 

13.4 

8.5 



Pet. 

60 

23 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

3.5 

0.8 



Interc, 

57 

86 1 

10.0 

2.3 

1.3 

100 1 

68.9 

75.0 



Prost. 

60 

63 

5.5 

0.9 

0.4 

100 

39.9 

30.0 



Homo. 

60 

40 

3.5 

0.9 

0.3 

90 

21.1 

4.6 

Educ. level 9-12 










48. 

Whole group 

Total 

263 

99 

25.0 

3.0 

2.3 









Mast. 

263. 

76 

7.0 

1.1 

0.6 

100 

40.9 

35.4 



Emiss. 

263 

71 

2.0 

0.3 

0.2 

100 

16.5 

7.1 



Pet. 

263 

28 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

65 

8.9 

0.8 



Interc. 

217 

74 

25.0 

1.7 

0.8 

100 

53.9 

58.3 



Prost. 

263 

44 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

15.6 

2.9 



Homo. 

263 

38 

11.0 

1.3 

0.7 

100 

33.7 

29.2 



Anim. 

263 

3 

0.5 

0.5 

0.1 

15 

17.2 

15.0 

49. 

Urban 

Total 

209 

100 

17,0 

3.2 

2.6 






Mast. 

209 

79 

7.0 

1.2 

0.7 

100 

41.5 

37.5 



Emiss. 

209 

70 

2.0 

0.4 

0.2 

80 

15.4 

7.5 



Pet. 

209 

29 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

65 

9.8 

0.8 



Interc. 

172 

72 

25.0 

1.6 

0.7 

100 

51.7 

52.5 



Prost. 

209 

41 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

90 

15.0 

2.5 



Homo. 

209 

43 

14.0 

1.6 

0.9 

100 

36.9 

35.0 



Anim. 

209 

2 

Scant 

0.7 

0.1 

Scant 

0.5 

1.0 


698 



CLINICAL TABLES : SINGLE 


699 


SINGLE WHITE MALES 

43. Age 21-25. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Lower level males are usually 
married by this age. If single, total outlets still average 3.3 per week. Most males 
ignore the law and have intercourse (86%), some (61%) with prostitutes, 62% 
still masturbate, 60% have emissions, 22% have homosexual. of outlet from 
intercourse (including 16% from prostitutes), highly promiscuous. Only 34 
outlet from masturbation. All other outlets minor. Personality disturbances over 
sex rare, more often products of inadequate personalities rather than of sex. 
Inhibited only in regard to nudity, oral eroticism, and other petting techniques. 

44. Age 21-25. Educ. level 0-8. Urban. More active than average of whole 
lower level (Group 43). Intercourse accepted by 91%, 66% having some with 
prostitutes, nearly 80% of outlet from intercourse. Masturbation once every two 
weeks for 63%, homosexual nearly as often for 29%. Group accepts sex, may be 
promiscuous, is often infected with venereal disease. 


45. Age 21-25. Educ. level 0-8. Rural. Rather less active than city males 
(Group 44), having notably fewer socio-sexual contacts; intercourse, petting, 
prostitutes, and homosexual all lower. Intercourse supplies 68% of outlet for 
80% of farm-bred males, masturbation supplies 45% for 64%, homosexual sup- 
plies 25% of outlet for 17%. At this age, animal intercourse minor for Eastern 
rural males, often a distinct part of Western rural histories. 

46. Age 21-25. Educ, level 0-8. Protest., inactive. Typical of lower level group, 
as described above (Group 43). 

47. Age 21-25. Educ. level 0-8. Cath., inactive. Not differing from whole lower 
level group (Group 43), except in an apparently higher incidence (40%) of homo- 
sexual contacts. At this age, inactive Protestants and inactive Catholics are so 
far away from their churches that religion does not have enough direct effect to 
differentiate two groups. 

48. Age 21-25. Educ. level 9-12. Whole group. Active, with mean total outlet 
of 3.0, but this lower than in younger males (mean of 3.3). Many involved in wide 
variety of activities. Most important is intercourse (for 74% this averages 58% 
of outlet), masturbation (for 76%, averages 35% of outlet), and homosexual (for 
38%, averages 29% of outlet). Homosexual attractive to group, but many evi- 
dence conflict by condemning it and punishing the homosexual partner. This and 
next age group includes the street-walking, tavern-frequenting, exhibitionistic 
city homosexual group. Masturbation often shunned, not really accepted. All 
other outlets minor. Many accept nudity, oral eroticism, and petting; others as 
inhibited as grade school level (Group 43) on these points. 

49. Age 21-25. Educ. level 9-12. Urban. Adequately covered by description of 
whole high school level (Group 48). A larger number (43%) has some homosexual 
experience, drawing 34 theix outlet from that source. 



Table 152 . Clinical Tables (continued) 
SINGLE WHITE MALES 




SOURCE 



1 FREQ. PER WEEK 

1 % OF TOTAL OUTLET 





INCID. 







AGE 

21-25 

OUTLET 


% 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

L^auc. level 










50. 

Protest., inactive 

Total 

130 

99 

14.0 

2.9 

2.1 

— 

— 





Mast. 

130 

75 

5.0 

1.1 

0.7 

100 

39.8 

35.0 



Emiss. 

130 

72 

2.0 

0.4 

0.2 

80 

15.9 

7.5 



Pet. 

130 

26 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

35 

7.0 

0.8 



Interc. 

132 

80 

11.0 

1.6 

0.7 

100 

55.3 

60.0 



Prost. 

130 

46 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

100 

14.1 

1.6 



Homo. 

130 

32 

5.0 

1.2 

0.8 

100 

32.2 

30.8 

Educ. level 13+ 










51. 

Whole group 

Total 

1898 

100 

21.0 

2.5 

1.9 

— 

— 





Mast. 

1898 

87 

15.0 

1.5 

0.9 

100 

59.4 

65.2 



Emiss. 

1898 

87 

7.0 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

26.6 

13.6 



Pet. 

1898 

52 

7.0 

0.4 

0.1 

100 

14.6 

2.3 



Interc. 

1593 

54 

17.0 

0.8 

0.3 

100 

27.4 

15.3 



Prost. 

1898 

17 

3.0 

0.2 

0.1 

40 

3.5 

0.7 



Homo. 

1898 

9 

7.0 

1.0 

0.3 

95 

24.5 

9.6 



Anim. 

1898 

1 

1.0 

0.3 

0.1 

35 

10.1 

3.0 

52. 

Urban 

Total 

1844 

100 

21.0 

2.6 

2.0 



_ 




Mast. 

1844 

87 

15.0 

1.6 

0.9 

100 

57.5 

61.4 



Emiss. 

T844 

86 

6.5 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

28.4 

16.4 



Pet. 

1753 

54 

7.0 

0.4 

0.1 

100 

14.8 

3.7 



Interc. 

1377 

55 

17.0 

0.8 

0.3 

100 

27.6 

15.8 



Prost. 

1753 

18 

3.0 

0.2 

0.1 

40 

4.4 

0.8 



Homo. 

1844 

10 

7.0 

0.9 

0.3 

100 

25.9 

15.0 



Amm. 

1753 

1 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

0.5 

0.7 

53. 

Rural 

Total 

266 

99 

9.0 

2.2 

1.7 






Mast. 

266 

86 

7.0 

1.4 

1.0 

100 

66.2 

73.6 



Emiss. 

266 

89 

4.5 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

29.1 

14.4 



Pet. 

266 

44 

1.5 

0.2 

0.1 

45 

6.2 

0.7 



Interc. 

200 

47 

6.0 

0.7 

0.3 

100 

26.0 

9.7 



Prost. 

266 

16 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

5 

1.1 

0.6 



Homo. 

266 

9 

2.5 

0.6 

0.2 

60 

15.1 

6.3 



Anim. 

266 

4 

1.0 

0.4 

0.3 

35 

14.3 

12.5 

54. 

Protest., active 

Total 

384 

100 

11.0 

2.0 

1.4 






Mast. 

384 

84 

6.0 

1.3 

0.8 

100 

63.5 

68.8 



Emiss. 

384 

90 

5.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

35.3 

23.7 



Pet. 

339 

48 

3.0 

0.4 

0.1 

90 

14.6 

1.4 



Interc. 

348 

32 

7.5 

0.6 

0.2 

95 

19.7 

5.8 



Prost. 

339 

7 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

30 

5.0 

0.8 



Homo. 

384 

6 

3.5 

0.6 

0.2 

40 

15.5 

6.9 



Anim. 

339 

1 

0.5 

0.2 

0.2 

25 

16.8 

13.0 

55. 

Protest., inactive 

Total 

1000 

100 

17.0 

2.6 

2.0 






Mast. 

1000 

90 

12.0 

1.6 

0.9 

100 

59.9 

65.2 



Emiss. 

1000 

87 

6.0 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

24.6 

12.4 



Pet. 

841 

51 

4.5 

0.4 

0.1 

95 

13.0 

1.1 



Interc. 

867 

59 

9.0 

0.7 

0.3 

100 

26.1 

13.9 



Prost. 

841 

19 

1.0 

0.1 

0.1 

30 

2.6 

0.7 



Homo. 

1000 

11 

6.5 

0.8 

0.2 

100 

22.6 

8.1 



Anim. 

841 

1 

1.0 

0.4 

0.1 

20 

8.6 

5.0 

56. 

Cath., devout 

Total 

94 

100 

7.0 

1.9 

1.3 






Mast. 

94 

79 

5.0 

1.0 

0.5 

100 

53.6 

52.5 



Emiss. 

94 

90 

3.5 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

41.4 

34.2 



Pet. 

94 

53 

1.5 

0.3 

0.1 

55 

15,4 

6.3 



Interc. 

71 

38 

4.0 

0.7 

0.3 

100 

26.2 

20.0 



Prost. 

94 

12 

0.5 

0.3 

0.1 

Scant 

3.6 

0.8 



Homo. 

94 

11 

2.0 

0.7 

0,3 

35 

16.8 

. 

3.0 


700 


CLINICAL tables: SINGLE 


701 


SINGLE WHITE MATES 

50. Age 21-25. Educ. level 9-12. Protest., inactive. Almost exactly as described 
for whole high school level at this age (Group 48). 

51. Age 21-25. Educ. level 13+. Whole group. Many college level males are 
still unmarried at this age. They often disclaim moral restraints, but are most in- 
hibited of all social levels. Highly erotic, often involved in social relations and 
petting with females, and highly aroused but still avoiding coitus. Some upset 
when they do have intercourse or (particularly) the homosexual. Hardly more 
than attempt intercourse, and they derive only 15% of outlet from it. 87% still 
depend on masturbation for % of outlet, rarely upset over that. 87% derive 14% 
of outlet from emissions. Other outlets minor. About 69% of outlet from solitary 
sources. In contrast to grade school group, this college group freely accepts nud- 
ity and elaborate petting techniques. Oral techniques in experience of 35%, more 
often in next age period. 

52. Age 21-25. Educ. level 13 +. Urban. Almost exactly as described for whole 
college level of this age (Group 51). 

53. Age 21-25. Educ. level 13+* Rural. Very close to their urban equivalents 
(Group 52), therefore closely matching the description given for whole college 
level at this age (Group 51). A little less often in socio-sexual contacts — petting 
in 44%, intercourse in 47%, homosexual in 9%. 86% of group derives ^ of outlet 
from masturbation. Animal intercourse remains in histories of 4%, who derive 
12.5% of their outlet therefrom. 

54. Age 21-25. Educ. level 13+. Protest., active. Definitely restrained version 
of college group (Group 51). Even more likely to present cases of conflict over 
sexual activities they do have. Masturbation in 84%, provides 69% of outlet. 
Depend on emissions (average 24% of outlet for 90% of males) more often than 
non-rehgious males. Petting in only 48%, intercourse in 32%, prostitutes in 7%, 
homosexual in 6% — all minor sources of outlet. 

55. Age 21-25. Educ. level 13+. Protest., inactive. Definitely less restrained 
than corresponding group of active Protestants (Group 54). More (90%) accept 
masturbation; emissions are less important. Petting (51%), intercourse (59%), 
with prostitutes ( 19 %), and homosexual (11%) are decidedly more frequent, and 
all had with less disturbance than in religious group. 

56. Age 21-25. Educ. level 13+. Cath., devout. College level pattern (Group 
51), more restrained than average, but less restrained than active Protestants. 
Sometimes in serious conflict over sex (although often claiming to be examples 
of sublimation). Mean total outlet (1.9), petting (53%) same as in non-devout, 
but intercourse in only half as many (38%). 



Table 152. Clinical Tables {continued) 
SINGLE WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 

OF 

OUTLET 

CASES 

INCID. ! 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 

AGE: 21-25 

% 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 134- 










57. Cath., inactive 

Total 

125 

100 

14.0 

2.9 

2.1 

— 

— 

— 


Mast. 

125 

82 

8.5 

1.4 

0.8 

100 

42.0 

40 0 


Emiss. 

125 

88 

3.0 

0 5 

0.3 

100 

27.9 

14.2 


Pet. 

125 

52 

3.0 

0.3 

0.1 

90 

13.8 

3.0 


Interc. 

80 

75 

6.0 

1.3 

0.5 

100 

40.8 

32.5 


Prost. 

125 

27 

1.5 

0.2 

0.1 

35 

6.4 

1.5 


Homo. 

125 

15 

4.5 

1.7 

1.5 

90 

45.1 

47.5 

58. Jewish, inactive 

Total 

331 

100 

17.0 

3.1 

2.4 



— 




Mast. 

331 

86 

15.0 

1.8 

1.0 

100 

54.3 

54.0 


Emiss. 

331 

81 

6.5 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

26.9 

16.4 


Pet. 

331 

69 

3.0 

0.3 

0.2 

90 

15.0 

5.6 


Interc. 

182 

67 

12.0 

1.1 

0.4 

100 

30.8 

21.9 


Prost. 

331 

24 

2.0 

0 3 

0.1 

10 

1.7 

0.7 

AGE: 26-30 

Homo. 

331 

8 

3.5 

0.9 

0.1 

75 

42.0 

47.5 

59. Whole group 

Total 

550 

99 

17.0 

2.7 

1.9 



— 



Mast. 

550 

77 

9.0 

1.2 

0.7 

100 

47.3 

45.3 


Emiss. 

550 

79 

4.0 

0.4 

0.2 

100 

23.0 

10.7 


Pet. 

550 

34 

4 0 

0.3 

0.1 

90 

12.3 

0.9 


Interc. 

607 

66 

16 0 

1.5 

0.7 

100 

49 3 

46.8 


Prost. 

550 

30 

4.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

22.8 

10.8 


Homo. 

550 

25 

15.0 

1.5 

0.7 

100 

37.0 

28.9 

Educ. level 0-8 










60. Whole group 

Total 

159 

99 

19.0 

3.1 

2.0 

— 

— 

— 

Mast. 

159 

60 

7.0 

1.0 

0.5 

100 

33.9 

24.6 


Emiss. 

159 

65 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

14.4 

4.6 


Pet. 

159 

19 

1.0 

0.3 

0.1 

40 

4.8 

0.6 


Interc. 

137 

88 

4.0 

2.1 

1.2 

100 

67.5 

74.4 


Prost. 

159 

72 

5.5 

0.6 

0.4 

100 

30.2 

20.8 


Homo. 

159 

28 

5.5 

1.4 

0.5 

100 

29.9 j 

20.0 

61. Urban 

Total 

88 

99 

16.0 

3.1 

2.1 








Mast. 

88 

65 

7.0 

1.0 

0.5 

100 

29.3 

21 0 


Emiss. 

88 

64 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

60 

12.0 

3.8 


Pet. 

88 

17 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

3.2 

0.6 


Interc. 

92 

90 

13.0 

2.1 

1.5 

100 

67.8 

73.3 


Prost. 

88 

75 

4.0 

0.8 

0.4 

100 

27.7 

21.1 


Homo. 

88 

35 

4.5 

1.0 

0.4 

100 

25.5 

12.5 

62. Protest., inactive 

Total 

77 

100 

13.0 

2.7 

2.1 








Mast. 

77 

68 

1 3.5 

0.8 

0.4 

100 

30.7 

21.9 


Emiss. 

77 

70 

1.5 

0.3 

0.1 

95 

14.3 

5.4 


Pet. 

77 

20 

1.0 

0.4 

0.1 

40 

6.7 

0.7 


Interc. 

80 

93 

13.0 

1.9 

1.1 

100 

67.8 

75.8 


Prost. 

77 

75 

3.0 

0.6 

0.3 

100 

28.7 

20.3 


Homo. 

77 

19 

3.5 

1.3 

j 0.6 

75 

1 29.8 

25.0 

Educ. level 9-12 








l 


63. Whole group 

Total 

117 

99 

14.0 

2.9 

2.1 

— 

— 

— 

Mast. 

117 

79 

5.0 

1.0 

0.5 

100 

37.8 

36.9 


Emiss. 

117 

70 

2.0 

0.3 

0.2 

85 

16.9 

8.2 


Pet. 

117 

26 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

45 

7.9 

0.7 


Interc. 

95 

72 

11.0 

1.6 

0.9 

100 

51.7 

51.3 


Prost. 

117 

43 

2.0 

0.4 

0.2 

80 

24.2 

8.0 


Homo. 

117 

46 

11.0 

1.6 

0.7 

95 

36.9 

36.7 


702 



CLINICAL TABLES : SINGLE 


703 


SINGLE WHITE M AT PS 

57. Age 21-25. Educ. level 13+, Cath., inactive. One of less restrained college 
groups. Incidence of coitus half again as high (75%) as in whole college group; 
prostitutes in 27%. Nevertheless, the restraint of whole college group (Group 51) 
is still evident, and masturbation provides largest portion of outlet for 82%, 
Homosexual provides nearly 3^^ of outlet for 15% of males. 

58. Age 21-25. Educ. level 13+. Jewish, inactive. Highest total outlet (3.1) 
among college groups (Group 51). definitely less restrained than average college 
male in socio-sexual activities. Masturbation still provides 54% of outlet for 
86%, petting in 69% of histories. Intercourse in 67%, provides 22% of outlet 
therefrom. Prostitutes in 24%, but very minor outlet; homosexual in a few. 

59. Age 26-30. Whole group. Shows some effect of aging, mean total outlet 
averaging only 2.7. Males of grade and high school levels who are unmarried by 
this age begin to be a select group. Some are prevented from marrying by un- 
toward, circumstances, some are actively and promiscuously heterosexual and dis- 
inclined to settle down to a single partner in marriage, and to nearly 3^ have 
homosexual histories, a fair number of which are now so pronounced as to inter- 
fere with marriage. Clinician might well determine reason for delay in marriage 
in each case. College level males more often unmarried because of custom of 
group or because of demands of an extended education, less often because of 
sexual situations. Patterns now well fixed, not liable to re-direction. 

60. Age 26-30. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Least inhibited of social levels. 
Nearly all actively involved in intercourse (88%), prostitutes increasingly im- 
portant (in 72%) because easier to secure. 74% of outlet from intercourse, prac- 
tically unaffected by moral arguments. Masturbation quite taboo, but still in 
60% for whom it supplies 25% of outlet. Homosexual in 28%, for many of whom 
it constitutes a material source and sometimes the sole source of outlet. 

61. Age 26-30. Educ. level 0-8. Urban. The city-bred portion of this lower edu- 
cational level is almost exactly as described for whole group above (Group 60), 
except that even a larger number (35%) has homosexual which, however, is not 
an important outlet. Quite unrestrained, often carefree and promiscuous. 

62. Age 26-30. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. Pattern close to that of whole 
lower level at this age (Group 60). Quite unrestrained, with 93% having inter- 
course. Apparently fewer males (19%) involved in homosexual. 

63. Age 26-30. Educ. level 9-12. Whole group. Single males of this level quite 
unrestrained in late twenties. Masturbation 79%, emissions 70%, petting in 26%, 
intercourse in 72% of whom 43% have some with prostitutes. Incidence of homo- 
sexual apparently higher (46%) than in any other segment of entire male popu- 
lation, because many heterosexual males have left the group for marriage. Some 
of most active homosexual cases here, sometimes upset, over-react to social 
pressures, this and previous age group including street-walking, tavern-frequent- 
ing, exhibitionistic city groups. 



Table 152. Clinical Tables (continued) 
SINGLE WHITE MALES 




SOURCE 

CASES 

INCID. 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 











AGE 

: 26-30 

OUTLET 


/o 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 9-12 










64. 

Urban 

Total 

73 

97 

14.0 

3.1 

2.3 

— 

— 

— 



Mast. 

73 

81 

4.5 

1.0 

0.5 

100 

40.3 

41.3 



Emiss. 

73 

70 

1.5 

0.3 

0.2 

85 

15.0 

7.9 



Pet. 

73 

25 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

45 

8.3 

0.7 



Interc. 

77 

68 

9.0 

1.4 

0.8 

100 

48.1 

42.5 



Prost. 

73 

34 

2.0 

0.5 

0.3 

80 

26.3 

10.0 



Homo. 

73 

52 

11.0 

1.8 

0.8 

95 

38.3 

37.5 

65. 

Protest., inactive 

Total 

51 

98 

12.0 

3.1 

2.3 









Mast. 

51 

75 

3.0 

0.9 

0.6 

100 

36.4 

38.8 

' 


Emiss. 

51 

73 

2.0 

0.4 

0.3 

75 

18.2 

11.0 



Pet. 

51 

18 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

15 

4.7 

0.8 



Interc. 

53 

72 

9.0 

1.6 

0.9 

100 

50.9 

51.3 



Prost. 

51 

39 

1.0 

0.3 

0.2 

80 

19.8 

6.9 



Homo. 

51 

49 

5.5 

1.7 

0.9 

85 

38.9 

42.5 

Educ. level 13+ 










66. 

Whole group 

Total 

487 

100 

17.0 

2.6 

1.8 

— 

— 

— 



Mast. 

487 

83 

10.0 

1.4 

0.7 

100 

53.9 

55.8 



Emiss. 

487 

85 

4.0 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

26.7 

13.3 



Pet. 

487 

45 

3.0 

0.3 

0.1 

90 

14.2 

2.6 



Interc. 

373 

56 

15.0 

1.1 

0.5 

100 

37.6 

28.4 



Prost. 

487 

16 

6.0 

0.5 

0.1 

25 

4.5 

0.9 



Homo. 

487 

17 

5.0 

1.3 

0.7 

100 

41.3 

37.5 



Anim. 

487 

1 

Scant 

0.4 

0.6 

Scant 

6.8 

8.0 

67. 

Urban 

Total 

479 

100 

17.0 

2.7 

1.9 



__ 




Mast. 

479 

86 

10.0 

1.4 

0.7 

100 

51.6 

51.4 



Emiss. 

479 

85 

4.0 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

26.2 

13.0 



Pet. 

445 

47 

3.0 

0.3 

0.1 

90 

15.7 

4.1 



Interc. 

308 

59 

15.0 

1.2 

0.5 

100 

36.6 

26.7 



Prost. 

445 

19 

6.0 

0.5 

0.1 

25 

4.6 

0.9 



Homo. 

479 

17 

5.0 

1.4 

0.6 

100 

43.4 

46.3 

68. 

Rural 

Total 

85 

99 

8.0 

2.2 

1.5 







Mast. 

85 

79 

7.0 

1.4 

0.9 

100 

63.1 

75.0 



Emiss. 

85 

86 

1.0 

0.3 

0.3 

100 

30.6 

, 14.7 



Pet. 

85 

43 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

35 

7.1 

! 0.8 



Interc. 

58 

45 

5.0 

1.1 

0.5 

100 

43.5 

38.8 



Prost. 

85 

7 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

0.5 

1.0 



Homo. 

85 

15 

1.5 

0.7 

0.4 

35 

25.1 

10.0 

69. 

Protest., active 

Total 

100 

100 

7.0 

1.9 

1.3 






Mast. 

100 

82 

5.5 

1.2 

0.7 

1 100 

59.5 

62.5 



Emiss. 

100 

95 

2.5 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

37.1 

30.7 



Pet. 

100 

42 

2.5 

0.4 

0.1 

75 

14.4 

1.5 



Interc. 

88 

35 

2.5 

0.5 

0.3 

90 

24.5 

16.3 



Prost. 

100 

9 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

10 

3.4 

0.8 



Homo. 

100 

10 

3,5 

1.1 

0.4 

30 

23.3 

14.2 

70. 

Protest., inactive 

Total 

279 

100 

16.0 

2.6 

1.8 






Mast. 

279 

88 

9.0 

1.5 

0.8 

100 

57.0 

62.0 



Emiss. 

279 

85 

1.5 

0.3 

0.2 

100 

23.8 

10.7 



Pet. 

279 

44 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

75 

12.3 

0.9 



Interc. 

187 

60 

3.0 

0.7 

0.3 

100 

28.9 

18.6 



Prost. 

279 

15 

5.5 

0.4 

0.1 

15 

3.5 

0.9 



Homo. 

279 

21 

5.0 

1.1 

0.5 

100 

36.4 

29.2 


704 



CLINICAL tables: SINGLE 


705 


SINGLE WHITE MALES 


64. Age 26-30. Educ. level 9-12. Urban. Typical of whole high school level 
(Group 63). Heterosexual slightly lower, homosexual rather higher. Intercourse 
in of cases accounting for 43% of outlet, half as many cases with some pros- 
titute contacts. Masturbation in 81% accounting for 41% of their outlet. Homo- 
sexual in more than half, accounting for more than of their outlet. 


65. Age 26-30. Educ. level 9-12. Protest., inactive. Pattern very close to that 
described for whole high school level (Group 63). 

66. Age 26-30. Educ. level 13+. Whole group. Even in late twenties males of 
college level are remarkably restrained. Restrained individuals increase with ad- 
vancing age because more social individuals have already married. Many highly 
erotic, nearly half (45%) involved in heterosexual petting, most with fantasies 
during masturbation and in nocturnal dreams, but hardly more than half (56%) 
yet attempting overt intercourse, and then not often, and with few partners. 
Much disturbed by blockage of sexual response, and those not having intercourse 
may need special consideration from clinician. Masturbation accepted, providing 
56% of outlet. Petting of social value, but provides very minor outlet. Homo- 
sexual (17%) is much lower than in high school group, but sometimes serious 
disturbance over it. Nudity accepted, oral erotic techniques now in 57%. Pros- 
titutes largely avoided, providing less than 1% of outlet. No particular VD 
problem. 

67. Age 26-30. Educ. level 13+. Urban. Pattern for this city-bred group almost 
exactly as described for whole college level at this age (Group 66). 

68. Age 26-30. Educ, level 13+. Rural. A somewhat less active portion of col- 
lege level (Group 66), differing from corresponding urban population (Group 67) 
in having lower incidences in most of outlets. 79% derive of total outlet from 
masturbation, 45% derive a little more than % from intercourse. Other outlets 
more minor. Animal intercourse now very rare in rural histories from Eastern 
part of U. S., not uncommon in histories of this and older ages from Western 
areas. 

69. Age 26-30. Educ. level 13+. Protest., active. Most restrained segment of 
college level in late twenties (Group 66). Mean total outlet only % as high as aver- 
age for whole college group. Incidences lower on every outlet except nocturnal 
emissions, 82% deriving 63% of outlet from masturbation, 95% with emissions 
giving 31% of outlet. Coitus in only of group. Homosexual in 10%, which is 
lowest among all groups at this age. Clinician may expect sexual conflicts here. 

70. Age 26-30. Educ. level 13+. Protest., inactive. Not materially different 
from average for whole college level of this age (Group 66), 



Table 152. Clinical Tables {continued) 
SINGLE WHITE MALES 


FREQ. PER WEEK 


% OF TOTAL OUTLEf 


AGE: 26-30 
Educ. level 13+ 


AGE: 31-35 
72. Whole group 


Educ. level 0-8 

73. Whole group 


Educ. level 13+ 

Whole group 


5. Urban 


OUTLET 


/o 

Range 

Mean 

Total 

104 

100 

15.0 

3.2 

Mast. 

104 

80 

6 0 

1.3 

Emiss. 

104 

75 

4.0 

0.4 

Pet. 

104 

57 

1.5 

0.3 

Interc. 

77 

84 

15.0 

2.0 

Prost. 

104 

28 

2.0 

0.8 

Homo. 

104 

8 

3.0 

1.4 

Total 

195 

99 

14 0 

2.4 

Mast. 

195 

71 

7.0 

1.0 

Emiss. 

195 

71 

1.5 

0.3 

Pet. 

195 

24 

1.0 

0.3 

Interc. 

223 

68 

13.0 

1.5 

Prost. 

195 

41 

3.0 

0.6 

Homo. 

195 

30 

4.5 

1.7 

Total 

67 

99 

9.5 

2 8 

Mast. 

67 

70 

4.0 

0.9 

Emiss. 

67 

61 

1.5 

0.3 

Pet, 

67 

8 

0 5 

0.3 

Interc. 

70 

84 

7.0 

1 4 

Prost. 

67 

66 

3.0 

0.8 

Homo. 

67 

30 I 

4.0 

2.5 


6. Protest., inactive Total 


Total 

87 

99 

Mast. 

1 87 

77 

Emiss. 

87 

83 

Pet, 

87 

32 

Interc, 

107 

58 

Prost, 

87 

16 

Homo. 

87 

26 

Total 

72 

100 

Mast. 

72 

78 

Emiss. 

72 

86 

Pet. 

72 

32 

Interc. 

89 

61 

Prost. 

72 

17 

Homo. 

72 

28 

Total 

53 

98 



Range 

Mean 

Med 

100 

39.9 

31.7 

100 

16.3 

8.8 

35 

10.0 

3.8 

100 

53.6 

56.7 

45 

11.4 

1.0 

100 

53 S 

65.0 

100 

40.4 

33.3 

100 

21.7 

8.3 

80 

13.7 

1.0 

100 

58.5 

63.0 

100 

28.7 

20.7 

100 

46,9 

50.0 

90 

30.6 

20.0 

100 

17.0 

6.7 

5 

2.5 

1.0 

100 

61.4 

70.0 

100 

37.8 

31.3 

100 

48.3 

37.5 

100 

51.0 

48.8 

100 

22.8 

8.6 

80 

17.7 

9.2 

100 

49.9 

46.3 

15 

4.1 

0.9 

100 

48.5 

56.7 

100 

48.6 

47.5 

100 

23.2 

9.1 

50 

16.4 

10.0 

100 

51.4 

48.8 

15 

4.7 

1.7 

90 

50.4 

54.2 

100 

49.5 

40.0 

m 

20.4 

12.9 

7.5 

3.0 

100 

15 

40.6 

A 7 

35.0 

1 7 

85 

52.7 

1 . / 
60.0 



CLINICAL tables: SINGLE 


707 


SINGLE WHITE MALES 

71. Age 26-30. Educ. level 13+. Jewish, inactive. Socio-sexually most active 
of all college groups (Group 66). Many Jewish males are slow in starting sexual 
activity and inhibited during their earher years (Groups 21, 42); but those who 
are unmarried in late twenties become distinctly freer. Clinician will do well to 
examine possibility of maladjustment due to change of pattern. 84% now have 
intercourse, which accounts for 57% of outlet. Only 3 .^ of these have any inter- 
course with prostitutes. Masturbation still accounts for ^ of outlet for 80%. 
Homosexual incidence low. 

72. Age 31-35. Whole group. Males who are unmarried by this age often have 
special problems in socio-sexual adjustment. Only % are having intercourse, de- 
riving 2 ,^ of total outlet therefrom, of these depend in part on prostitutes — a 
further indication of difficulty in making socio-sexual advances. This is par- 
ticularly true at lower educational levels. Masturbation still in ^ of histories, ac- 
counting for of outlet of those males. Homosexual in nearly of histories, ac- 
counting for ^ of their outlet, many individuals primarily or exclusively homo- 
sexual by this age. Rates of outlet lowered by advancing age. A fair number of 
individuals derive their total outlet now from a single source. 

73. Age 31-35. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Similar to whole age group de- 
scribed above (Group 72). More intercourse, especially with prostitutes. 84% 
draw 70% of outlet from intercourse, but 66% get of outlet from intercourse 
with prostitutes. Petting nearly absent. Many lower level males have increasing 
difficulty in finding sexual partners as they grow older. Some persons physically 
handicapped, mentally dull, and physically unattractive, and a satisfactory ad- 
justment with prostitutes is about all that can be hoped for. Masturbation in 
70% but not really accepted by group. Often with strong taboos against nudity 
and variety of techniques. of group with some homosexual history, many ex- 
clusively homosexual, this not leading to conflict as often as in upper social, 
levels. 

74. Age 31-35. Educ. level 13+. Whole group. As described for whole age 
group above (Group 72), except for a somewhat higher dependence on masturba- 
tion and emissions, and a lower utilization of intercourse. Contacts with pros- 
titutes rare. These males still in conflict over moral aspects of socio-sexual con- 
tacts. have never yet had intercourse, and little possibility of most of them ever 
developing facility in securing such contacts. Only a portion is homosexual. 

75. Age 31-35. Educ. level 13+. Urban. Almost exactly as described for whole 
college-bred portion of this age group (Group 74). 

76. Age 31-35. Educ. level 13 +. Protest., inactive. Almost exactly as described 
for whole college-bred portion of this age group (Group 74). 



Table 152. Clinical Tables {concluded) 
SINGLE WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 



FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 




INCID. 








OF 

cases 

1 V 








OUTLET 


1 /o 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

AGE: 36-40 



1 







77. Whole group 

Total 

97 

98 

9.0 

2.1 

1.4 








Mast. 

97 

63 

7.0 

1.2 

0.6 

100 

47.1 

45.0 


Emiss. 

97 

60 

1.5 

0.2 

0.1 

100 

19.1 

6.4 


Pet. 

97 

11 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

35 

13.0 

1.0 


Interc. 

no 

70 

8.5 

1.3 

0.6 

100 

57.7 

60.0 


Prost. 

97 

49 

2.5 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

39.5 

35.0 

AGE: 41-45 

Homo. 

97 

40 

4.0 

1.1 

0.7 

100 

42.2 

37.5 

78. Whole group 

Total 

56 ' 

96 

7.5 

1.9 

1.1 







Mast. 

56 

61 

7.0 

1.0 

0.4 

100 

43.7 

32.5 


Emiss. 

56 

48 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

100 

19.5 

8.3 


Pet. 

56 

11 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

10 

1\1 

0.9 


Interc. 

61 

66 

6.5 

1.1 

0.5 

100 

66.6 

73.8 


Prost. 

56 

39 

1.5 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

48.2 

47.5 


Homo. 

56 j 

38 

5.0 

1.2 

0.5 

100 

42.2 

35.0 


708 



CLINICAL tables: SINGLE 


709 


SINGLE WHITE MALES 

77. Age 36-40. Whole group. Frequencies of total outlet have been declining 
since late teens, now average 2.1. Males unmarried by this age include some who 
are heterosexuaily very active but unwilling to settle down with a single partner; 
some who are timid or otherwise handicapped for making socio-sexual contacts, 
and some who are primarily or exclusively homosexual. Group rather too old to 
modify patterns, and clinician’s function primarily one of helping individual ac- 
cept whatever pattern he now has. Occasionally marriage at this or later age is 
successful, usually not. 

78. Age 41-45. Whole group. A continuation of all trends described for pre- 
vious age group (Group 77). Remarks made there apply in even greater force to 
this older group. 



Table 153. Clinical Tables 
MARRIED WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 

OF 

OUTLET 

CASES 

INCID. 

FREQ. PER WEEK. 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 

AGE: 16-20 

% 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

79. Whole group 

Total 

272 

100 

28.0 

4.7 

3.2 

_ 




Mast. 

272 

39 

3.0 

0.4 

0.1 

65 

9.9 

2.3 


Emiss. 

272 

53 

2.5 

0.3 

0.1 

65 

5 6 

1.3 


Interc. 

272 

100 

25.0 

3.8 

2.6 

100 

84.6 

95.3 


Extra. 

272 

38 

7.5 

1.2 

0.3 

75 

18.0 

9.7 


Prost. 

272 

14 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

15 

2.9 

0.7 


Homo. 

272 

9 

6.0 

1.2 

0.3 

30 

9.2 

4.1 

Educ. level 0-8 










80. Whole group 

Total 

158 

100 

29.0 

4.7 

3.1 








Mast. 

158 

29 

2.5 

0.4 

0.1 

50 

10.1 

2.6 


Emiss. 

158 

48 

2.5 

0.3 

0.1 

50 

5.3 

1.0 


Interc. 

158 

100 

28.0 

3.7 

2.5 

100 

83.9 

95.3 


Extra. 

158 

44 

7.0 

1.2 

0.5 

60 

18.4 

12.5 


Prost. 

158 

17 

0.5 

0.2 

0 1 

10 

2.1 

0.8 


Homo. 

158 

10 

4.5 

1.4 

0.4 

30 

10.1 

4.5 

81. Urban 

Total 

97 

100 

20.0 

4.7 

3.0 





Mast. 

97 

32 

2.0 

0.4 

0.1 

50 

9.5 

2.1 


Emiss. 

97 

54 

2.5 

0.4 

0.1 

50 

6.4 

1.2 


Interc. 

97 

100 

18.0 

3.5 

2.3 

100 

81.5 

95.1 


Extra. 

97 

47 

7.0 

1.4 

0.5 

60 

20.6 

14.2 


Prost. 

97 

19 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

2.4 

0.8 


Homo. 

97 

12 

4.0 

1.5 

0.4 

30 

11.5 

7.5 

82. Protest., inactive 

Total 

96 

100 

24 0 

4.7 

3.0 





Mast. 

96 

31 

2.5 

0.5 

0.2 

50 

10.8 

2.4 


Emiss. 

96 

53 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

40 

4.4 

0.9 


Interc. 

96 

100 

18.0 

3.8 

2.4 

100 

84.5 

95.6 


Extra. 

96 

44 

4.5 

1.2 

0.5 

60 

18 2 

12.5 


Prost. 

96 

17 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

2.5 

0.8 


Homo. 

96 

7 

4.0 

1.4 

0.1 

30 

9.8 

3.0 

Educ. level 9-12 










83. Whole group 

Total 

87 

100 

24.0 

5.1 

3.6 






Mast. 

87 

39 

3.0 

0.4 

0.1 

50 

7.1 

1.0 


Emiss. 

87 

54 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

5.0 

1.8 


Interc. 

87 

100 

17.0 

4.1 

2 8 

100 

85.9 

95.9 


Extra. 

87 

38 

6.0 

1.4 

0.2 

60 

18.6 

10.0 


Prost. 

87 

16 

2.0 

0.5 

0.1 

10 

4.4 

0.8 


Homo. 

87 

9 

1.0 

1.1 

0.4 

15 

7.1 

3.8 

84. Urban 

Total 

61 

100 

20.0 

4.7 

3.6 





Mast. 

61 

39 

3.0 

0.4 

0.1 

50 

7.4 

1.3 


Emiss. 

61 

57 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

5.3 

1 .0 


Interc. 

61 

100 

16.0 

3.6 

2.8 

100 

83.3 

95.5 


Extra. 

61 

43 

6,0 

1.6 

0.4 

60 

21.1 

15.8 


Prost. 

61 

20 

0.5 

0.4 

0.1 

5 

4.5 

0.7 


Homo. 

61 

11 

1.0 

1.3 

0.4 

15 

7.6 

4.0 

85, Protest., inactive 

Total 

60 

100 

20.0 

4.5 

3.3 





Mast. 

60 

35 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

3.1 

0.9 


Emiss. 

60 

52 

0.5 

0 2 

0.1 

20 

4.1 

1 .4 


Interc. 

60 

100 

16.0 

3.8 

2.8 

100 

88.5 

96.4 


Extra. 

60 

40 

6.0 

1.3 

0.1 

60 

17.9 

4.0 


Prost. 

60 

12 

0.5 

0.6 

0.1 

5 

7.3 

1.0 

3.0 


Homo. 

60 

5 

0.5 

0.2 

0.3 

5 

2.2 


710 




CLINICAL TABLES : MARRIED 


711 


Table 153. Clinical Tables 

married white males 

79e Age 16-20. Whole group. Males married at this age have maximum sexual 
performance of any group in total population. Mean total outlet 4.7, frequencies 
for many individuals going to 10, 14, or even 20 per week, with no ill effects. 
95% of outlet from marital intercourse, more in high school level; not enough 
males of college level married at this age to warrant generalizations, but some 
proportion of grade school males marrying before twenty. 

80. Age 16-20. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. 95% of total outlet of group de- 
rived from marital intercourse, but nearly of lower level males have extra- 
marital intercourse which accounts for 13% of their outlet. Frequencies of all 
other outlets low, although a fair number involved in masturbation, emissions, 
and the homosexual, even while living with wives. Group often irresponsible, 
many quickly deserting wives, some not self-supporting, becoming drifters, 
sometimes involved in petty underworld activities. Many promiscuous in extra- 
marital relations, VD rates rather high. 

81. Age 16-20. Educ. level 0-8. Urban. Almost precisely as described for the 
whole lower educational level (Group 80). 

82. Age 16-20. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. Almost precisely as de- 
scribed for the whole lower educational level (Group 80). 

83. Age 16-20. Educ. level 9-12. Whole group. Remarkably close to grade 
school group of same age (Group 80). 96% of total outlet of whole group from 
maritarintercourse, 38% get 10% from extra-marital intercourse, 9% get 4% 
from homosexual. More acceptant of extra-marital intercourse at this age than 
later. All other outlets very minor. Group probably more reliable, more balanced 
than corresponding grade school group. Considerable diversity in mental 
ability and educational background, similar diversity in regard to religious re- 
straint and sexual inhibition. Devoutly religious persons often traditional in 
respect to sexual mores, but group as a whole nowhere near as restrained as 
college level. 

84. Age 16-20. Educ. level 9-12. Urban. Almost precisely as described for 
whole high school level (Group 83). 43% having extra-marital intercourse, which 
accounts for 16% of total outlet, and some marital discord may grow out of this. 
Extra-marital relations for this group are reduced in later age periods. 

85. Age 16-20. Educ. level 9-12. Protest., inactive. Not materially different 
from whole high school level as described above (Group 83). Not closely enough 
connected with church backgrounds to have behavior materially affected, unless 
in regard to extra-marital intercourse which accounts for only 4% of total outlet 
of 40% of males. 



Table 153. Clinical Tables (continued) 
MARRIED WHITE MALES 


AGE; 21-25 
86. Whole group 


Educ. level 0-8 
87. Whole group 


88. Urban 


89. Rural 


90. Protest., inactive 


Educ. level 9-12 

91. Whole group 


92. Urban 


SOURCE 

OF 

OUTLET 

CASES 

Total 

751 

Mast. 

751 

Emiss. 

751 

Interc. 

751 

Extra. 

751 

Prost. 

751 

Homo. 

751 

Total 

324 

Mast. 

324 

Emiss. 

324 

Interc. 

324 

Extra. 

324 

Prost. 

324 

Homo. 

324 

Total 

162 

Mast. 

162 

Emiss. 

162 

Interc. 

162 

Extra. 

162 

Prost. 

162 

Homo. 

162 

Total 

128 

Mast. 

128 

Emiss. 

128 

Interc. 

128 

Extra. 

128 

Prost. 

128 

Homo. 

1 128 

Total 

206 

Mast 

206 

Emiss. 

206 

Interc. 

206 

Extra. 

206 

Prost. 

206 

Homo. 

206 

Total 

164 

Mast 

164 

Emiss. 

164 

Interc. 

164 

Extra. 

164 

Prost. 

164 

Homo. 

164 

Total 

107 

Mast. 

107 

Emiss. 

107 

Interc. 

107 

Extra. 

107 

Prost. 

107 

Homo. 

107 


FREQ. PER WEEK 


% OF TOTAL OUTLET 


INCID. 

% 


100 

48 

59 

100 

27 

13 

8 


100 

29 

45 

99 

34 

15 

9 


100 

33 

48 

98 

41 

20 

14 


100 

25 

40 

100 

25 

9 

6 


100 

30 

47 

100 

35 

16 

10 


100 

42 

63 

100 

42 

25 

13 


100 

41 

64 

100 

46 

29 

16 


Range 


29.0 

4.0 

4.0 

29.0 

18.0 

2.0 
3.0 


29.0 

3.0 

2.0 

28.0 
18.0 

2.0 

3.5 


29.0 

3.0 

2.0 
20 0 

18.0 
1.0 
3.5 


17.0 
1.0 
1.5 

16 0 

2.0 
0.5 
0.5 


29.0 

3.0 

2.0 

17.0 

18.0 
1.0 
1.0 


26.0 

3.0 

1.0 
25.0 

7.0 

2.0 
2.0 


20.0 

3.0 

1.0 
15.0 

7.0 

2.0 

0.5 


Mean 


3.9 

0.5 

0.2 

3.2 

1.2 
0.2 
0.4 


4.0 

0.3 

0.3 

3.3 

1.5 

0.2 

0.6 


4.4 
0.4 
0.3 

3.5 
1.8 
0.2 
0.6 


3.4 

0.3 

0.2 

3.0 

0.6 

0.2 

0.4 


4.2 
0.4 
0.2 

3.3 
1.8 
0.2 
0.4 


4.2 

0.4 

0.2 

3.4 

1.1 

0.3 

0.3 


4.1 
0.4 
0.2 

3.1 
1.3 
0.2 
0.2 


Med. 


2.8 

0.2 

0.1 

2.3 

0.3 

0.1 

0.1 


2.6 

0.1 

0.1 

2.2 

0.3 

0.1 

0.1 


2.9 

0.1 

0.1 

2.4 

0.3 

0.1 

0.1 


2.3 

0.1 

0.1 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

0.1 


2.7 

0.1 

0.1 

2.3 

0.4 

0.1 

0.1 


2.9 

0.1 

0.1 

2.5 

0.3 

0.1 

0.1 


3.0 

0.1 

0.1 

2.6 

0.4 

0.1 

0.1 


Range 


90 

70 

100 

95 

50 

50 


75 

45 

100 

90 

40 

25 


50 

45 

100 

90 

30 

25 


40 

20 

100 

40 

5 

5 


50 

35 

100 

75 

40 

10 


75 

30 

100 

70 

30 

15 


70 

30 

100 

70 

30 

15 


Mean 


12.2 

6.5 
85.3 
16.0 

4.0 

7.6 


8.5 
5.3 

87.7 

19.5 

4.5 
3. '9 


7.9 

5.8 

86.4 

20.5 
4.5 

4.9 


8.7 

3.7 
90.9 
15.5 

5.5 

1.3 


8.4 

4.4 
87.8 
19.7 

4.7 

3.9 


7.6 

4.8 
85.7 
16.0 

4.4 

3.8 


8.6 

5.0 

83.4 

19.2 

4.6 

3.6 


Med. 


4.0 
1.7 
95 2 
5.4 
0.8 
1 4 


2.4 

1.0 

96.0 

11.9 

0.9 

1.0 


2 7 
1 0 
95.8 
10.0 
0.9 
2 2 


2.7 

0.9 

96.5 

13.3 

1.0 

0.9 


2.0 

1.0 

96.0 

15.6 

0.9 

2.0 


1.0 

1.0 

95.2 

5.5 

0.7 

1.0 


1.0 

1.0 

94.1 

10.0 

0.7 

0.9 


712 



CLINICAL tables: MARRIED 


713 


married white males 

86. Age 21-25. Whole group. A very high proportion of males at this age are 
now married except in college level. Mean total outlet only 3.9, which is defi- 
nitely below mean for males who are married in late teens. Extra-marital inter- 
course in accounting for 5% of outlet, homosexual in 8%, masturbation in 48%, 
but niost masturbation among males of college level. Marital intercourse ac- 
counting for 95% of total outlet. All other outlets minor except for extra-marital 
intercourse in certain groups. 

87. Age 21-25. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Mean total outlet 4.0. Most from 
marital intercourse, frequencies commonly 7 and in many cases 14 and 20 per 
week. Intercourse with minimum of pre-coital petting, entire relation usually 
completed in 2 to 5 minutes. Males give little consideration to female orgasm, 
but females in actuality more often reaching orgasm and sexual relation satis- 
factory for them more often than in college level. Marital discord rarely originat- 
ing from unsatisfactory sexual relations, but often resulting from extra-marital 
intercourse which involves of group and supplies 12% of their outlet. Drinking, 
non-support, physical cruelty to wife are most common sources of manital diffi- 
culty. Masturbation, emissions, and homosexual in limited number of histories 
and supplying only minor portion of outlet. 

88. Age 21-25. Educ. level 0-8. Urban. Much as described for whole grade 
school level (Group 87). City-bred males are, however, somewhat more active 
sexually, with mean total outlet 4.4. 41% with extra-marital intercourse, 14% 
with homosexual. Often considerable irresponsibility in marriage. 

89. Age 21-25. Educ. level 0-8, Rural. General pattern as described above for 
whole grade school level (Group 87), but definitely less active. Not a product of 
restraint as much as of less developed social interest and facility, even with wife. 
Mean rate of marital intercourse 3.0, and rates of 7 and more much less frequent. 
Marital intercourse supplying 97% of outlet. Extra-marital intercourse in only 
i' 4 , supplying 13% of their outlet. Masturbation, emissions, and homosexual in 
fewer instances than in city group. 

90. Age 21-25. Educ. level 0-8. Protest, inactive. Closely matching descrip- 
tion given for whole grade school level (Group 87). Less active than urban group 
(Group 88), with fewer persons involved in extra-marital intercourse or homo- 
sexual. Religious backgrounds too remote to have much direct influence. 

91. Age 21-25. Educ. level 9-12. Whole group. Most active group of males of 
this age, with mean total outlet 4.2, with high frequencies of marital intercourse 
in individual cases, and with highest incidences of extra-marital intercourse and 
homosexual experience after marriage. 95% of outlet from marital intercourse, 
all other outlets minor, but masturbation, emissions, extra-marital intercourse, 
experience with prostitutes, and homosexual in a considerable portion of all 
histories. 

92. Age 21-25. Educ. level 9-12. Urban. A rather active group, with general 
pattern as described above for whole high school level (Group 91), nearly with 
extra-marital intercourse, which accounts for 10% of their outlet. One in six with 
homosexual activity, averaging one in two months. Masturbation and emissions 
with about same frequency as homosexual but with higher incidences. 



Table 153. Clinical Tables {continued) 
MARRIED WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 

OF 

OUTLET 

cases 

INCID- 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 

AGE: 21-25 

% 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 9-12 










93. Protest., inactive 

Total 

108 

100 

21.0 

3.8 

2.7 

— 

— 

— 


Mast. 

108 

41 

3.0 

0.4 

0.1 

75 

7.6 

0.9 


Emiss. 

108 

63 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

25 

4.5 

0.9 


Interc. 

108 

100 

20.0 

3.1 

2.3 

100 

85.1 

95.3 


Extra. 

108 

47 

7.0 

1.0 

0.3 

70 

16.9 

5.0 


Prost. 

108 

29 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

4.5 

0.7 


Homo. 

108 

12 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

2.8 

1.0 

Educ. level 13 -f- 










94. Whole group 

Total 

440 

100 

22.0 

3.7 

3.1 

— 

— 

— 

Mast. 

440 

66 

4.0 

0.5 

0.2 

85 

14.9 

5.8 


Emiss. 

440 

66 

4.0 

0.3 

0.1 

70 

7.9 

2.7 


Interc. 

440 

100 

20.0 

3 1 

2.5 

100 

82.9 

91.9 


Extra. 

440 

15 

3.0 

0.5 

0.1 

50 

8.4 

2.4 


Prost. 

440 

5 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

2.3 

0.7 


Homo. 

440 

3 

2.0 

0.7 

0.6 

50 

24 .-8 

10.0 

95. Urban 

Total 

460 

100 

22.0 

3.7 

3.1 

— 





Mast. 

428 

67 

4.0 

0.5 

0.2 

85 

15.1 

6.3 


Emiss 

460 

66 

4.0 

0.3 

0.1 

70 

8.3 

3.0 


Interc. 

460 

100 

20.0 

3.1 

2.6 

100 

82.5 

91.2 


Extra. 

460 

16 

3.0 

0 5 

0.1 

50 

8 7 

2.6 


Prost. 

428 

5 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

2.4 

0.8 


Homo. 

428 

3 

2.0 

0.6 

0.5 

50 

24.8 

10 0 

96. Protest., active 

Total 

91 

100 

15.0 

3.3 

2.7 

_ 




Mast. 

91 

60 

2.0 

0.4 

0.2 

70 

14.4 

4.9 


Emiss. 

91 

67 

1.5 

0.3 

0.1 

25 ) 

6.5 

2.7 


Interc. 

91 

99 

11.0 

2.8 

2.2 

100 

86.7 

95.3 


Extra. 

91 

8 

0.5 

0.5 

0.1 

5 

7.2 

0.9 

97. Protest., inactive 

Total 

280 

100 

22.0 

3.9 

3.2 

__ 




Mast. 

280 

70 

4.0 

0.5 

0 2 

70 

13.9 

6.0 


Emiss. 

280 

68 

4.0 

0.3 

0.1 

70 

8.5 

2.7 


Interc. 

280 

100 

20.0 

3.2 

2.5 

100 

! 82.1 

90.3 


Extra. 

280 

18 

2.5 

0.5 

0.2 

30 

! 7.9 

3.3 


Prost. 

280 

5 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

10 

' 3.3 

0.8 


Homo. 

280 

3 

2.0 

0.7 

0.7 

50 

34.3 

30.0 

AGE: 26-30 

98. Whole group 

Total 

737 

100 

29.0 

3.3 

2.5 




Mast. 

737 

48 

4.0 

0.4 

0.1 

90 

12.5 

4.2 


Emiss. 

737 

63 

3 0 1 

0.2 

0.1 

65 

6.5 

1.8 


Interc. 

737 

100 

25.0 1 

2.7 

2.0 

100 

85.5 

95.0 


Extra. 

737 

29 

6.0 

0.6 

0.1 

75 

12.2 

4.2 


Prost. 

737 

12 

2,0 

0.2 

0.1 

40 

5.0 

0.8 


Homo. 

737 

5 

2.0 

0.5 

0.1 

50 

11.4 

2.8 

Educ. level 0-8 










99. Whole group 

Total 

292 

99 

29,0 

3.5 

2.4 








Mast. 

292 

21 

•4.0 

0.4 

0.1 

70 

7.7 

0.8 


Emiss. 

292 

47 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

50 

6.2 

1.0 


Interc. 

292 

99 

25.0 

3.0 

2.1 

100 

88.8 

96.1 


Extra. 

292 

35 

6.0 

0.8 

0.3 

60 

14.9 

8.3 


Prost. 

292 

17 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

5.3 

1.0 


Homo. 

292 

5 

1.0 

0.3 

0.1 

10 

2.8 

0.9 


714 



CLINICAL TABLES .* MARRIED 


715 


married white males 

93. Age 21-25. Educ. level 9-12. Protest., inactive. Almost exactly as described 
for whole high school level (Group 91). Total outlet a bit lower, a mean of 3.8, 
but individual outlets only slightly less than described for Group 91. 

94. Age 21-25. Educ. level 13 +. Whole group. This is the most active age 
period for college males, but among both single and married males this is much 
the most restrained of all social levels. Only 92% of total outlet from marital in- 
tercourse, and in successive age periods the proportion steadily drops. of males 
still draw outlets from masturbation and nocturnal emissions. Only 1 in 7 has 
extra-marital intercourse, 3% with homosexual. Even in marriage, years of pre- 
marital restraint continue to affect sexual adjustment. Lack of facility in ap- 
proaching even the wife for sexual relations. An occasional male with occasional 
impotence because of such inhibitions, and many avoid frequencies of intercourse 
higher than 3 or 4 per week on moral, ethical, or other grounds. Group does ac- 
cept nudity, variety of techniques in pre-coital play, including some oral erotic 
play, and these may aid in development of satisfactory sexual relations. Females 
of group more restrained than males, poorly responsive or completely unrespon- 
sive in a fair proportion of cases; tliis difference between male and female the 
chief sexual factor in marital discord. Since group assumes marital obligations 
with considerable sense of responsibility, and since higher level of education 
provides esthetic and other cultural interests, marriages on a whole more stable 
here than in any other group. 

95. Age 21-25. Educ. level 13+. Urban, Almost precisely as described for 
whole college level above (Group 94). 

96. Age 21-25. Educ, level 13+. Protest., active. Pattern much as described 
for whole college level (Group 94); but somewhat more restrained. Mean fre- 
quency of marital intercourse down to 2.8, often because of doubts as to pro- 
priety of more frequent sexual relations even with wife. All other socio-sexual 
contacts, both heterosexual and homosexual, very minor. Masturbation, how- 
ever, in 60% for whom it provides 5% of outlet. 

97. Age 21-25. Educ. level 13+* Protest., inactive. Essentially as described for 
whole college level (Group 94). Group little affected by religious background, 
but restraint derived from mores of social level. 

98. Age 26-30. Whole group. While still in prime of life by various criteria, 
group has aged sexually ; mean total outlet no higher than that of average younger 
adolescent boy (3.3 per week) — a drop of about 1 .5 per week from rate of married 
teen-age males. Marital intercourse supplies 95% of total outlet and all other out- 
lets minor ; but considerable differences at different social levels, and description 
for each group should be consulted. 

99. Age 26-30. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Grade school and high school 
segments of this age a bit more active socio-sexually than college segment 
(Group 106). Mean total outlet 3.5 per week. 96% of total outlet from marital 
intercourse, mean average frequencies 3.0, but rates of 7, 14, and more not in- 
frequent. Extra-marital intercourse in more than but frequency reduced, of 
these contacts with prostitutes. Number of companions may be high. Mastur- 
bation in but rates low ; all other outlets definitely ininor. Group uninhibited in 
simple intercourse, often inhibited on nudity, pre-coital petting, oral eroticism, 
etc. 



Table 153. Clinical Tables (continued) 
MARRIED WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 


INCID. 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 

AGE: 26-30 

OUTLET 


% 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 0-8 










100. Urban 

Total 

148 

99 

20.0 

3.4 

2.6 

— 

— 

— 


Mast. 

148 

23 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

20 

5.5 

0.7 


Emiss. 

148 

50 

2 0 

0.3 

0 1 

50 

7.3 

1.0 


Interc. 

148 

99 

15.0 

2.9 

2.4 

100 

88.0 

96.0 


Extra. 

148 

35 

5.0 

0.7 

0.1 

60 

15.7 

9.7 


Prost. 

148 

18 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

15 

4.4 

1.4 


Homo. 

148 

7 

0.5 

0.3 

0.1 

5 

1.8 

0.8 

101. Rural 

Total 

117 

100 

25.0 

3.3 

2.1 

_ 

_ 



Mast. 

117 

24 

3.5 

0.5 

0.1 

45 

9.3 

1.0 


Emiss. 

117 

42 

1 0 

0.2 

0.1 

20 

3.9 

1.0 


Interc. 

117 

100 

24.0 

2.9 

1.8 

100 

91.0 

96.5 


Extra. 

117 

30 

3.5 

0.5 

0.2 

50 

11.6 

4.0 


Prost. 

117 

14 

1.0 

0.3 

0.1 

30 

6.5 

0.9 


Homo 

117 

4 

0.5 

0.3 

0.3 

5 

4.9 

4.0 

102. Protest., inactive 

Total 

166 

100 

23.0 

3.4 

2.4 





Mast. 

166 

27 

2.5 

0.3 

0.1 

45 

6.7 

0.8 


Emiss. 

166 

51 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

40 

5.8 

1.0 


Interc. 

166 

100 

20.0 

2.9 

2.1 

100 

88.4 

96.0 


Extra. 

166 

39 

4 0 

0.7 

0.3 

60 

15.0 

9.1 


Prost. 

166 

19 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

5.7 

1.0 


Homo. 

166 

4 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

5 

3.4 

2.0 

Educ. level 9-12 










103. Whole group 

Total 

135 

100 

20.0 

3.6 

2.5 







Mast. 

135 

37 

3.0 

0 5 

0.1 

60 

11.8 

4.3 


Emiss. 

135 

56 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

5.4 

1.8 


Interc. 

135 

100 

15.0 

2.9 

2.1 

100 

85.4 

94.5 


Extra. 

135 

44 

5.0 

0.7 

0.1 

60 

12.4 

5.0 


Prost. 1 

135 

20 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

30 

6.5 

0.8 


Homo. 

135 

8 

1.0 

0.4 

0.3 

15 

6.9 

4.0 

104. Urban 

Total 

81 

100 

17.0 

3.3 

2.6 





Mast. 

81 

36 

3.0 

0.5 

0.2 

60 

13.6 

5.0 


Emiss. 

81 

57 

! 1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

20 

4.6 

1.6 


Interc. 

81 

100 

7.5 

2.6 

2.1 

100 

84.0 

93.6 


Extra. 

81 

48 

5.0 

0.9 

0.2 

60 

1 15.1 

7.5 


Prost. 

81 

21 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

8.2 

0.8 


Homo. 

81 

11 

1.0 

0.3 

0.1 

15 

6.6 

3.0 

105. Protest., inactive 

Total 

77 

100 

17.0 

3.3 

2.5 





Mast. 

77 

36 

2.0 

0.4 

0.1 

60 

11.5 

3.0 


Emiss. 

77 

60 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

5.5 

1.2 


Interc. 

77 

100 

14.0 

2,7 

1.9 

100 

85.3 

95.1 


Extra. 

77 

51 

2.0 

0.5 

0.1 

60 

12.0 

3.0 


Prost. 

77 

21 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

8.5 

0.8 


Homo. 

77 

8 

1.0 

0.5 

0.4 

15 

9.3 

4.3 

Educ. level 13+ 

Total 

532 

100 

22.0 

3.2 

2.6 




106. Whole group 

Mast. 

532 

66 

3.5 

0 4 

0.2 

90 

13.7 

4.9 


Emiss. 

532 

73 

3.0 

0.2 

0.1 

65 

6.9 

2.1 


Interc. 

532 

99 

20.0 

2.6 

2.1 

100 

83.4 

91.1 


Extra. 

532 

22 

3.5 

0.4 

0.1 

70 

8.9 

1.2 


Prost. 

532 

6 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

10 

2.6 

0.7 


Homo. 

532 

3 

2.5 

1.2 

1.3 

50 

25.3 

15.0 


716 


CLINICAL tables: MARRIED 


717 


MARRIED WHITE MALES 

100. Age 26-30. Educ. level 0-8. Urban. Almost exactly as described for total 
grade school level of this age (Group 99). 

101. Age 26-30. Educ. level 0-8. Rural. Almost exactly as described for total 
grade school group of this age (Group 99). Not particularly distinct from urban 
males (Group 100), although this rural group at earlier age levels is a bit less 
active sexually. Extra-marital intercourse not so frequent. 


102. Age 26-30. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. Almost precisely as de- 
scribed for whole grade school level of this age (Group 99). 


103. Age 26-30. Educ. level 9-12. Whole group. Most active of all social levels 
in this age period. Mean total outlet 3.6 per week, but frequencies of marital 
intercourse of 7, 10, or more in 10%. Relative lack of restraint in group further 
shown by nearly having extra-marital intercourse in this period, half of these 
with prostitutes. Nevertheless, marital intercourse accounts for 95% of total out- 
let, all other outlets definitely minor except extra-marital intercourse. Over still 
with masturbation. Group closer to college level in its acceptance of nudity, pre- 
coital petting, and mouth-genital eroticism, but closer to grade school level in its 
acceptance of frequent marital intercourse and of extra-marital intercourse. 

104. Age 26-30. Educ. level 9-12. Urban. Almost precisely as described for 
whole high school level at this age (Group 103). All differences minor. 

105. Age 26-30. Educ. level 9-12. Protest., inactive. Group almost exactly as 
described for whole high school level (Group 103). 95% of total outlet coming 
from marital intercourse, over having extra-marital intercourse, 21% of group 
having some intercourse with prostitutes, but outlets from this and all other 
sources minor. 

106. Age 26-30. Educ. level 13+. Whole group. Sexually less active than males 
of other educational levels at this age, mean total outlet 3.2. Only 91% of outlet 
from marital intercourse, with mean frequency of 2.6. Long period of pre-marital 
restraint reflected in restraint on marital intercourse in this group. See younger 
age group of same college level (Group 94). % of males masturbating, drawing 
5% of outlet from that source. Extra-marital intercourse in but exceedingly in- 
frequent and usually with only 1 or 2 partners. Very few married males in this 
group with homosexual experience at this age. 



Table 153 . Clinical Tables (continued) 
MARRIED WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 



FREQ. PER WEEK 

1 % OF TOTAL OUTLET 




INCID. 








OF 

cases 








AGE: 26-30 

OUTLET 

% 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 13+ 










107. Urban 

Total 

561 

100 

22.0 

3.4 

2.7 

— 

— 




Mast. 

516 

68 

: 3.0 

0.4 

0.2 

90 

13.6 

5.3 


Emiss. 

561 

72 

I 3.0 

0.2 

0.1 

65 

6.3 

2.0 


Interc. 

561 

99 

20.0 

2.8 

2.2 

100 

83.3 

90.8 


Extra. 

561 

25 

‘ 3.5 

0.4 

0.1 

70 

9.1 

1 .0 


Prost. 

516 

6 

1.5 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

2.8 

0.8 


Homo. 

516 

2 

2.5 

1.2 

1.3 

50 

27.8 

20.0 

108. Rural 

Total 

86 

100 

9.0 

3.0 

2.4 





Mast. 

86 

57 

2.0 

0.4 

0.2 

75 

15.3 

4.1 


Emiss. 

86 

73 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

50 

9.8 

2.3 


Interc. 

86 

100 

9.0 

2.5 

1.9 

100 

83.3 

95.0 


Extra. 

86 

9 

1.0 

1.3 

0.4 

.10 

6.0 

5.0 

109. Protest., inactive 

Total 

346 

100 

22.0 

3.5 

2.8 





Mast. 

346 

71 

3.0 

0.5 

0.2 

80 

13 6 

7.1 


Emiss. 

346 

72 

1.5 

0.2 

0.1 

60 

7 0 

2.4 


Interc. 

346 

100 

20.0 

2.8 

2.1 

100 

82.5 

88.6 


Extra. 

346 

26 

3.5 

0.4 

0.1 

70 

9.4 

1.8 


Prost. 

346 

7 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

3.2 

0.8 


Homo. 

346 

3 

2.0 

0.7 

0.1 

5 

16.6 

1.0 

AGE: 31-35 










110. Whole group 

Total 

569 

100 

21.0 

2.7 

2.1 





Mast. 

569 

45 

4.0 

0.4 

0 1 

95 

12 4 

3.9 


Emiss. 

569 

61 

3.0 

0.2 

0.1 

90 

7.2 

1.7 


Interc. 

569 

100 

20.0 

2.2 

1.8 

100 

85.1 

95.0 


Extra. 

569 

29 

4.0 

0.6 

0.2 

75 

15.0 

8,0 


Prost. 

569 

11 

1.5 

0.2 

0.1 

40 

5.4 

0.9 


Homo. 

569 

4 

2.0 

0.5 

0.1 

30 

10.5 

2.5 

Educ. level 0-8 










111. Whole group 

Total 

186 

100 

16.0 

2.6 

1.9 





Mast. 

186 

19 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

35 

5.7 

O.S 


Emiss. 

186 

40 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

65 

6.2 

0.9 


Interc. 

186 

100 

15.0 

2.3 

1.7 

100 

90.5 

96.4 


Extra. 

186 

32 

3.5 

0.6 

0.3 

70 

15.3 

10.6 


Prost. 

186 

16 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

25 

5.8 

1 .0 


Homo. 

186 

4 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

5 

2.1 

0.9 

112. Urban 

Total 

109 

100 

19.0 

3.0 

2.3 





Mast. 

109 

17 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

15 

2.2 

0.7 


Emiss. 

109 

49 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

65 

7.5 

0.9 


Interc. 

109 

100 

17.0 

2.7 

2.2 

100 

89.9 

96.2 


Extra. 

109 

37 

1.0 

0.3 

0.1 

50 

13.0 

8.8 


Prost. 

109 

20 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

15 

5.3 

1 .0 


Homo. 

109 

4 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

2.0 

0.8 

113. Rural 

Total 

93 

100 

16.0 

2.8 

1.7 





Mast. 

93 

16 

1.0 

0.4 

0.1 

20 

9.5 

3.0 


Emiss, 

93 

32 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

15 

3.3 

0.9 


Interc. 

93 

100 

14.0 

2.4 

1.6 

100 

92.1 

96.7 


Extra. 

93 

23 

3.5 

0.8 

0.4 

50 

19.7 

20.0 


Prost. 

93 

9 

1.0 

0.5 

0.3 

10 

5.9 

3.0 

■ — — — 

Homo. 

93 

4 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

5 

2.2 

3.0 


718 



CLINICAL tables: MARRIED 


719 


married white males 

107. Age 26-30. Educ. level 13 +. Urban. A bit more active than corresponding 
rural group of this level (Group 108). Mean total outlet of 3.4, Otherwise almost 
exactly as described for whole college level at this age (Group 106). 


108. Age 2^30. Educ. level 13 +. Rural. Slightly less active than average for 
whole college level of this age (Group 106), consequently a bit more distinct from 
urban population (Group 107). Dra.ws more of its outlet (95%) from marital in- 
tercourse and less from masturbation and extra-marital intercourse, thus ap- 
proaching lower educational levels more than urban college segment. 

109. Age 26-30. Educ. level 13 +. Protest., inactive. Slightly more active than 
any other segment of college level at this age (Group 106X as far as available 
data go. A mean total outlet of 3.5, with only 89% of that outlet from marital 
intercourse. Somewhat higher incidence (71%) and higher frequency of mastur- 
bation, and 26% with experience in extra-marital intercourse. 


110. ' Age 31-35. Whole group. Males in “prime of life,” but with mean total 
outlet averaging only 2.7 per week, which is nearly 20% below mean for boys 
who have just turned adolescent. 95% of outlet drawn from marital intercourse. 
29% of males with extra-marital intercourse which accounts for 8% of total out- 
let. All other outlets minor except masturbation in college level. 

111. Age 31-35. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Grade school level of married 
males at this age definitely slowing up sexually, mean total outlet only 2.6, high 
frequencies of marital intercourse no longer common. This a product of poorer 
physical condition of many males at this level, and not of sexual maladjustment. 
96% of total outlet from marital intercourse, figure increasing for group in later 
age periods. Extra-marital intercourse still in accounting for 1 1% of their out- 
let, of these having contacts with prostitutes. Masturbation taboo, definitely 
rare; less than with nocturnal emissions and that outlet minor. Homosexual 
fairly common at younger ages in this group, now reduced to low incidence and 
scant frequencies. 

112. Age 31-35. Educ. level 0-8. Urban. A bit more active than average for 
whole grade school level at this age (Group 111), with a slightly higher percent- 
age having extra-marital intercourse. Otherwise as described for whole group 
(Group 111). 

113. Age 31-35. Educ. level 0-8. Rural. Very close to pattern described for 
whole grade school level (Group 111), with somewhat lower incidences in mas- 
turbation, nocturnal emissions, extra-marital intercourse, and contacts with 
prostitutes. Marital intercourse much the most important aspect of sex life of 
this group, constituting 97% of total outlet. 



Table 153. Clinical Tables {continued) 
MARRIED WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 


INCID. 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 




V 







AGE: 31-35 

OUTLET 


/o 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 0-8 










114. Protest., inactive 

Total 

119 

100 

16.0 

2.6 

2.0 

— 

— 

— 


Mast. 

119 

25 

1.0 

0.3 

0.1 

35 

6.3 

0.8 


Emiss. 

119 

45 

1.5 

0.2 

0.1 

35 

5.6 

0.8 


Interc. 

119 

100 

14.0 

2 2 

1 8 

100 

89.4 

96.3 


Extra. 

119 

37 

2.5 

0.5 

0.3 

70 

15 0 

10.4 


Prost. 

119 

19 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

25 

5.8 

0.9 


Homo. 

119 

3 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

5 

1.8 

2.0 

Educ. level 9-12 










115. Whole group 

Total 

82 

100 

20.0 

3.4 

2.5 

— 

— 

— 


Mast. 

82 

37 

2.0 

0.4 

0.1 

35 

10.6 

4.3 


Emiss. 

82 

56 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

40 

6.3 

1.6 


Interc. 

82 

100 

15.0 

2.8 

2.1 

100 

86.1 

95.2 


Extra. 

82 

37 

2.0 

0.5 

0 2 

50 

14.4 

4.6 


Prost. 

82 

17 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

5.9 

0.8 


Homo. 

82 

6 

1.0 

0.8 

0.7 

20 

13.5 

15.0 

116. Urban 

Total 

58 

100 

7.0 

3.0 

2.4 

__ 

„ 



Mast. 

58 

40 

2.0 

0.4 

0.1 

30 

9.2 

2.3 


Emiss. 

58 

52 

1.0 

0.2 

0 1 

35 

6.2 

1.4 


Interc. 

58 

100 

7.0 

2.5 

2.1 

100 

85.3 

94.6 


Extra. 

58 

36 

2.0 

0.6 

0.2 

50 

17.5 

10 0 


Prost. 

58 

16 

0.5 

0.3 

0.1 

30 

8.8 

1 0 


Homo. 

58 

7 

1.0 

0.7 

0.8 

20 

14.9 

17.5 

117. Protest , inactive 

Total 

54 

100 

14.0 

3.2 

2.6 

_ 

_ 



Mast. 

54 

32 

1.0 

0.3 

0.2 

30 

11.8 

7.5 


Emiss. 

54 

65 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

40 

5.9 

1.0 


Interc. 

54 

100 

14.0 

2.7 

2.0 

100 

86.7 

95.6 


Extra. 

54 

35 

1.5 

0.4 

0.2 

35 

12.6 

4.0 


Prost. 

54 

17 

0.5 

0 2 

0.1 

30 

8.6 

0.8 


Homo. 

54 

' 6 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

25 

21.3 

25.0 

Educ. level 13+ 










118. Whole group 

Total 

301 

100 

18.0 

2.7 

2.2 

— 






Mast. 

301 

64 

4.0 

0.4 

0.1 

95 

14.0 

4.8 


Emiss. 

301 

75 

3.0 

0.2 

0.1 

75 

7.7 

2.3 


Interc. 

301 

99 

16.0 

2.1 

1.7 

100 

81.4 

90.5 


Extra. 

301 

25 

3.5 

0.6 

0.3 

70 

15.0 

7.8 


Prost. 

301 

7 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

15 

4.5 

0.9 


Homo. 

301 

3 

2.0 

0.7 

0.1 

25 

16.3 

3.0 

119. Urban 

Total 

438 

100 

20,0 

3.0 

2.3 





Mast. 

402 

67 

5.0 

0.4 

0.1 

95 

13.0 

4.5 


Emiss. 

438 

69 

3.0 

0.2 

0.1 

75 

7.6 

2.4 


Interc. 

438 

100 

18.0 

2.4 

1.9 

100 

81.4 

90.0 


Extra. 

438 

34 

4.5 

0.5 

0.2 

70 

15.6 

8.1 


Prost. 

402 

8 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

15 

4.7 

0.9 


Homo. 

402 

2 

2.5 

0.9 

0.3 

25 

20.5 

5.0 

120. Protest., active 

Total 

109 

100 

6.0 

2.3 

2.2 





Mast. 

109 

60 

3.5 

0.4 

0.1 

90 

16.0 

4.L 


Emiss. 

109 

79 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

55 

10.5 

4.0 


Interc. 

109 

98 

5.5 

1.9 

1.8 

100 

82.1 

91.3 


Extra. 

109 

13 

Scant 

0.3 

0.1 

10 

5.5 

0.9 


720 



CLINICAL tables: MARRIED 


721 


married white males 

114. Age 31—35. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. Very closely matches urban 
section of whole grade school level, as described above (Group 112). 

115. Age 31-35. Educ. level 9-12. Whole group. Definitely more active than 
either grade school or college level at this age, with mean total outlet 3.4. 90% 
of total outlet from marital intercourse, of males with some masturbation, 3^^ 
with extra-marital intercourse (3^^ of these with prostitutes), and with some 
nocturnal emissions; but all of these other outlets very minor. Wives at this 
level fairly responsive and sexual adjustments with them fairly good; but extra- 
marital intercourse may cause some diflBculty. 

116. Age 31-35. Educ. level 9-12. Urban. Almost exactly as described for 
whole high school level (Group 115). 

117. Age 31-35. Educ. level 9-12. Protest., inactive. Very closely matching de- 
scription given for whole high school level (Group 115). 

118. Age 31-35. Educ. level 13+. Whole group. College males still reflect re- 
straint of pre-marital years, even at this age in marriage. Mean total outlet only 
2.7 per week, only 91% of that from marital intercourse. For % of males, mastur- 
bation supplies 5% of outlet and 3^^ draw 8% from extra-marital intercourse. This 
extra-marital intercourse, very little of which is had with prostitutes, gradually 
becoming more common, in incidence and in frequency, as these college level 
males grow older. Correspondingly, percentage of outlet from marital intercourse 
gradually decreasing. On both of these items group is quite opposite to lower 
level groups. Sexual problems concern unresponsiveness of wives and, increas- 
ingly with advancing age, problems arise from extra-marital intercourse. 

119. Age 31-35. Educ. level 13+. Urban. Slightly more active group of mar- 
ried males of college level (Group 118), with drawing 8% of their outlet from 
extra-marital intercourse and % drawing 5% from masturbation. All other outlets 
except marital intercourse minor. 

120. Age 31-35. Educ. level 13+. Protest., active. Most restrained segment of 
college group of married males (Group 118) on which data are available. Total 
outlet only 2.3 per week, marital intercourse averaging less than 2 per week, 
never more than 5.5 in most extreme case. Group definitely restrained and al- 
lowing significance of sex in marriage to become gradually less. Both marital 
and extra-marital intercourse less frequent than in remainder of college group 
at this age (Group 118). Many individuals in group not well satisfied with this 
sexual arrangement, although they are not always conscious of that. 



Table 153. Clinical Tables ( continued ) 
MARRIED WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 

OF 

OUTLET 

CASES 

INCID. 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 

AGE; 31-35 

% 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 134- 










121. Protest, inactive 

Total 

270 

100 

20.0 

3.1 

2.4 

— 

— 

— 


Mast 

270 

71 

5.0 

0.4 

0.2 

80 

14,3 

6.6 


Emjss. 

270 

72 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

50 

6.2 

2.0 


Interc. 

270 

100 

18.0 

2.4 

1.9 

100 

79.6 

87.9 


Extra. 

270 

35 

3.5 

0.6 

0.3 

70 

17.7 

10.0 


Prost. 

270 

8 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

15 

5.5 

3.0 


Homo. 

270 

5 

2.5 

0.6 

0.1 

20 

15.5 

2.0 

AGE: 36-40 










122. Whole group 

Total 

390 

100 

22.0 

2.5 

1.9 







Mast 

390 

37 

3.0 

0.4 

0.1 

100 

12.4 

3.7 


Emiss. 

390 

53 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

55 

6.8 

2.2 


Interc. 

390 

99 

20.0 

2.0 

1.6 

100 

85.8 

95.2 


Extra. 

390 

29 

4.0 

0.7 

0.2 

100 

18.8 

9.0 


Prost. 

390 

11 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

50 

7.3 

0.9 


Homo. 

390 

3 

1.0 

0.6 

0.1 

20 

7.6 

5.0 

Educ. level 0-8 










123. Whole group 

Total 

143 

100 

13.0 

2.3 

1.7 

— 

— 



Mast. 

143 

11 

1.0 

0.3 

0.2 

35 

12.2 

5.6 


Emiss. 

143 

29 

1.0 

0.1 

0.1 

20 

4.0 

0.9 


Interc. 

143 

99 

12.0 

2.0 

1.6 

100 

90.3 

96.5 


Extra. 

143 

27 

3.5 

0.7 

0.4 

90 

23.6 

17.2 


Prost. 

143 

10 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

20 

9.1 

2.5 


Homo. 

143 

3 

Scant 

0.2 

0.1 

5 

3.0 

3.0 

124. Urban 

Total 

75 

100 

7.0 

2.5 

2.2 





Mast 

75 

8 

0.5 

0.3 

0.2 

10 

18.0 

0.9 


Emiss. 

75 

33 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

20 

5.3 

1.0 


Interc. 

75 

99 

7.0 

2.3 

2.0 

100 

87.9 

96.2 


Extra. 

75 

33 

3.5 

0.6 

0.2 

90 

24.7 

16.3 


Prost. 

75 

16 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

20 

10.3 

3.0 

125. Rural 

Total 

84 

100 

15.0 

2.5 

L7 





Mast 

84 

12 

1.0 

0.3 

0.3 

15 

6.1 

6.7 


Emiss. 

84 

31 

'l.O 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

2.7 

0.8 


Interc. 

84 

100 i 

14.0 

2.2 

1.5 

100 

93.1 

96.8 


Extra. 

84 

19 

1.5 

0.6 

0.5 

50 

22.6 

' 22.5 


Homo. 

84 

5 

0.5 

0.3 

0.3 

5 

3.8 

5.0 

126. Protest., inactive 

Total 

93 

100 

13.0 

2.5 

1 

1.8 





Mast 

93 

16 

1.0 

0.3 

0.2 

■ 35 

12.2 

5.0 


Emiss. 

93 

37 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

20 

3.9 

0.8 


Interc. 

93 

99 

12.0 

2.1 

1.6 

100 

89.6 

96.3 


Extra. 

93 

29 

3.5 

0.7 

0.4 

80 

22.9 

15.7 


Prost. 

93 

11 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

20 

11.3 

2.0 


Homo. 

93 

3 

Scant 

0.3 

0.1 

5 

3.8 

5.0 

Educ. level 9-12 










127. Whole group 

Total 

58 

100 

10.0 

2.6 

2.0 








Mast 

58 

26 

1.0 

0.3 

0.2 

35 

14.8 

8.3 


Emiss. 

58 

52 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

7.0 

2.5 


Interc. 

58 

100 

10.0 

2.3 

1.7 

100 

87.7 

95.4 


Extra. 

58 

33 

1,0 

0.3 

0.2 

35 

11.4 

5.0 

1 

Prost. 

58 

19 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

8.7 

0.9 


722 



CLINICAL tables: MARRIED 


723 


married white males 

121 • Age 31-35. Educ, level 13+* Protest., inactive. Pattern typical of married 
males of college level (Group 118). Group definitely less inhibited than actively 
religious males of same level (Group 120). Mean total outlet 3.1, only 88% from 
marital intercourse, remainder supplied by masturbation (for 71% it provides 
7% of outlet) and extra-marital intercourse (10% of total outlet for 35% of 
males). Developing an increasing dependence upon masturbation and extra- 
marital intercourse as substitute for sexual relations with wives who are unre- 
sponsive or inhibited in accepting such variety of techniques as average male 
at this level would like. In spite of their expressed desires, however, these males 
themselves are inhibited. 

122. Age 36-40. Whole group. Married males at this age on a steady decline 
sexually. Mean total outlet 2.5, 95% of total from marital intercourse; mastur- 
bation in but mostly at college level and quite taboo at lower levels. Nocturnal 
emissions in more than 3 .^, but very minor and chiefly in high school and college 
levels. Extra-marital intercourse in nearly at this age as abundant or more 
abundant in college level than in lower level. 

123. Age 36-40. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Married males of grade school 
level with mean total outlet 2.3. Males of this level often begin to show physical 
effects of aging, with mean frequencies of marital intercourse down to 2.0 and 
frequencies above 7 now rare. With advancing age, an increasing proportion of 
outlet (97%) from marital intercourse (in contrast to decreasing proportion 
among males of college level). Extra-marital intercourse in not more than ac- 
counting for 17% of their outlet. All other outlets infrequent and of minor sig- 
nificance. 

124. Age 36-40. Educ. level 0-8. Urban. Pattern much as described for whole 
grade school level of this age (Group 123), but city males more often have extra- 
marital intercourse (in 33%) and more often go to prostitutes (in 16%). 

125. Age 36-40. Educ. level 0-8. Rural. Closely fitting description given for 
whole grade school level (Group 123). Incidence of extra-marital intercourse 
(19%) not as high as in city group; otherwise closer to city group than was true 
among rural populations at earlier ages. 

126. Age 36-40. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. Closely fitting description 
given for whole grade school level at this age (Group 123). 

127. Age 36-40. Educ. level 9-12. Whole group. Even this high school group 
begins to show effects of aging; now not more active than any other social level. 
95% of total sexual outlet from marital intercourse. masturbate, have extra- 
marital intercourse, more than having some with prostitutes. All outlets except 
marital intercourse infrequent and a minor part of total outlet. 



Table 153. Clinical Tables (continued) 
IvIARRIED WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 

CASES 

INCID 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 



0 / 







AGE: 36-40 

OUTLET 


o 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 13 -f 










128. Whole group 

Total 

189 

100 

22.0 

2.6 

2.0 

— 

— 

— 


Mast 

189 

5) 

3.0 

0 4 

0.1 

95 

12.1 

3.3 


Emiss. 

189 

72 

2 0 

0.2 

0 1 

55 

7.6 

3.1 


Interc. 

189 

99 

12 0 

1 9 

1.6 

100 

81.9 

90.2 


Extra. 

189 

30 

3.5 

0 9 

0.2 

100 

18.1 

7 1 


Prost 

189 

9 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

20 

4 9 

0 9 


Homo 

189 

3 

0 5 

0 9 

0.1 

10 

7.0 

5.0 

129. Urban 

Total 

281 

100 

22 0 

2 7 

2 1 

_ 

_ 



Mast 

281 

62 

4 0 

0 4 

0.1 

95 

11 0 

3.0 


Emiss. 

281 

67 

2 0 

0 2 

0.1 

55 

7.0 

3.0 


Interc. 

281 

99 

12.0 

2.1 

1.7 

100 

82 3 

90.1 


Extra. 

281 

37 

6.0 

0 7 

0.2 

100 

19.3 

7.5 


Prost. 

281 

8 

1.0 

0.5 

0.1 

20 

4.9 

0.9 

130. Protest., active 

Total 

73 

100 

4 5 

2.0 

1 9 





Mast. 

73 

55 

3.0 

0.4 

0 1 

95 

18.8 

4.6 


Emiss. 

73 

75 

1.0 

0.2 

0 1 

50 

10.3 

4 2 


Interc. 

73 

99 

4.0 

1.6 

1.6 

100 

81.5 

89 6 


Extra. 

73 

16 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

15 

5.3 

2.0 

131. Protest., inactive 

Total 

187 

99 

22.0 

2.9 

2.2 





Mast. 

187 

71 

3.0 

0 4 

0 2 

70 

10.4 

3 9 


Emiss. 

187 

67 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

40 

6.8 

3.1 


Interc. 

187 

98 

12 0 

2.1 

1.6 

100 

80.0 

88.3 


Extra. 

187 

41 1 

6 0 

0.8 

0.3 

100 

22.0 

9.5 


Prost. 

187 

10 

1.0 

0.5 

0.1 

20 

5.5 

0.9 


Homo. 

187 

3 

0.5 

0.7 

0.1 

10 

7 0 

5.0 

AGE: 41-45 










132. Whole group 

Total 

272 

100 

15.0 

2.0 

1.6 

_ 

__ 



Mast. 

272 

33 

3.0 

0.3 

0.1 1 

90 

12.7 

4.3 


Emiss. 

272 

54 

I.O 

0.2 

0.1 

60 

7 5 

2.0 


Interc. 

272 

99 

14.0 

1.6 

1.3 

100 

85 9 

95.4 


Extra. 

272 

24 

2.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

24 3 

16.3 


Prost. 

272 

9 

0.5 

0.2 

0.1 

35 

8.0 

1.0 


Homo. 

272 

i 2 

Scant 

0.8 

0.1 

5 

5.5 

0.9 

Educ. level 0-8 



! 







133 Whole group 

Total 

100 

100 

9.5 

1.9 

1.4 







Mast. 

100 

9 

1.0 

0.3 

0 2 

10 

5.2 

5.0 


Emiss. 

100 

31 

1.0 

0.1 

0 1 

20 

4.4 

0.9 


Interc. 

100 

99 

9 0 

1.7 

1.2 

100 

90.8 

96.7 


Extra. 

lOU 

21 

2.0 

0 6 

0.4 

100 

32.2 

19.0 


Prost. 

100 

11 

0.5 

0.3 

0.2 

20 

12.3 

5.0 


Homo. 

100 

3 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

1.3 

1.0 

134. Rural 

Total 

52 

100 

9.5 

2.1 

1.4 





Mast. 

52 

12 

1 0 

0.4 

0.4 

10 

7.6 

7.5 


Emiss. 

52 

27 

0 5 

0.2 

0.1 

10 

3.0 

0.9 


Interc. 

52 

ICO 

9 0 

2 1 

1.3 

100 

93.7 

97.0 


Extra. 

52 

12 

1.0 

0 6 

0 6 

50 

26 3 

27.5 


Prost 

52 

4 

Scant 

0.2 

0 3 

Scant 

1.8 

3.0 


Homo. 

1 

4 

Scant 

0 1 

0.1 

Scant 

1.8 

3.0 


724 



MARRIED WHITE MALES 

128. Age 36-40. Edoc. level 13+. Whole group. Married males of this college 
level usually in fair condition physically and sexually capable enough. Not ma- 
terially different from college level in preceding 5-year period (Group 118). 
Whole group shows effect of pre-marital restraint which is characteristic of this 
level. 90% of total outlet from marital intercourse. 59% masturbate, usually with 
low frequencies; 30% have extra-marital intercourse, usually not with prosti- 
tutes; other outlets of minor significance. Problems of sexual adjustment in mar- 
riage not as frequent as at an earlier age because male rates have dropped, and 
female has lost some of previous inhibitions and is now near prime of her re- 
sponsiveness. Occasionally this erotic development of female comes too late to 
correct a maladjustment that developed earlier in the marriage; and male’s ex- 
tra-marital intercourse may become a source of difficulty. 

129. Age 36-40. Educ. level 13+. Urban. Very well described by paragraph 
above for whole college level (Group 128). 

130. Age 36-40. Educ. level 13+. Protest, active. Most restrained segment of 
college level, as far as data are available. General pattern that of whole level 
(Group 128), but mean total outlet down to 2.0 per week. Mean frequency of 
marital intercourse 1.6, in no individual more than 4 per week, supplying only 
90% of total sexual outlet. More than 3^^ of males masturbate but infrequently, % 
have emissions but infrequently. Extra-marital intercourse in only 16%, supply- 
ing only 2% of outlet, but even then a likely source of conflict. Marked contrast 
to inactive Protestants of same level (Group 131). Clinician may well examine 
possibility that maladjusted individual from this group is in sexual conflict, even 
when he asserts otherwise. 

131. Age 36-40. Educ. level 13+. Protest., inactive. Most active segment of 
college level at this age. General pattern quite like that described for whole col- 
lege level (Group 128), but mean total outlet 2.9 per week, marital intercourse 2.1 
per week, with maximum frequencies still up to 12 per week. 88% of outlet from 
marital intercourse. 41% with extra-marital intercourse, which supplies 10% of 
outlet, prostitutes in only 3^^ of these cases. Masturbation in nearly but not fre- 
quent. 

132. Age 41-45. Whole group. Sexual activity continues to drop, mean total 
outlet averaging only 2.0 by this age. 95% of total outlet from marital intercourse, 
mean frequency 1.6, but some individuals still averaging 7, 10, or more per 
week. Masturbation still in chiefly, however, at college level ; frequencies low. 
Nocturnal emissions in but of no great significance. Extra-marital intercourse 
in y, other outlets very minor. 

133. Age 41-45. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Males of this level in early 
forties so aftected by age that their rates for first time are now lower than rates 
of college level. Early aging probably a product of poor nutrition, hard work, or 
original physical or mental incapacity. Marital intercourse supplies 97% of out- 
let, but mean frequencies down to 1.7, and for y of population not more often 
than 1.2 per week. Very few individuals with frequencies to 7 per week. Extra- 
marital intercourse in only 21%, all other outlets very minor. Problems of sexual 
adjustment less significant because of lowered interest of these males in sexual 
activity, and particularly because of decrease in frequency of extra-marital in- 
tercourse. 

134. Age 41-45. Educ. level 0-8. Rural. Married males of rural group drawing 
97% of total outlet from marital intercourse, mean frequency 2.1 per week, 
median 1.3. Extra-marital intercourse in only 12%, all other outlets very minor. 

725 



Table 153. Clinical Tables {continued) 
MARRIED WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 



FREQ, PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 


OF 

CASES 








AGE; 41-^5 

OUTLET 


/o 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 0-8 










135. Protest., inactive 

Total 

60 

100 

9.5 

1.9 

1.3 

— 

— 




Mast. 

60 

13 

0.5 

0.2 

0.2 

10 

4.9 

3.0 


Emiss 

60 

42 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

20 

4.5 

0.9 


Interc. 

60 

98 

9.0 

1.8 

1.0 

100 

87.8 

96.5 


Extra. 

60 

27 

2.0 

0.6 

0.5 

100 

36.4 

27.5 


Prost 

60 

12 

0.5 

0.3 

0.2 

20 

17.3 

7.5 


Homo. 

60 

3 

Scant 

0.1 

0.1 

Scant 

1.8 

3.0 

Educ. level 13 -f 










136. Whole group 

Total 

138 

100 

9.5 

2.0 

1.7 

— 

«« 



Mast. 

138 

55 

3.0 

0.3 

0.1 

90 

13.2 

4.3 


Emiss. 

138 

73 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

45 

7.5 

2.5 


Intcrc. 

138 

99 

8.0 

1.5 

1.3 

100 

82.0 

91.0 


Extra. 

138 

24 

2.0 

0.5 

0.2 

85 

21.4 

15.0 


Prost. 

138 

5 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

5 

1.9 

0.8 

137. Urban 

Total 

112 

100 

9.5 

2.1 

1.8 





Mast. 

112 

55 

3.0 

0.4 

0.1 

90 

13.0 

4.4 


Emiss. 

112 

74 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

45 

7.0 

2.4 


Interc. 

112 

99 

8.0 

1.6 

1 3 

100 

82.1 

91.2 


Extra. 

112 

23 

2.0 

0.6 

0.3 

85 

23.0 

12.5 


Prost. 

112 

6 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

5 

1.9 

0.8 


Homo. 

112 

3 

1 

Scant 

1.5 

0.1 

Scant 

9.7 

1.0 

138. Protest., inactive 

Total 

83 

100 

9.5 

2.1 

1.7 





Mast. 

83 

60 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

55 

11.0 

5.8 


Emiss. 

83 

72 

1 0 

0.1 

0.1 

30 

6.4 

2.0 


Interc. 

83 

100 

8.0 

1.5 

1.3 

100 

80.0 

87.2 


Extra. 

83 

31 

2.0 

0.6 

0.4 

85 

25.9 

22.5 


Prost. 

83 

7 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

1 5 

2.2 

0.9 


Homo. 

83 

4 

Scant 

1.5 

0.1 

Scant 

9.7 

1.0 

AGE: 46-50 










139. Whole group 

Total 

175 

1 99 

14.0 

1.8 

1.2 





Mast. 

175 

31 

1,5 

0.3 

0.1 

70 

14.9 

6.8 


Emiss. 

175 

48 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

80 

9.4 

2.2 


Interc. 

175 

97 

14.0 

1.5 

0.9 

100 

83.7 

95.8 


Extra. 

175 

24 

2.5 

0.7 

0.4 

95 

29.1 

18.8 


Prost. 

175 

6 

0.5 

0.3 

0.2 

10 

12.3 

5.6 


Homo. 

175 

2 

Scant 

1.5 

0.1 

5 

12.2 

5.0 

Educ. level 0-8 










140. Whole group 

Total 

70 

97 

14.0 

1.9 

1.2 

___ 





Mast. 

70 

7 

1.0 

0.4 

0.1 

10 

6.5 

1.0 


Emiss. 

70 

29 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

20 

5.1 

1.0 


Interc. 

70 

97 

14.0 

1.7 

0.9 

100 

90.1 

96.8 


Extra. 

70 • 

19 

1.5 

0.6 

0.4 

80 

33.6 

35.0 


Prost 

70 

9 

0.5 

0.3 

0.3 

10 

17.2 

6.3 

Educ. level 13+ 










141. Whole group 

Total 

81 

100 

7.5 

1.8 

1.3 

— 





Mast 

81 

57 

1.5 

0.3 

0.1 

70 

14.5 

6.9 


Emiss. 

81 

68 

1.0 

0.1 

0.1 

50 

9.2 

2.5 


Interc. 

81 

98 

5.5 

1.3 

0-9 

100 

78.2 

92.5 


Extra. 

81 

27 

2.5 

0.9 

0.6 

95 

30.3 

22.5 


726 



CLINICAL tables: MARRIED 


121 


MARRIED WHITE MALES 

135. Age 41-45. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. Pattern well covered by 
description for whole grade school level at this age (Group 133). 

136. Age 41-45. Educ, level 13+- Whole group. Married males of college level 
usually in good condition physically at this age, although mean total outlet has 
continued to drop since late teens, now down to 2.0. Marital intercourse 91% 
of total outlet, extra-marital intercourse in 34 group accounting for 15% of 
their outlet. Masturbation still in more than nocturnal emissions in but 
these and other outlets of minor significance. Group offers no special sexual 
problems other than those which are the product of general restraint character- 
istic of college group throughout their histories. 

137. Age 41-45. Educ. level 13+. Urban. Pattern almost exactly as described 
in whole college level (Group 136). 

138.. Age 41-45. Educ. level 13+. Protest., inactive. Very close to pattern de- 
scribed for whole college level (Group 136). Marital intercourse accounting for 
only 87% of total outlet. Somewhat higher incidence (31%) of extra-marital in- 
tercourse, accounting for 23% of outlet. Masturbation in 60%, accounting for 
6% of their outlet. Other sexual activities less significant. 

139. Age 46-50. Whole group. General level of sexual activity continues to de- 
cline with age. In these late forties, mean total outlet is 1.8 per week, about 96% 
of that from marital intercourse, of males with extra-marital intercourse which 
provides 19% of their outlet, masturbation in 3^^, other outlets minor. First cases 
of permanent impotence from age, although healthy males may not expect im- 
potence for another 10 or 20 years, if ever. Some impotence now appearing is 
product of inhibition or loss of interest in sexual activity. Some problems of 
marital adjustment now develop because of waning interest in sexual activity, 
although not generally so for another 10 years for most males. 

140. Age 46-50. Educ. level 0-8. Wliole group. Males of grade school level de- 
rive higher proportion of total outlet from marital intercourse, with incidence of 
extra-marital intercourse (19%) definitely lower than in corresponding college 
level (Group 141). Aging problems more acute in this lower level at this age, 
than in college level (see Group 139). 

141. Age 46-50. Educ. level 13+. Whole group. General pattern as described 
for all married males of this age (Group 139). Aging problems less acute than 
with lower social levels. Intercourse accounting for only 93% of outlet, extra- 
marital intercourse increasing, with incidence now 27%. Masturbation increasing, 
now in 57%, accounting for 7% of outlet. Emissions still in 68%, accounting for 
only 3% of outlet. 



Table 153. Clinical Tables {conclude d) 
MARRIED WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 



1 FREQ. PER WEEK 

1 % OF TOTAL OUTLET 




INCID 









CASES 








AGE: 46-50 

OUTLET 

0/ 

/ o 

Range 

Mean 

Med 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Educ. level 13-}- 










142. Urban 

Total 

64 

100 

7.5 

2 0 

1.5 

— 

— 

— 


Mast. 

64 

58 

1 5 

0 3 

0.1 

70 

15.4 

7.5 


Emiss. 

64 

69 

1 0 

0.2 

0.1 

45 

7.9 

3 0 


Interc. 

64 

97 

5 5 

1.4 

1.0 

100 

79.3 

91.9 


Extra. 

64 

27 

2.5 

1 0 

0 5 

95 

29.0 

15.0 


Frost. 

64 

3 

0 5 

0 3 

0.4 

5 

5.5 

7.5 

AGE: 51-55 










143. Whole group 

Total 

109 

98 

7 0 

1 6 

1.0 


__ 

_ 


Mast. 

109 

26 

1 5 

0 4 

0 1 

50 

18.2 

11.3 


Emiss 

109 

44 

1 0 

0.2 

0.1 

50 

7.9 

0.9 


Interc. 

109 

97 

6.0 

1.2 

0 8 

100 

83.1 

96.1 


Extra. 

109 

22 

2 0 

0.8 

0.5 

90 

33.3. 

32.5 


Frost. 

109 

7 

0 5 

0 2 

0.2 

25 

15.2 

3.0 


Homo. 

109 

2 

Scant 

0 7 

0 8 

5 

15.5 

17.5 

Educ. level 0-8 










144. Whole group 

Total 

50 

96 

6.0 

1.4 

0.9 








Mast. 

50 

6 

Scant 

0.3 

0.1 

Scant 

3 0 

1 0 


Emiss. 

50 

20 

1.0 

0.2 

0 1 

50 

12.0 

0 9 


Interc. 

1 50 

96 

6 0 

1 2 

0.7 

100 

88.3 

97.0 


Extra. 

50 

20 

1 5 

0 5 

0.3 

90 

32.5 

22.5 


Frost. 

50 

10 

0.5 

0.3 

0.3 

25 

23.5 

5.0 

AGE: 56-60 










145. Whole group 

Total 

67 

99 

4.5 

1.1 

0.8 








Mast. 

67 

19 

0.5 

0 2 

0.1 

60 

20.9 

7.8 


Emiss. 

67 

28 

0,5 

0.1 

0 1 

60 

13.8 

1 .0 


Interc 

67 

94 

3.0 

0.9 

0.7 

100 

81 1 

96 3 


Extra. 

67 

25 

2.0 

0.6 

0.4 

100 

37,0 

15.0 


Frost. 

67 

8 

0.5 

0.3 

0.1 

5 

19.5 

1.0 


728 



CLINICAL tables: MARRIED 


729 


MARRIED WHITE MALES 

142. Age 46-50. Educ. level 13+* Urban. Very close to pattern described for 
whole college level at this age (Group 141). 

143. Age 51-55. Whole group. Steady decline in sexual activity continues, as it 
has since late teens. Mean total outlet now 1.6, 2% of total population no longer 
experiences orgasm. Maximum frequencies for any male 7 per week, although 
median frequencies ar^e only 1 per week. Nearly all total outlet still from marital 
intercourse, more true of lower levels, less true of upper levels. Masturbation 
practically out for lower educational level, on increase for upper level. Extra- 
marital intercourse steadily increasing for upper level. Among males of college 
level, marital intercourse may not account for more than 62% of total outlet at 
this age. Decline of sexual interest between spouses only occasionally causing 
serious trouble at this age. Impotence slightly more frequent but still in not more 
than 9% of population. 

144. Age 51-55. Educ. level 0-8. Wliole group. Some differences from whole 
group described above (Group 143). Mean total outlet (1.4) lower. Percentage 
derived from marital intercourse steadily increasing, now totals 97%. Only 20% 
having extra-marital intercourse, from which they derive 23% of outlet. Mastur- 
bation exceedingly scant at this lower level, other outlets not important. 4% of 
group inactive, no longer experiencing orgasm. 

145. Age 56-60. Whole group. The aging process leads to a mean frequency of 
only 1 . 1 per week for this group. The most extreme male experiences orgasm only 
4.5 per week. Nearly all outlet of lower educational level derived from marital 
intercourse, not % of it so derived by college level. Extra-marital intercourse in- 
frequent at lower level, more frequent at upper level. Masturbation and noc- 
turnal emissions account for very little of lower level’s outlet, may account for 
as much as 22% of outlet of college level at this age. Impotence now in 18%. 



Table 154. Clinical Tables 

PREVIOUSLY MARRIED WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 

OF 

OUTLET 

CASES 

: INCID. 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 

AGE ; 21-25 

1 % 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

146. Whole group 

Total i 

119 

98 

23.0 

3.8 

2.0 







Mast. 

119 

45 

3.0 

0.6 

0.3 

100 

26.3 

17.5 


Emiss. 

119 

62 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

50 

10.3 

2.7 


Interc. 

150 

96 

28.0 

3.3 

1.6 

100 

79.5 

95.1 


Comp. 

119 

96 

20.0 

2.9 

1.2 

100 

68.2 

85.4 


Prost. 

119 

47 

1.5 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

18.4 

2.7 


Homo. 

.119 

27 

4.0 

1.1 

0.3 

85 

18.9 

4.6 

Educ, level 0-8 










147. Whole group 

Total 

93 

96 

23.0 

3.7 

1.8 

— 

— 

— 

Mast. 

93 

34 

2.0 

0.5 

0.3 

100 

22.2 

10.8 


Emiss. 

93 

50 

2.0 

0.3 

0.2 

50 

10.1 

4.8 


Interc. 

93 

94 

28.0 

3.4 

1.5 

100 

86.5 

95.6 


Comp. 

93 

88 

20.0 

3.3 

1.2 

100 

74.0 

89.2 


Prost. 

93 

46 

1.0 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

31.7 

17.5 


Homo. 

93 

14 

1.0 

0.5 

0.2 

15 

9.7 

5.0 

148. Protest., inactive 

Total 

69 

97 

18.0 

3.4 

1.7 



' 



Mast. 

69 

36 

2.0 

0.6 

0.3 

100 

26.9 

18.3 


Emiss. 

69 

48 

2.0 

0.3 

0.2 

40 

9.4 

4.4 


Interc. 

69 

94 

20.0 

3.2 

1.5 

100 

86.4 

95.7 


Comp. 

69 

87 

18.0 

3.1 

1.2 

100 

73.2 

88.2 


Prost. 

69 

51 

1.0 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

32.4 

20.0 


Homo. 

69 

12 

1.0 

0.3 

0.2 

10 

4.6 

4.0 

Educ. level 9-12 










149. Whole group 

Total 

57 

100 

22.0 

4.2 

2.3 

— 

— 

— 

Mast. 

57 

56 

3.0 

0.6 

0.2 

65 

19.1 

11.3 


Emiss. 

57 

72 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

50 

10.7 

1.9 


Interc. 

56 

91 

20.0 

3.4 

1.8 

100 

77.6 1 

93.8 


Comp. 

57 

91 

20.0 

3.3 

1.7 

100 

73.9 1 

90.8 


Prost. 

57 

46 

2.0 

0.2 

0.1 

30 

6.7 1 

0.8 


Homo. 

57 

39 

3.5 

1.4 

0.6 

80 

25.8 

12.5 

AGE; 26-30 









150. Whole group 

Total 

182 

97 

23.0 

3.0 

1.8 

— 




Mast. 

182 

44 

4.0 

0.6 

0.3 

100 

24.0 

12.5 


Emiss. 

182 

64 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

12.6 

3.7 

i 

Interc. 

192 

95 

20.0 

2.4 

1.3 

100 

78.6 

94.0 


Comp. 

182 

90 

20.0 

2.4 

1.1 

100 

68.5 

81.7 


Prost. 

182 

56 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

100 

22.3 

6.4 

i 

Homo. 

182 

18 

4.0 

0.9 

0.3 

85 

18.7 

6.9 

Educ. level 0-8 










151. Whole group 

Total 

108 

95 

17.0 

2.9 

1.6 

— 





Mast. 

108 

35 

2.0 

0.5 

0.2 

100 

24.4 

13.8 


Emiss. 

108 

58 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

50 

10.6 

4.6 


Interc. 

117 1 

94 

17.0 

2.3 

1.0 

100 

83.3 

95.4 


Comp. 

108 

86 

17.0 

2.4 : 

0.9 

100 

68.3 

81.0 


Prost. 

108 . 

63 

1.5 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

29.6 

14.6 


Homo. 

108 

14 

3.0 

0.7 

0.2 

— 

— 

— 

152. Urban 

f Total 

68 

99 

17.0 

3.2 

1.7 





Mast. 

68 

38 

1.5 

' 0.4 

0.2 

j 60 

19.4 

11.3 


Emiss. 

68 

62 

2.0 

0.3 

0.1 

50 

10.0 

3.6 


Interc. 

75 

97 

17.0 

2.6 

1.2 

100 

82.9 

95.4 


Comp. 

68 

93 

17.0 

2.6 

1.0 

100 

68.2 

80.0 


Prost. 

68 

69 

1.5 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

27.5 

15.0 


Homo. 

68 

19 

3.0 

0.7 

0.2 

25 

11.3 

6.3 


730 



Table 154 . Clinical Tables 
PREVIOUSLY MARRIED WHITE MALES 

146. Age 21 - 25 . Whole group. Males who have been previously married but 
who no longer live with wives, have patterns closer to those of married males than 
to those of single males. 96% of whole population continues heterosexual coitus, 
3^^ having some with prostitutes, but most frequently with other females. Mastur- 
bation in less than 3^, but accounting for 18% of their outlet. Nocturnal emissions 
in nearly Homosexual activities much more frequent than among married 
males, less frequent than among some groups of single males, now in 27% ac- 
counting for 5% of outlet. Often present personality problems which not infre- 
quently involve sexual maladjustments. Many come from lower educational 
levels where there is considerable irresponsibility, incapacity, unwillingness or 
ineffectiveness in securing and holding jobs, failure to support wives, cruelty, and 
problems arising from drinking. While married, were promiscuous in extra- 
marital relations and had a fair amount of homosexual activity, and these some- 
times caused divorce. This, however, a personahty problem rather than a 
sexual problem in origin. 

147. Age 21--25. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Pattern well described in para- 
graph above (Group 146). Apparently somewhat lower incidence of masturba- 
tion and of homosexual. Few inhibitions on socio-sexual contacts in this group, 
but more inhibitions against masturbation, nudity, petting techniques, and oral 
eroticism. Promiscuous, and VD often high. 

148. Age 21-25. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. Pattern covered by descrip- 
tions in two paragraphs above (Groups 146, 147). 

149. Age 21-25. Educ. level 9-12. Whole group. Most active males of this age 
group, with mean total outlet 4.2. 91% have intercourse which accounts for 94% 
of their outlet, of these have some intercourse with prostitutes, which accounts 
for less than 1% of outlet. 56% masturbate, so deriving 11% of their outlet, 39% 
have homosexual relations which account for 13% of their outlet. In heterosexual 
promiscuity and high incidence of homosexual, this group a close duplicate of 
single males of same educational level and age (Group 48). Marriage a passing 
episode to many of these individuals; many present basic personality problems. 
Others more balanced. Clinicians will do well to make a sharp distinction be- 
tween persons with these diverse backgrounds. 

150. Age 26-30. Whole group. Males who have been previously married but 
are no longer living with wives, have patterns closer to those of married males 
than to those of single males. 95% have intercourse which provides 94% of total 
outlet, nearly 60% have some of that intercourse with prostitutes; and consider- 
able promiscuity for grade school and high school males of this ^oup. Mastur- 
bation in 44%, accounting for 13% of their outlet, but more of this in upper edu- 
cational levels, less in lower educational levels. Homosexual in 18%, accounting 
for 7% of outlet. Compare younger age level of previously married males 
(Group 146). A larger number of these males have made a real attempt at marital 
adjustment and have encountered more serious difficulties, which in many cases 
have involved sexual problems. 

151. Age 26-30. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Remarks made in paragraph 
above (Group 150) applicable to grade school level of this age group. Mastur- 
bation less frequently a source of outlet (in 35% of males), prostitutes providing 
a larger portion of intercourse (for 63% of males); homosexual of minor sig- 
nificance but in 14% of histories. Few inhibitions on socio-sexual contacts in 
this group, but more against masturbation, nudity, petting techniques, and oral 
eroticism. 


731 



CLINICAL TABLES : PREVIOUSLY MARRIED 


733 


PREVIOUSLY MARRIED WHITE MALES 

152. Age 26-30. Educ. level 0-8. Urban. A somewhat more active segment of 
grade school level (Group 151). Mean total outlet 3.2 with frequencies of 10, 14, 
or more not uncommon, 97% with intercourse which provides 95% of total out- 
let, 69% with some intercourse with prostitutes, 1 9% with homosexual outlet. 
Group heterosexually almost as active as married males and homosexually more 
so. 


153. Age 26-30. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. Almost exactly as de- 
scribed above (Group 152). 

154. Age 31-35. Whole group. Males who have been previously married very 
little less active than married males of same age, with mean total outlet 2.0. In- 
tercourse in 91%, accounting for 95% of their total outlet. 61% have some inter- 
course with prostitutes, 11% have homosexual relations, 39% have masturbation 
which accounts for only 15% of outlet. 5% apparently without sexual outlet, 
these the individuals most likely to be disturbed and most often involved in basic 
personality difficulties. Any individual among previously married males who is 
having outlets much less frequently than those given here, needs especial con- 
sideration. 

155. Age 31-35. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Pattern very close to that de- 
scribed for whole group above (Group 154). Mean total outlet 1.8, 94% deriv- 
ing 96% of outlet from intercourse, over 2^ of these having some intercourse with 
prostitutes, masturbation in only of histories, 5% apparently without any sex- 
ual outlet. 

156. Age 31-35. Educ. level 0-8. Urban. A less restrained segment of grade 
school group (Groups 154, 155). Only 1% without any sexual outlet. 97% with 
some intercourse accounting for 96% of outlet, about % of these with some of 
intercourse with prostitutes. Masturbation very minor source of outlet. 

157. Age 31-35. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. Record very close to de- 
scription given for whole grade school level (Group 155). 

158. Age 36-40. Whole group. Aging effect on males who have been previously 
married somewhat sharper than on married males. Mean total outlet down to 1.7, 
but nearly all males (98%) with active histories. 92% of group deriving 96% of 
outlet from intercourse, nearly % of these having some with prostitutes, 6% with 
homosexual, masturbation in 42% accounting for 17% of total outlet. 

159. Age 36-40. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Available data do not show this 
group significantly different from description given above (Group 158), except 
that 70% have intercourse with prostitutes, which accounts for 30% of outlet. 



Table 154. Clinical Tables {concluded) 
PREVIOUSLY MARRIED WHITE MALES 



SOURCE 


INCID. 

FREQ. PER WEEK 

% OF TOTAL OUTLET 

AGE: 36-40 

OUTLET 


% 

Range 

Mean 

Med. 

Range 

Mean 

Med, 

Educ. level 0-8 










160. Protest, inactive 

Total 

62 

97 

16.0 

1.7 

1.0 

— 

— 

— 


Mast 

62 

44 

1.0 

0.4 

0 2 

95 

25.8 

15.0 


Emiss. 

62 

48 i 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

100 

13.9 

0.9 


Interc. 

66 

92 

5.0 

1.1 

0.7 

100 

80.3 

96.1 


Comp. 

62 

76 

10.0 

1.4 

0.5 

100 

65.8 

87.5 


Prost. 

62 

69 

1.5 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

39.1 

35.0 


Homo. 

62 

5 

Scant 

1.6 

0,1 

10 

13.8 

10.0 

AGE: 41-45 





I 





161. Whole group 

Total 

96 

94 

12.0 

1 1.6 

1.0 







Mast 

96 

33 

2.0 

0.6 

0.3 

95 

32 1 

17.5 


Emiss. 

96 

39 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

100 

21.6 

5.0 


Interc 

107 

86 

10.0 

1.4 

0.8 

100 

85.1 

96.2 


Comp. 

96 

69 

10,0 

1.2 

0.6 

100 

62 .‘4 

69.4 


Prost. 

96 

60 

1.5 

0.5 

0.4 

100 

46,9 i 

47.5 


Homo. 

96 1 

5 

1.0 

1.3 

0.5 

35 

22.5 

15.0 

Educ. level 0-8 










162. Whole group 

Total 

69 

93 

12.0 

1.5 

0.9 

— 





Mast j 

69 

26 

1.5 

0.6 

0.3 

95 

33.3 

15.0 


Emiss. 

69 

32 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

100 

18.1 

2.0 


Interc. j 

71 

87 

10.0 

1.3 

0.8 

100 

87 0 

96.5 


Comp. 

69 

68 

10.0 

1.2 

0.5 

100 

62.1 

70.0 


Prost. 

69 

68 

1.5 

0.5 

0.4 

100 

49.1 

48.0 


Homo. 

69 

6 

0 5 

1.4 

0.4 

15 

13.6 

12.5 

AGE: 46-50 









163. Whole group 

Total 

63 

94 

6.0 

1.4 

0.8 

! 




! Mast. 

1 63 

44 

2,0 

0.5 

0.4 

100 

36.6 

32.5 


Emiss. 

63 

32 

1.0 

0.2 

0.1 

100 

21.3 

5.8 


Interc. 

72 

82 

5.0 

1.3 

0.7 

100 

83,3 

96.3 


Comp. 

63 

67 

4.5 

1.2 

0.5 

100 

63.7 

77.5 


Prost. 

63 

52 

1.0 

0.4 

0.3 

100 

48.8 

50.0 


734 



CLINICAL tables: PREVIOUSLY MARRIED 


735 


PREVIOUSLY MARRIED WHITE MALES 

160. Age 36-40. Educ. level 0-8. Protest., inactive. The relatively scant data 
available on this group do not show it significantly different from description 
given above (Group 159). 

161. Age 41-45. Whole group. Aging effects continue to mount; apparently 
6% of group without sexual outlet. Only 86% have intercourse, which accounts 
for 96% of their outlet, 60% have some of this with prostitutes, but this probably 
more true of lower educational level which is chief source of present sample. 
Only masturbate. Homosexual in 5%. 

162. Age 41-45. Educ. level 0-8. Whole group. Scant data available on this 
group do not show it significantly different from description given above (Group 
161). 

163. Age 46-50. Whole group. A continued decline in sexual activity evident 
in this group. Mean total outlet 1.4, with 6% of males apparently inactive. Only 
82% in limited sample with any intercourse, which contributes 96% of their out- 
let. 52% with some intercourse with prostitutes, which contributes of their 
total outlet — this a measure of their social maladjustments. Nearly of group 
masturbates, so deriving 3^ of their outlet. Homosexual too minor to record on 
this small sample. 



APPENDIX ON SAIVIPLE SIZE 


The problem involved in determining the number of cases necessary in 
the present study to provide an adequate sample of any larger population, 
has already been discussed in Chapter 3. The generalizations made there 
have been based upon a scries of pragmatic tests of the values of calcula- 
tions derived from samples of various sizes. The specific data derived from 
those tests are shown in the tables presented in this Appendix. 

As previously explained (Chapter 3), calculations have been made sys- 
tematically for samples of 50, 100, 200, 300, 400, and (where the material 
is available) 600, 1000, and still larger numbers of cases. In all, 698 different 
populations have been used in these tests. Each population has been 
homogeneous for sex, race, marital status, age, educational level, 
either the rural-urban background or the religious background of the 
included individuals. The samples of various size have all been selected 
by a strict randomization performed on an IBM sorter; and in each 
instance the sample has been drawn directly from the total number of 
histories available in each population. In no case has a larger sample been 
built up by adding cases to an originally smaller sample of 50, 100, or 
other size. 

In the tables given here, the statistics which are shown in boldface 
represent calculations which lie within 5 per cent, plus or minus, of the 
calculations obtained from the largest sample available in that group; 
except that comparisons of incidence data have been made with an allow- 
ance of 2 per cent, plus or minus. The boldface figures therefore represent 
results that are adequate, as judged by the results obtained from the use 
of the largest sample, and within the allowed range of error. Of course, 
an error of any other size might have been arbitrarily selected as a basis 
for judging adequacy in these tables. See Chapter 3 (Table 2) for a sum- 
mary tabulation of the number of adequate samples which are shown here 
in these tables. 

The eight tables which follow present the following statistics on each 
of the sample populations: 

Table 155: Mean frequencies per week (and the standard deviation) 
on the total population in each sample 
Table 156: Mean frequencies per week (and the standard deviation) 
for the active population in each sample 
Table 1 57 : Median frequencies per week for the total population in 
each sample 


736 



APPENDIX ON SAMPLE SIZE 


737 


Table 158: Median frequencies per week for the active population 
in each sample 

Table 159: Incidence of active cases in each sample 

Table 160: Locus of the mode for the frequency distribution in each 
sample 

Table 161 : Height of the mode for the frequency distribution in each 
sample 

Table 162: Range of variation (represented as the maximum fre- 
quency minus one case) in each sample 

Wherever all of the medians in an entire section of any table have fallen 
into a zero frequency class, they have not been shown in these tables. 

In column one, in each of the following tables, these abbreviations have 
been used: 

S = single 
M = married 
U = urban 

Px = Protestant, inactive 
PV = Protestant, devout 
Jx = Jewish, inactive 



N OF Total Population: Size of Adequate Sample 
Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes 



i-NlrOT-H r-lT-l»-l 


oot-'\0'0‘n*jnvorof^'— ^r^t^t^corO'^OooOr^^r:;;- 

ooooooc5ooooooooooooooc> 

cnf<^rocoC'io 4 f^c^fOcor 4 rofn<^cNr 4 (N<NCSro«NeN 


OC^C> 0 C 50 ^r)T}•fN'«^■r^■^ 

ddddoodddo 

forrinv©i^^vi>v«qt^'^, 

ddddr 4 drifsden 


d d d d 
-H -H -H -H 

rs On 00 »0 

o o Tir 
d d r 4 o 4 


Ti•0^r^O00^0^^^'s0»/^'v0'^0^^o^0^^r^m»--lO00T^•m 

dddddddddddddddddooooo 

Trf<i<^Trr-voio^^*ow}r^t:-^^ 3 'voint;' 5 ;oN <3 

dddddddddddddddr 4 r 4 n»Hcon<N 

vorlTfcnO\r' 0 '^OOOi-ivO«or'loo»o‘onoo»o^ 

<NCMr'l(N»-<'r^cNlcN<Nr4C4cnr^(N(N’-<»-H^^’rMrHr-jiF-H 

ddddddddddddddddddoooo 

<NC\r-<Nr^r^f^®\oo'sOMo\voMTfrfGooeoot^t^oo 
T— rjrOTfONt^'Ot^oeeST-jTtTHfnr-ONJOrjooTHt^io 

ddddddddddddddddddr-iddn 


eNcooNOvr>r 4 ^ 00 smm»— lCO'sD(NO^T^ 

«/->rnmmrno4c4mc'lcNCNcommr}*<NCSr-iT-^rn^CN4 

doddddddddddddoddddddd 

4nOrr‘O^OOrnf^rr)0»Of^>r)CO(NO>f^'tH»f)r-TH 

'Orn»nfN^‘QOopcOOC«OOOfSTj;(Nr' 4 '^’^Oi-;Jt>VO 

ddd" 5 i-ddo 4 dddc 4 (sddddddr 4 dc 4 c 4 

r^«nTl'‘na^oo'OvO'^i•a^^oO<Nmooc^vo^~^^^cnc<l 

r^cO'*^v^»ri«r)cncnfn<N<N'O's 0 TtcncNC 0 CS(N'^'^cn 

dddddddddddddddddddddd 

^-vo'0^oo©o^moov^:)r«■ocs(NO■^^o^m■rt<NTf 

r-T-HOvit><Noo«o«oppoOoop'^oO'«^oo'«toqoocor^ 

'^dddddddddddddricNCNCSt-HCNdd 


>rjO'oO'nO‘OO'OO‘n</-^O»o»r)O»OO'o»OOW 0 

111111111111111111111 ? 

T-H'O»-<'O^^r--<V0i--i'Or--<T-HV0T-HT-HV0r- ('0^r-|V0»-< 


Icnrofn^forncot^ 

tr— I 


m ^ vxx / i ^< nm ^ i ^^^ kx / ix / xx / i ^ x / i ^ Tjixfimx / xx/x 



Masturbation 


o^oo^'^fSO'0■'5■^^ooo^^^OO'^<ooo^^»— (O 
'<^'Tt‘o>/^r^t^ooTfri'r:i‘cov,0'>0‘n«ncn':r»/^o 

T— I ^ ^ 


pppOOr-^Or-tT-iT-HOOOOOO 

ooodddododdoodddddo 

^»OfSlom»ov-i^^oo■T^corta^(»ooo^r>vo 

t^pppr'ippi-Htoc?NOOf<»ooou^v-Hr{*os'^ 

^d<Nr-HeS’-^’-HT-3r-5oT-Hr4c4r-i^T-ir4r-4«-I 


o o o 

■H ^ ^ 

to 


o o o 
■H 41 -H 
r- ^ ^ 

p p 

r4 r4 *>H* 


l-t^>*r-lO^<NC^OOO^T-l^>- VDcnOOO <SIOO 

t-*0T-^0'-<^00T-<0 T-(r-Hr-<0 r-<r-lO 


oooooooooo oooo 

o^^oo^ooTfoe^^THOo t^vo'O'o 
t^porjpppppp 


o o o 
•H -H -H 


rJhoo^cnfsl'0»oc^»-tOoorJoo’^»-iosr''Omi-i 

ddddddddddddddddddd 

?-»iofOoop'/^'o»oasf^OfnoN*/)o*orrNTH 

ir^ppppppiHppppppTtpTrpTr 


roo^'^0'^ooor^■^r>vCl'00^^(S'OT^•(SO^^-tnf^ 

ddddddddddddddddddd 

fOVO^fnOOMOTfVOrof^FM^fniH^ovo-^ 

lopON'^’^pTrpppor'jopviT-jvooe'v 


ooooooooooooooooooo 

r^^>•ooT-^Tl-ooo■«toooo^^-»-lOO^/)r'0^o'0 


0^ 0^ ON 

1-4 o 1H r4 CM 


I ^ CN CM 1-1 


lomi-ti-icsooooNfnt^vo'O'ooNNooot^'ooN 
'i^’T-icncsmiriCMT-'cncMcorocMcMCMi— icncncM 

ddddddddddddddddddd 

Of*'cn‘nvo'^o<N»oo'Ooor-'i— lONOOOi-^oo 

cnONppppppppp'^pppppTr»r> 
CMOCS-i-^CSi-ii-^d-^cScMcMi— CMi-4iHr4cMi-4 


VN0«^0'00^0'00'nin0»n0‘nvn0v*i 
i-(CM»-^<Ni— ICMCMCOI-^CM-I— <i-iCMi-<CNcMi-<<NCM 

^^r-^r-<T-irHCM<N--ii-H*-Ki-<i-ii-(i-icMi“<i-iCM 


ooooJ^J^+ + + + oo._ 




ONOi-^OOOCMn-ONO 
ONOOeN(NONO’^r-MD 
'^Tt’»n»nt^r^oo-^Tt 
CM CM i-t 


i^rMcMcMi-H^THCMCM 

OOOOOOOOO 

d d d d d d d d d 

invoNOfn<c^cMONr-(r^ 
O^^P^POTS’cncni— ( 

ddddddddd 


o o o 
■H 41 -H 

IT) <M On 
n Tf cn 


^^f^NCM'^tcncncMcM 
o o o o o o o o o 
ddddddddd 

4)4141414141414141 

^VOVOCCit^^OOFHt^ 

pTHlHfMfn-itCOfniH 

dddoddddd 


1— leMcMrov-immcncM 
oooodoooo 
ddddddddd 

414141414141414141 

^t^ri-iTTlOrir-VNCh 

OvHi-iCMtl^Trt^C<ir-H 

dddddoodd 


poooooooo 

ddddddddd 

414141414141414141 

S 0ONO’^fC5r--00©0© 
, p 1-J CM P Tf p c<j tH 

ddodddoo© 


p o p o o O ' 

ddddddddd 

414141414141414141 

oomcMfMn^t^l>©rr 

Or-H-MCMICCJCninCClr-l 

ddddodood 


^»cNcMmT-<'0'o>r»Tj- 

pOOOr-HOOOp 

ddddddddd 

414141414141414141 


OOOOOOOOO 


tnO'oO'oOinov^ 

rjlCMrjlCMr7lC^r4COCM 


,vi ivA. 


t ^ CM CM CM 


MT 1 idcocncnro 

O O On On ri r— I T-S ' 


cocococococowwS 


739 


{Table continued on next page) 



Mean of Total Population: Size of Adequate Sample 
Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes 



'sDfoooosr^OO'<^‘‘^oor;rr^Q 

w-kTt'^Tt<^'sCi' 0 »niDmTt‘«oO 


p o p p p o p p p p p p p 
odoocJoooooooo 
•H-H-ll-H-ll-H-tl-H-fl-W-N-H-H 

vOTl'VO'^Tj'T-HTj-rfi— i<Smr'l'sO 
^r-ipT-lY-ITt'^COTf'^p'^p 

ddddddddddddd 


■H -H -H -H -H ■]] -11 

^ VO o lo a^ 

Tf ^ ^ 

odd© ©do 


OOOOOOOOOOppp 

ddddddddddddo 

■H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-fl-ll 

vomvOfOrfONt^V)^<^MV£)iO 

ddddddddddddo 


ooooooooooopp 

ddddddddddddd 

'r-<i-io»HT-(fnTrfn'^mcSfOf^ 

ddddddddddddd 


ooooooooooooo 
I ddddddddddddd 

r^vovoTffni-<rJiovvHo\voTi-^ 

'r-('.-i©i-<T-<’^'«fcS'^cncocnfn 

ddddddddddddd 


ooooooooooooo 

vofO‘oift»omO<>i^ooHr^oo 

FHr-iO»HvH<S'Tj-cnfncor)rr<r) 

ddddddddddddd 


0 ‘nv^O'oviovoO«n«no<n 

3223223^32 j!3!2 

<NrO»--tT-^T— I t— 


T-IT— (OOCJNt-lt-t^'rHr-HT-l,— IT-I 


| 3 pXi<X X X X 

4 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ <? 

.<»^C/2WIZlCOCOC(OWCOiZi£/l(/l 



S-U 0-8 16-20 1.40 ± 0.23 1.66 ± 0.27 1.58 ± 0.17 1.65 ± 0.14 1.76 ± 0.15 1.76 =fc 0.13 486 

S-U 9-12 11-15 0.25 ± 0.21 0.92 =»= 0.22 0.81 ± 0.14 0.87 ± O.M 0.88 ± 0.12 0.83 ± 0.10 521 

S-U 9-12 16-20 1.87 ± 0.51 1.67 ± 0.36 1.02 ± 0.12 1.09 ± 0.13 1.36 ± 0.14 1.34 ± 0.12 520 

S-U 13-t- 11-15 0.19 ± 0.14 0.15 i 0.10 0.04 ± 0.01 0.06 ± 0.02 0.05 ± 0.02 0.10 ± 0.03 0.07 ± 0.01 2708 

S-U 13-1- 16-20 0.27 =t 0.09 0.16 ± 0.05 0.26 ± 0.07 0.20 ± 0.03 0.29 ± 0.05 0.25 =*= 0.03 0.25 ± 0.02 2762 



Pre-marital Intercourse with Companions {continued) 


ooTi-TfTt'co' 0 'o»r>iofn-«^»n< 


ro^cNmcN^cncNfnrrr^C'lTr 
qq^, ^^r-joqqooooo 
o o o o o o o o o o o’ o o 

o^pfnT}-»-i^O'v£)'o^oo»nvo 

ooT^^dddddo’do’d 


Tj- 


fS m VN 

o 


o o o 

d 


d d d 

•H 


■H -H -H 

VO 


00 r-4 VO 



O tS rr 

d 


© o d 

r- 00 Tt VO 

m <S cn 

m »n 

O O r-K ^ 

o o o o 

O O O 

d d d d 

d d d d 

d d d 

■H -H -H ^ 

■H -H -H -H 

-H •« 41 

00 VO On fn 

^ VO Vl 

O VO m 

vN q i-j q 

O O rH 

^ Tf 

d d iH th 

d d d d 

d d d 


r't^CN 00 ’<;fT-HTtfn«/^«O'rJ-'s 0 r' 

OO^'.^^OOOOOOOO 

d d d d d d d d d d d d d 


OOOt-hOOOOOOOOO 


r^i-HTj’c 4 'Orotn<N'^sOroTl-t^ 

O^CSCS^OOOOOOOO 

d d d d d d d d d d d d d 

rfvooioOcn^Tf‘ti*r-(^<sm 

rtVO'^v< 5 ooO<SO’-H<Sq<N-«t 

d d ^ ^ d d d d d d d d d 


fSr-ioo<NOQrt-'r>i— ivo-^inO 

^cN'-^mcsOoO’-Hqqq^ 

d d d d d d d d d d d d d 


OOOv-HOOOOOOOOO 


m<N’-HOvo^'OoocNco<NaNr>- 

mm’^^T-tOOoqooqT-^ 

ddddddddddddd 

oovocoa\vof^v£)rt-roOfor4r^ 

r- 00 '^t-'^©T-(T-(O^q(NTj; 

od^rnddddddddd 


loOinOwoioO'oOuomO'/^ 

r<lrO’r-^(S^'>--(cS'rM(N€N^cS<S 


+ + 


00 OO 'tH 

<i C5 ON 



mmmcncnmrom 


X X X x; X 

xhdkdididiixhdidixhxhvim^ 


C3 

*C 

05 


O T-H 00 

VO VO m 


O ^ 00 

VO VO m 

Tf 


tT m rl- 

CnI T-H 


<N m 

r-H r~4 f-H 


o o o 

d d d 


d d d 

■H -H -H 


41 41 41 

m r- 


00 i~i r- 

r-. fo 


O ^ -TH 

d c4 r4 


d d d 




fo 


m m 

t— < T-H 


O O 

d d 


d d 

41 41 


41 41 

m 00 


00 tH 

'i-i 


© 

d r4 


© © 

VN (N 


m m 
o o o 



d d d 

(1> 

d d d 

41 41 41 

So 

41 41 41 

TH ^ 1/) 

o 

© © 

m 


O th T-t 

cN r4 r4 

B 

d © d 


C3 





On W 

<— 1 

m m 


CC 

o o o 

d d d 

'C 

d d d 

41 41 41 

C5 

s 

41 41 41 

lO Tl- 

c5j 

ON© 

rj © in 

u 


d f4 cN 

w 

d © © 


1—1 


CO CS (S 

c5 

<N m m 

<N O'! (N 

O 

o o o 

d d d 

H 

d d d 

41 41 41 


41 41 41 

©mm 


m m 

iH Tf f'J 


o ^ m 

d c4 d 


d d d 

m VO m 


© r- 00 

m (N (N 


o o o 

d d d 


d d d 

41 41 41 


il 41 41 

00 © m 


O m © 

00 


»— 1 1— t 'pH 

<N d d 


d d © 

mom 


mom 

Cji m m 


N m m 

^ VO »-H 


^ VO 

cs <N m 


C4 <N m 

4" + 4" 


+ + + 

m m m 


m m m 






:3p;d 

sis 


SSS 


741 


{Table continued on next page) 



Mean of Total Population: Size of Adequate Sample 
Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes 



fNj (S ^ ^ T-( 


qooooooqoqqpqqqqqqq 

oooooooooooocSoooooo 

csr4fori-ooo(Sc4tNqqqqqp^qq 

doooododoooocJoooooo 


fSr^c<><N 

ooooooooo oooo qqo 

d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d 

'Or^C^QOOO»/^^<^^coa^ 'sO^Or^ <nono 

cNcNcNrfOO’-<«NCSTH OO^q 

d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d 

VD'^V£>aNCnfNI<N«OtO'^OnmfO(N<N^^^*^ 

qoooooooooqooooqoqo 
d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d 

»cxD(NooO‘nvOT-<r<i©»n^vorx\omTHV;;)T-< 

r|cSf0«n^oO(NcSM<Noqooqf^q^ 
o d d d d d d d d © d d d d © d d d d 


r-*vn'ooofOfNm\ovov-)'ntr 4 r'ir 4 Ti*'r-*m»r»'ri* 

oooooooooooooooqqqo 

ddddddddddddddddddd 

®\^oaNU^mr-'gNV 5 *^t--ON^^^ 2 \r:ji-if^o 

r4M(Nrfr-^©©rJic<i^»-HOOOOOiH^^ 

ddddddddddddddddddd 


0 >oto^'<-<mvnoov->ooOoow^Tj*c 4 ''~ifni--ifr) 

^Or-KOlOOOOOOr-tOOOOOOOO 

ddddddddddddddddddd 


oo©oooooooooooooooo 


ooooooooooooooooooo 

tn'^'TtcvJOOOfSro^mOOi— 'OOOr-1^ 
d d d d d d d o’ d d d d d d d d d d d 


ir>0'no«oo»nOv^O‘o»oO‘nO'n'OOv^ 

»^cs^cNr^cNolro^c 4 >pHi~<rsiT-M<scSr-icSr^ 


ooooS ^+ + + + c 


■<0 0 0 \ T-Ml— (T-^T— <r— 


c/sajc^JooOTwcowoicowoacowwc/icooooo 


742 


Table 155. Size of sample necessary to secure stable means on total population 

Boldface figures designate calculations that lie within 5 per cent, plus or minus, of the calculations obtained from the largest sample. Means are fre- 
quencies per week. 




Mean of Active Population: Size of Adequate Sample 



c^oor^^f^JOvO'^^~'' 0 'Ofoco< 7 ^t^OO^JQoo^^O 

04 <s ^ ^ 


Cj\|>V0vx?'O«O'Of0f0»-^0'l00t^0N<^'^O00O00^^ 
1 — It-It-H^OOOT— IT-Ir-HT— Ir-Hy— JOOO 

dodddddddddoddddoooooo 

•H-N-H-H-ll-H-H-H-tl-H-tl-fl-H-n-H-H-H-H-H-H'H'll 

r4<r^vor^^-ooor'r^^ooc^^'V^^2oOMr4r'00^ 

v£)lr 5 VO'OO^t^^o^^f^o^^ 4 f^•QOO^^coO'rHco'sO 

dcorndoao4c4r4ddo4cndcncoo4r4o4o4cno4<N 


o o o 
•H -H -H 

•«!*• M VO 
o OS »o 
d 04 r 4 


dddddddddd 

■H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H 

f 0 ) 00 r^ 0404 OTH 0 -t^^ 

voTr' 0 'ooN^vov£?r>rr 

ddddo 4 do 4 oldd 


^ ^ o 

d d d d 
•H -H -H 

On 00 »0 
tH O lo fO 


o o o 
■H -H -H 

VO 00 rp 

04 00 VO 


voO 04 O 00 'O 04 V 0 i 0 NNOTpOmT-^ 0 N' 0 Nm'f-^OCJN'^fO 

o 4 r 4 r 4 o 4 ^»-H',~(^^rHT-io 4 r 4 o 4 ^''-^’-H^'^'^>-^’-; 

dddddddd odd d dddddddddd 

T-tCN»Dt-*oovoiniH'vDiDino-©©©rri>v©o-io>a\;^ 

voiQ'oinoNi^io'OinfnoaN^«/)©ON'oo 4 ©fn©vo 

ddddo 4 o 4 f 4 o 4 dddo 4 dddo 4 o 4 o 4 FHm‘oio 4 


ooo 4 xtmOr^O'^OOOo 40 'VOrocO»oioo 4 ooinvo 

0404 r 404 C 4 '.~i 0404 r 40404 mr 4 CSr 4 i-Ht— II— 

d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d 

coTrooo 4 iHt^fr)Ovoo©oiTto 40 '' 0 '<i-o©©fno^oo 

fnfnrf*5tfHt^'Ot'.cser4i-jvooj'^ooa\r^o4oqo4t^iO 

cncnddHo 4 o 4 o 4 dddfOfndf^io 4 olo^^*do 4 o 4 


oooooooooooooooooooooo 

v 00 -f 0 O^o 4 Ornf 0 roOm© 0 ~ 04 r 4 i/) 0 ^fn 00 T-(TH 

O 03 t-;C 4 r 4 «QC 0 q 0 qO 00 p 04 ©^ 04 VOTj;©r 4 C~^vo 

•>!4^ddTpcnr4c4dddcSrndd«rJrno4o4o4rnc4oi 

vo»nvx?»r>oco'sO^''^o\mr4r4»oooaN\Dr~i>oomfn 

r^cnTp«o>'©iocommo 4 c 4 vO' 0 ' 5 prnr 4 mr 4 o 4 ^J-'^cn 

d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d 

oovoinrr'^o©«/)oo'sor^'«^r 4 t^o^rt-c 7 NO'Ooo 4 Tf 

T-jr-iin»>cooo'qinq'sqGOoq'^oo'^0'^ooo\cnt^ 

<ocnrodcoo 4 o 404 cnfno 4 '^'ddo 4 o 4 cnc 4 ^o 4 do 4 


(nO»oO*^O»nO‘OO‘ 0 N‘nOw^vnO‘nO‘o»oOv 0 

r-iNO*-HVOi-HV£)— I' sO»--I^»-Hv-hVO»— I— 1 ^— 
^^T-i^^^r 4 r 404 rNim— Hr-(,-H^r-(T-i>-io 4 ^i — 104 


'■h + oooo^ ■+■ + + + + + + + 


co»:(Ziwc/ 2 (/ic/ 2 c/ 2 lSj<Sc/ 2 oaiy 3 c/ 2 i> 2 Wc>ai/i{>ocA!/i 


744 




o^oo^sr^^0'0r^^^ooo^^'00’^«ooo^-»^0 
TtTt‘otor^t^oO'^Tt‘^fn'0\o»rj«ocorf‘00 
r4 cs »-i ^ ^ 


ovo^oco<n:$ono 

ONOo<N(NOvO'«rr^vo 
Tl*rt»o»oi>r^c>0'^Tt 
cvi ri r-i 


r-|0^00000^0^^^000000 


cnfn»r)rnrJ|T-'rN|<NfNj 

poooopppp 


ddddddddddddddddddd 


ddddddddd 




-H-H-Hii-H-fl-H-W-H 


ooc^'>5tO(Noo»/^r^cyNOcocr>ooro»n<Nfom^ 

aN^-H<Ntor^o»nrnr^^oaNfomr'COooT-H'o 


or-oocNONr^i/^r^vo 

cScScncn-^'^f'^pcN 


T— * t-h‘ fs c4 r4 r-H* ^ r-J <Ni r4 cN r4 ^ CM cs T-H 


ddddddddd 


<N ON 00 O ON r- 

r-H O O T -1 O p 


Tt- 

o o p 


d d d d d d 


d d d 


•H -H -H -H -H -H 


•H 41 -H 


On «/N ri VO ^ 00 

r- ON irj VO 


VO IT) 

Tf TT 


r4 rH* iH fs r-5 


odd 


^g^OfncogO<N^ oOTt-Og *^22 


ooooooppp 


d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d 


ddddddddd 






ONOorrooTf-t^r^THfn ONfoooo\ rjoo'o 

ONpp 


eNvoor^oONfnt^»<o 


THTHr4THr4(NT-KfHTHT-^ cnrirlvH 


ddddddddd 


vobNrfror--r^O(NOaNfST-HmcNOoooomc^ 
r— iT—i^OT-HCS^r-tT-HO^'^'^ 


T}*fO>nTtr^'?f'^foro 

o p p o p p P P P 


ddddddddddddddddddd 


ddddddddd 


-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H 




^<NTHi^u^T-HOt^oor-ooTrn-vooovoT-.t^t^ 

p(NMl/)t^CN'^C*^WnpC?NONC'J^pr^p'rtlO 

M 

r^xo<<)or:-2pp 

rHCJIcOfOinTr-^TtCN 


r4^r4iHf4<N^^’-^t-5^rlcNc4THTHcor4T-5 

P 

o 

dddddddoo 

CJ 




,0 


a 

fNVNON'^vovnVNm*^ 

ooooooppp 

X) 

V-i 

d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d 


ddddddddd 

P 


1 i 


KJ 

s 

00<^OVOOOCN«/^^000»nVOVOTf«fHt^ONVO 

1 



»-^F^r4i-Kr4<S’<-^’-HT-JiHr4<Sc4<NTHiHfS|r-H'(H 

0 

ddddddddd 


OvovNOoO'--HOt^‘ni>OON^O«nf0^oo<r) 

rOr-HCMcnmcJlcN^cN^-icSTt-cScNT^iT-Mroi-HtN 


^oooopppp 


d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d 


d d d d d d d d d 






rooC'fHONr-fovooNt^oovo'^»r)i«-<ONmoooov) 
cNTHfn‘ooopr^r<^«r-|ONpfnTrpfop©'p i 




<NiHfNi»^r4^r-^THTHT-<^dc4c4’-^THcoc4tH 


1 ddddddooo 


ONVDcocNr4<Sr-Hovor'iooooN^r^omr-m 

Tj-r-i(ncNvovoeN<Nmfn'^ro<smcN<Nr|-mcn 


mr^‘^»OTi'VOvovovo 

OOOO^pppp 


d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d 


ddddddddo 


'•H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H-H 




T-HiOOOinOOfHOOOOOOOOOTfr^VOirJrO'F-iOO 

pp'^p<NFHCNp^THTtoOp‘Op«SppOO 


Sa2SKP;9?5S 


c^^^<;^^^(ric4^dc4?-5r4^^^4r4<NTHr4c^i^ 


ddddddddd 


inO‘oO‘nO'nO‘nO‘o«nO«oo»ovNO‘o 

23i3l323j;3jii3l3!!!j!3I2 


«no»oo»nO»oO'o 

T-ltNr-lCNjt-^C^C^fnCS 

jHvir-HviJnvijiviJj 

ICSCSCS 


oooo2^+ + + + oooo2+ + + + + + + + 
j| 1 Icococnmjl icocomcncnrocom 


2 2 _|. _j_ ^ «j_ _|> 

1 I’Tnrcocncncncn 
ddONONT-H^t-*^^ 


dixhmdivixhdididixhdidiKhdidididididi 


tpDCp9DSJ?9 J 

iZJWi/ii>owaic/5aii< 


745 


(Table continued on next page) 



Mean of Active Population: Size of Adequate Sample 

Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes cases 



'<0fnc00>l^OOrfW-)00^^O 

m'<4*'!tTl'mvj0vD>o»ncori-v^O 


ooooooooooooo 

ddddddddodddd 

-H-H-H-fl-H-H-H-N-H-Hfl-tl-H 

1— 1»— »<S'0‘r>n'-r-HOOu^i^oow^c^ 

ddddddddodddd 


o o o o 
•H -H ^1 


o o o o o o p q q q q q q 
ddddddddddddd 

©dddddddddodo 

cNrOTi’Tt'sOvO'omwn'^mtnm 

oooooooqqqqqq 

ddddddddooooo 


ooooooooooooo 

cnfnr^r~OoovO”^'r>'jDO«r)»o 

OOOOt-^OOOOO'-^OO 

ddddddddddddd 

«sr4i<Nr'4fO*n^'^'^Tr»ofOf<) 

ddddddddddddd 


00 ^ 000 »-^ 0 r-. 0 »~H^ 

ddddddddddddd 


ddddddddddddd 


O‘n»nO‘n>nO»nOw-j0-)O‘o 

3223123232222 


^^3 3 2 




<N O O 
CN oo q 
cn f4 


S-U 9-12 16-20 2.40 =b 0.63 2.35 ± 0.49 1.39 =fc 0.15 1.68 ± 0.18 1.92 ± 0.19 1.91 ± 0.16 520 

S-U 13+ 11-15 3.22 ± 1.81 1.48 ± 0.92 0.38 ± 0.09 0.77 ± 0.20 0.57 ± 0.17 1.06 ± 0.32 0.81 ± 0.11 2708 

S-U 13+ 16-20 0.63 =t 0.19 0.53 ± 0.14 0.66 ± 0.17 0.53 ± 0.07 0.70 ± 0.12 0.61 ± 0.08 0.64 ± 0.04 2762 



Pre-marital latercourse with Companions {continm 


■^t^oooNr~-oOTi-inoor^T-i< 


v-jT-HOunroinvT'^t-i,— 

O-r-^CS^tS^OtS^r-t^OO 

d d d d d d d d d d c5 d d 

r-vovor<io0'^o0's0'0'«:t'^r<ioo 

O0'^>-^00^fnr^•00'0'^000'^0^^ 

dT^cNcit-^dddddddd 



VO 

OV 

o 

rM O 

o 

d 

d d 

d 

41 

41 41 

41 

m 

CO <N 

o 

00 


00 

d 

d d 



r-H fS rt- 00 
^ ^ <N 'th 

d d d d 
•H 41 -H -H 

CO Os • 
0^ r4 


o^ Tj- m 

O o <N 
d d d d 

41 41 41 41 

^ ^ VO 
fS lO 00 VO 


di-^rif4 dddd 


ov ov 

CN O O 

d d d 

41 41 41 

OS l> 
oo VO r«. 

d d d 


T-Hr-ifNioi/^TtoooooomaNCviT-i 

»-<r-^cN<Nr4»-^0(NT-HT-(rn^-r-s 

ddddddddddddd 

41414141414141414141414141 


tNmONf^f^Ovfnt-xoo^^vocNm 

0vO00OO<N‘oa\00V0O0000 

dr-lr-^cicidddddr-^dd 


t-hon’— ivnvncN»-^T-imr^mr^<N 
T-t^'^CMm»OT-HCSST-lT-((SO'^ 

ddddddddddddd 


ro fO VO IT) 0^ 
i> oj vq ON 00 
d ^ c4 1-5 T-i 


vn-r- icsovov^r^oo 
vo»nvovovovn'^t^ 

d d d d d d d d 


co 


00 lo 00 VO m 
^ oo <N CO m 

d d d d d 

41 41 41 41 41 


CO CO CO <N 

O »n O t-H 

d d d d d d d 

■B -H -H •« -H 


<Nr^coT-ifo»o»OTj'«OTfvovoco 

0^‘^voo^ooO'^cN''— ‘o<N»ooq 

o^^^^odr-i^dddd 


ooi>'^ooinvo<ov-ioovocS’r-Mvo 

vovnvovo'^O’-jvoO'^’-jcScN 

ddddddddddddd 

41414141414141414141414141 

''^l/^t^OOVOlOTfOmTOVq'rH'? 

COVO'^fOVO^'^rt’r-jVqcNvqt-; 

T-4^c4r4^*d dr-5 d dddd 


23 i 3 Il 3 l 32 :i 32 


(N CS rH rH ^ 


< T-i »-( <N r-i t-( CN 


cocolj |COCOCOCOCOCOCO( 

'i--lT-(OOOVrHr-4t--«r--(T-(r-<-r-ii 


czjc^wcociococ/acowc^aiwoa 


O rH 00 


O T-< 00 


O VO rH 

VO VO CO 


VO VO CO 


ON 00 CN 

rf •rj Tt 


rj- m Ti* 


Tt WO 

CN rl rH 


CO 00 00 


^ o o 

T— 1 1—4 K-H 


1-H O O 


T— 1 ■*— ( l-H 

d d d 


d d d 


d d d 

41 41 41 


41 41 41 


41 41 41 

<N 00 


o 


Tt wo r- 

T-j r- fo 


Tt vn 


O OO CO 

CO CN CN 


d d d 


^ d d 






-<1* CO 


wo o 


CO O O 

r-H r-H 


T-H 1-H 


y-^ T— ( T— ( 

d d 


d d 


odd 

41 41 


41 41 


41 41 41 

VO th 


O t-' 


Tf r- t-h 

tH 00 


wo Tj- 


© OO 

CO r4 


d d 


odd 

«o m CN 


ON ^ ^ 


O r-( wo 

^ 




CN ^ rH 

odd 

<D 

d d d 


odd 

41 41 41 

P 

41 41 41 


41 41 41 

CO VO VO 

1 o 

a 

rf Tt CO 


a\ rr iTi 

l> CO 

1 h 

w^ -cr «/} 

■*-> 

O 00 00 

CN ri CN 

P 

odd 

o 

'S 

r-5 d d 




0 


Ov W 

*CQ 

r-* o i 


wo T — 1 '<dh 



CO r-^ r-t 1 

’a 

CN -rH rH 

d d d 

a 

d d d 

p 

odd 

41 41 41 

41 41 41 

§ 

41 41 41 

r*^ ov »o 

p 

VO VO o 

o 

a 

O r- On 

CN vq uo 1 


VO >/i 

r-H C-- 

CO CN CN 

tc 

PJ 

o’ d d 

o 

K 

^ d d 



CO OO 



OO CN CN 

rt 


OO r^ 

CN CN CN 

O 

r— 1 rH T— 1 


CO ^ Tt" 

d d d 

H 

odd 


d d d 

41 41 41 


41 41 41 


41 41 41 

Ol VO 1^ 


OO OO OO 



tH ^ CN 


CN wo rr 


CO CN CN 


d d © 


»-5 c> 

CO VO CO 


CN 00 r- 


T-i ^ r-* 

CO CN CN 


CN CN cN 


Tf CO CO 

d d d 


d d d 


odd 

41 41 41 


41 41 41 


41 41 41 

00 o m 


wo O cO 


T-^ VO wo 

00 CN 


wo VO VO 


wo r-H CN 

CN CN CN 


d d d 


^ ^ ^ 

«0 O 


w^ O wo 


wo O wo 

CNCOCO 


r- 1 VO 


7 ‘?’T 

t-h VO ^ 




CN CN CO 


cN CN CO 



+ + H“ 


+ + + 


CN 

CO CO T-^ 

CO CO CO 


CO CO CO 


ci <i Orv 





PDP 

sss 




x/ivah 


747 


{Table continued on next page) 



Mean of Active Population: Size of Adequate Sample 

Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes cases 



o 

00 

fN tF ON 


cn 





t-r rj- 

rH 

CO 


CNj 

O VO 


r- 

00 

Ov 

r- o 

O rf 

wo 

00 




»o 


r-- 

00 

rt Tf rjh cn 

VO 

VO 

WV 

wo 

CO 


wo 


CN 

(N 

T-f 
















O VO 

CO 

<N O 

O c<v 

00 

On 

rH 


wo 

CO 


O 

o 

r— ( 

k-H 


1-H 


r-1 

f-H 

o 

O 

CN 

o 

O 

rH 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o o o 

d 

o o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

■H 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 


rO 

fO 

O' 

On 


rO 

T-l 


wo 

00 

r' 

CO 

rH 

Ov 

ov 

O 

'sT 

00 

cn 

Ov 

00 

00 

NT 

wo 

CO 

CO 

VO 


CO 

r'- 


O 

d 

d 


o o o 

d 

O O 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 


00 

o 

00 










VO 

Ov 

© 


o 












o 

o 



d 

d 

d 










d 

d 

d 


41 

41 

41 










41 

41 

41 


00 

rH 











VO 

ov 

rn 


TT 

rr 

r- 












00 


d 

d 

d 










o o 

© 

cn 

VO 


cn 



CN 



CO 

O 



00 O 

00 


o 

o 

CN 


r— 1 



T— 1 

rH 

th 

rH 


o 

t— 1 

rH 

d 

d 

d 

o o o 

d 


d 

d 

d 

d 


d 

d 

d 

■H 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 


41 

41 

41 

41 


41 

41 

41 


VO 

ov 

rh VO 

VO 

VO 


cn 

00 

VO 

o 


wo 

'it 

£N 

q 

m 


OV 

tN 

Cv 




-N* 'N* 


rr rf 

00 


d 

d 

d 


d 

d 


d 

O 

o 

o 


o 

o 

© 

r- 

o 


Cv ^ 

CO 

rf (N VO 

OV 

r- 

rt 

CO 

00 


CN 



T— ( 


<N 

r— 1 


r-H 

t-h 

H-i 

o 

rH 

rH 

© o 

CO 

d 

d 

d 

d 

O O 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o 

O 

d 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

00 

m VO 

00 


© 

1-1 VO 


CO 

CN 

U2 


CO CO 


CN 

rf CO 

VO 

o 

X 


VO 

rl- 

CO 

ro Tt* 

TT 

CO 

q 


d d 

O F-l 


d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

© d 

© 

d 

hH 

CO 



OO VO O 

Cv 


r-H 

CO 

ov 

CO 

wo 

O CO 

, 


l-H 

t-h 

CN 


CN 


rH 

1—1 

r— 1 

o 

CN 

hH 

1— ( 

CN 

CO 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 


fn 


ov in 

o\ 

CO 


VO 

CN 

Ov 


© 

? 


© 

q 

rr wn 

00 rr 

0\ 

VO 

VO 

<N 

VO 

CN 

WO 

CO 

© 

p- 


© d 

o 


© 

d 

d 

d 

d 

o o o 

d 

d 

o 

tT 

»n o 

VO 

00 

T-H 

cn 

ov 

<o 

VO 



OO 

o 

■t- 

CO 

Tf 

O CN rj- 

CsJ 

CN 

cn CN 




O 

rH 


o 

TH 

d 

d d d 

d 

o o 

o 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

VO 

OO (N 

vr> 

r-H 

rt VO 

m 

y~i 

r-- 


CO 

VO 

CO 

rl- 

CO 



»o 

r- 

<N 

VO O 

q 

OV 

CN 


CN 

CN 

CO 


wo 


d 

o o 


o 



d 

r-H 

d 

o o 

d 

d 

d 

o t- 

t^ 

VO 


VO 

cn 


!>- 

CN 

TF O 

Tt- 


o 


V— 1 


o -t 

m 

CN 

<N 


O 

CO 

o 

CO 

o 

•t- 

CO 

o o 

d 

d d 

O O 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 

41 


00 

wn O 


CN 

cn 

VO 

t- 




CO 

wo 

CO 

ov 

lO 

(N 

CO 

(N 

(N 

1-H 

VO 

OV 

CO 

r— 1 


rH 


rH 

VO 

VO 

d 

d 

d 

d 

T-l 


O O 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

O 

o 

lo 

o 


O 

*0 

>0 

o 

wo 

o 

wo 

wv 

o 

wo 


1 


7 

CO 

1 


7 


'-p 



7 

CN 

rH 



VO 

I 

VO 


VO 

T--I 

VO 

rH 

JL, 

VO 

tH 

VO 

rH 

rH 


1 




(N 

CN 





rH 

tH 


CN 



CN 

<N 

+ + + + 

00 

OO 

CN 

++++++++ 

T 

Ov 

ro 


m 

cn 

1 

1 

T 

CO 

m 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CO 

CO 





O 

o 

ov 


rH 








X 

pH 

X 

i-s 

X 

h-j 

>> 
e. fu 

> 

XXX 

PLi Ph CU| 

CO 

CO 

00 CO 

CO CO CO 

CO CO CO 

C/2 

C/3 GO 00 c/2 CO 


748 


Table 156. Size of sample necessary to secure stable means, on active population 

Boldface figures designate calculations that lie within 5 per cent, plus or minus, of the calculations obtained from the largest sample. Means are fre- 
quencies per week. 




GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

AGE 

Median of Total Population: Size of 
Adequate Sample. Calculations with 
Samples of Various Sizes 

CASES IN 

LARGEST 



i 

50 

100 

200 300 400 600 1000 

■ largest 

sample 

SAMPLE 

Total Outlet 

S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

2.94 

1.93 

2.02 1.87 1.88 

2.03 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

2.46 1 

2.29 

2.33 2.54 2.30 

2.37 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

2.13 1 

2.47 

2.55 2.47 2.53 

2.49 

521 

S-U 

9-12 

16-20 

2.65 

2.47 

2.81 2.75 2.66 

2.73 

520 

S-U ! 

13+ 

11-15 

2.42 

2.33 

2.48 2.19 2.22 2.22 2.25 

2.21 

2708 

S-U 

13+ 

16-20 

1.85 

2.02 

2.12 2.26 2.48 2.07 2.24 

2.19 

2762 

S-U ; 

13+ 

21-25 

2.04 

2.47 

2.14 1.95 1.99 1.86 2.07 

1.95 

1844 

S-U 

13+ 

26-30 

1.94 

2.19 

1.75 1.90 1.91 

1.92 

479 

M-U 1 

13+ 

21-25 

2.88 

3.38 1 

3.26 2.98 3.14 

3.11 

460 

M-U 

13+ 

26-30 

3.04 

2.48 i 

2.68 2.72 2.77 

2.74 

561 

M-U 

13+ 

31-35 

2.31 

2.40 

2.35 2.47 

2.33 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

2.75 i 

2.03 

2.02 1.60 1.88 

1.78 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

2.03 

2.25 

2.14 2.23 2.32 

2.24 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

2.55 

2.41 

2.25 2.26 

2.33 

^ 375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

1.88 

2.19 

2.11 2.03 2.30 

2.17 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

2.08 

2.63 

2.30 2.28 2.34 i 

2.30 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

2.21 

2.11 

2.22 2.08 1.93 

1.98 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

1.96 

1.93 

1.79 1.74 1.86 

1.83 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

1.16 

1.61 

1.36 1.43 

1.43 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

2.11 

2.29 

2.75 2.34 2.58 2.45 2.55 

2.45 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

2.56 

2.29 

2.27 2.41 2.26 2.39 2.40 

2.36 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

2.54 

1.94 ’ 

1.99 1.98 1.98 1.91 1.99 

1.99 

1000 


Masturbation 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

1.25 

1.03 

0.82 

0.81 

0.92 



0.93 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

0.62 

0.42 

0.48 

0.50 

0.43 



0.44 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

1.46 

1.16 

1.28 

1.29 

1.27 



1.30 

521 

S-U : 

9-12 

16-20 

0.59 

0.69 

0.71 

0.69 

0.67 



0.71 

520 

S-U 

13+ 

11-15 

1.69 

1.67 

1.78 

1.67 

1.61 

1.63 

1.67 

1.60 

2708 

S-U 

13+ 

16-20 

0.91 

1.17 

1.24 

1.26 

1.51 

1.10 

1.19 

1.23 

2762 

S-U 

13+ 

21-25 

0.47 

0.89 

0.71 

0.69 

0.69 

0.69 

0.70 

0.68 

1844 

S-U 

13+ 

26-30 

0.41 

0.59 

0.45 

0.49 

0.52 



0.49 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

1.32 

0.94 

1.01 

0.87 

0.91 



0.89 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

0.33 ! 

0.45 

0.47 

0.45 

0.49 



0.46 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

1.15 

1.10 

1.14 

1.10 




1.13 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 ‘ 

1,75 

1.72 

1.63 

1.47 

1.72 



1.58 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

1.13 

1.71 

1.53 

1.27 

1.55 



1.41 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

1.64 

1.55 

1.49 

1.52 

1.37 



1.42 

547 

s-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

1.28 

1.25 

0.97 

0.90 

1.09 



1.00 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

0.53 

0.74 

0.65 

0.58 




0.60 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

1.66 

1.89 

2.14 

1.73 

1.88 

1.78 

1.90 

1.82 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

1.88 

1.42 

1.21 

1.37 

1.39 

1.58 

1.39 

1.41 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

0.64 

0‘.72 

0.75 

0.74 

0.83 

0.76 


0.80 

1000 


Nocturnal Emissions 


S-U 0-8 11-15 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 490 

S-U 0-8 16-20 0.05 0.01 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.03 486 

S'U 9-12 11-15 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 521 

S-U 9-12 16-20 0.04 0.07 0.08 0.07 0.07 0.06 520 

S-U 13+ 11-15 0.22 0.12 0.11 0.10 0.10 0.11 0.10 0.11 2708 

{Table continued on next pa^c) 


750 


GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

AGE 

Median of Total Population: Size of 
Adequate Sample. Calculations with 
Samples of Various Sizes 

CASES IN 

LARGEST 

SAMPLE 

1 

50 

100 

200 

300 

400 

o^ 

o 

o 

1000 

largest 

SAMPLE 

Nocturnal Emissions {continued) 

S-U 

13 + 

16-20 

0.24 

0.22 

0.25 

0.25 

0.25 

0.25 

0.25 

0.25 

2762 

s-u 

13 + 

21-25 

0.27 

0.24 

0.24 

0.22 

0.20 

0.22 

0.23 

0.22 

1844 

S-U 

13 + 

26-30 

0.13 

0.17 

0.16 

0.20 

0.17 



0.18 

479 

M-U 

13 + 

21-25 

0.04 

0.04 

0.04 

0.05 

0.05 



0.05 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

0.06 

0.07 

0.04 

0.05 

0.06 



0.06 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

0.05 

0.06 

0.04 

0.05 




0.05 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 



0.00 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

0.00 

0.02 

0.02 

0.02 

0.03 



0.02 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 




0.00 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

0.08 

0.12 

0.10 

0.17 

0.12 



0.15 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

0.22 

0.27 

0.23 

0.26 

0.26 



0.24 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

0.14 

0.16 

0.10 

0.14 

0.12 



0.12 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

0.25 

0.27 

0.23 

0.26 

0.26 



0.27 

557 

S-P-v/ 

13+ 

21-25 

0.21 

0.17 

0.23 

0.26 




0.26 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

0.11 

0.12 

0.07 

0.09 

0.10 

0.09 

0.09 

0.09 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

0.17 

0.21 

0.24 

0.21 

0.25 

0.23 

0.24 

0.24 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

0.15 

0.21 

0.18 

0.20 

0.20 

0.20 

0.20 

‘ 0.20 

1000 


Pre-marital Intercourse with Companions 


s-u 

0-8 

11-15 

0.63 

0.00 

0.07 

0.06 

0.00 



0.02 

490 

s-u 

0-8 

16-20 

0.88 

0.74 

0.72 

0.68 

0.69 



0.69 

486 

s-u 

9-12 

11-15 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 



0.00 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

0.52 

0.41 

0.34 

0.27 

0.35 



0.34 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

0,00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

0.06 

0.06 

0.06 

0.07 

0.06 

0.03 

0.05 

0.04 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

0.04 

0.05 

0.03 

0.09 

0.06 



0.08 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

0.22 

0.09 

0.08 

0.00 

0.05 



0.06 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

0.63 

0.73 

0.51 

0.70 

0.77 



0.70 

493 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

0,00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 



0.00 

60 l 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 1 

0.00 i 



0.00 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 



0.00 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

0.00 

io.oo 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 



0.00 

557 

S-Px i 

13+ 

11-15 

0,00 

10.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 i 

0.00 i 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

0.00 

0,00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

0.06 

0.03 

0.02 

0.05 

0.04 

0.04 

0.05 

0.05 

1000 

Marital Intercourse 


M-U 

13 + 

21-25 

2.36 

2.58 

2.77 

2.41 

2.59 



2.57 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

2.46 

1,99 

2.22 

2.20 

2.22 



2.19 

561 

M-U 

13+ 

31-35 

1.92 

1.91 

1.92 

1.91 

• 



1.88 

438 



Total Extra-marital Intercourse 

M-U 

13 + 

21-25 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 



0.00 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 



0.00 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 

0.00 




0.00 

438 


Table 157. Size of sample necessary to secure stable medians on total population 


Boldface figures designate calculations that lie within 5 per cent, plus or minus, of the 
calculations obtained from the largest sample. Medians are frequencies per week. 

751 



GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

AGE 

Median of Actt\^ Population: Size of 
Adequate Sample. Calculations with 
Samples of Various Sizes 

CASES IN 

LARGEST 



50 

100 1 200 

300 

400 

1 

600 

1000 

LARGEST 

SAMPLE 

SAMPLE 


Total Outlet 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

3.38 

2.22 

2.31 

2.22 

2.26 



2.36 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

2.46 

2.33 

2.37 

2.60 

2.36 



2.44 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

2.63 

2,67 

2.61 

2.56 

2.63 



2.61 

521 

S-U 

9-12 

16-20 

2.65 

2.47 

2.81 

2.77 

2.67 



2.74 

520 

S-U 

13 + 

11-15 

2.50 

2.40 

2.63 

2.29 

2.37 

2.35 

2.35 

2.34 

2708 

S-U 

13 + 

16-20 

1.85 

2.05 

2.12 

2.26 

2.48 

2.07 

2.25 

2.20 

2762 

S-U 

13 + 

21-25 

2.04 

2.47 

2.14 

1.95 

1.99 

1.86 

2.08 

1.95 

1844 

S-U 

13 + 

26-30 

1.94 

2.19 

1.75 

1.90 

1.91 



1.92 

479 

M-U 

13 + 

21-25 

2.88 

3.38 

3.26 

2.98 

3.14 



3.11 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

3.04 

2.48 

2.68 

2.72 

2.77 



2.74 

561 

M-U 

13+ 

31-35 

2.31 

2.40 

2.35 

2.47 




2.33 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

3.10 

2.18 

2.15 

1.88 

2.13 



2.03 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

2.03 

2,29 

2.19 

2.26 

2.37 



2.29 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

2.80 

2.58 

2.38 i 

2.40 




2.48 

^ 375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

1.88 

2.31 

2.21 

2.12 

2.39 



2.29 

601 

S-Jx 

13 + 

16-20 

2.08 

2.63 

2.30 

2.28 

2.34 



2.30 

607 

S-PV 

13 + 

11-15 

2.33 

2.32 

2.34 

2.18 

2.02 



2.10 

547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 

1.96 

1.93 

1.80 

1.76 

1.86 



1.83 

557 

s-pv 

13 + 

21-25 1 

1.19 

1.64 

1.38 

1.43 




1.43 

384 

S-Px 

13 + 

11-15 i 

2.21 

2.46 

2.81 

2.50 

2.72 

2.56 

2.67 

2.58 

1471 

S-Px 

13 + 

16-20 

2.56 

2.29 

2.27 

2.41 

2.28 

2.41 

2.40 

2.37 

1513 

S-Px 

13 + 

21-25 

2.54 

1.94 

1.99 

1.99 

1.98 

1.91 

1.99 

1.99 

1000 


Masturbation 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

1.46 

1.47 

1.04 

1.05 

1.19 



1.18 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

0.68 

0.61 

0.64 

0.69 

0.56 



0.59 1 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

1.80 

1.48 

1.41 

1.48 

1.46 



1.52 

521 

S-U 

9-12 

16-20 

0.72 

0.83 

0.81 

0.86 

0.81 



0.86 

520 

S-U 

13+ 

11-15 

2.30 

1.96 

2.33 

1.97 

2.07 

2.10 

2.11 

2.09 

2708 

S-U 

13+ 

16-20 

1.04 

1.40 

1.58 

1.61 

1.69 

1.41 

1.56 

1.55 

2762 

S-U 

13+ 

21-25 

0.50 

1.54 

0.92 

0.83 

0.86 

0.86 

0.89 

0.87 

1844 

S-U 

13+ 

26-30 

0.44 

0.74 

0.73 

0.70 

0.73 



0.69 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

1.55 

1.28 

1.15 

1.05 

1.12 



1.08 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

0.43 

0.61 

0.61 

0.58 

0.64 



0.60 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

1.50 

1.24 

1.38 

1.30 




1.35 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

2.50 

2.28 

2.12 

2.01 

2.27 


1 

2.16 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

1.75 

1.83 

1.80 

1.56 

1.73 


i 

1.68 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

1.89 

1.92 

2.09 

1.99 

1.81 



1.88 

1 547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

1.41 

1.56 

1.36 

1.21 

1.38 



1.33 

! 557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

0.70 

0.92 

0.83 

0.79 




0.81 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

1.97 

2.46 

2.47 

2.42 

2.45 

2.24 

2.36 

2.30 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

2.25 

1.48 

1.39 

1.53 

1.56 

1.72 

1.59 

1.62 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

0.86 

0.90 

0.93 

0.88 

0.98 

0.88 

0.93 

0.93 

1000 


Nocturnal Emissions 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

0.06 

0.07 

0.07 

0.07 

0.08 



0.08 

490 

S-U : 

0-8 j 

16-20 

0.10 

0.09 

0.09 

0.10 

0.09 : 



0.09 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

0.09 

0.16 1 

0.09 

0.10 

0.10 



0.12 

521 


152 


{Table continued on next page) 



GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

i 

AGE 

Median of Active Population: Size of 
Adequate Sample. Calculations with 
Samples of Various Sizes 

cases in 

largest 



50 

100 

200 

300 

400 

600 

1000 

largest 

sample 

SAMPLE 


Nocturnal Emissions (continued) 


s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

0.13 

0.20 

0.14 

0.15 

0.14 



0.14 1 

520 

s-u 

13 + 

11-15 

0.31 

0.28 

0.28 

0.27 

0.27 

0.27 

0.27 

0.28 

2708 

S-U 

13 + 

16-20 

0.26 

0.28 

0.30 

0.29 

0.29 

0.30 

0.29 

0.29 

2762 

S-U 

13 + 

21-25 

0.33 

0.31 

0.29 

0.29 

0.27 

0.29 

0.29 

0.28 

1844 

S-U 

13 + 

26-30 

0.20 

0.24 

0.24 

0.26 

0.24 



0.25 

479 

M-U 

13 + 

21-25 

0.09 

0.08 

0.09 

0.10 

0.09 



0.09 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 • 

0.09 

0.09 

0.08 

0.08 1 

0.09 



0.09 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

0.08 

0.10 

0.09 

0.09 




0.08 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 ! 

0.08 

0.10 

0.08 

0.07 

0.08 



0.08 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

0 07 

0.07 

0.08 

0.08 

0.09 



0.09 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

0.26 

0.13 

0.10 

0.09 




0.10 

375 

S-Jx 

13 + 

11-15 

0.18 

0.29 

0.26 

0.29 

0.28 



0.29 

601 

S-Jx- 

13 + 

16-20 

0.28 

0.34 

0.30 

0.33 

0.32 



0.31 

607 

S-PV 

13 + 

11-15 

0.34 

0.33 

0.29 

0.29 

0.29 



0.29 

547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 

0.31 

0.31 

0.28 

0.28 

0.29 



0.30 

557 

S-PV 

13 + 

21-25 

0.23 

0.26 

0.28 

0.30 




j 0.30 

384 

S-Px 

13 + 

11-15 

0.33 

0.29 

0.24 

0.25 

0.31 

0.26 

0.27 

0.27 

1471 

S-Px 

13 + 

16-20 

0.27 

0.26 

0.29 

0.26 

0.29 

0.27 

0.27 

1 0.28 

1513 

S-Px 

13 + 

21-25 

0.23 

0.25 

0.27 

0.27 

0.26 1 

0.27 

0.26 

0.26 

1000 


Pre-marital Intercourse with Companions 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

1.25 

1.69 

1.13 

1.26 

1.34 



1.27 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

1.50 

0.89 

0.98 

1.18 

1.05 



1.06 

486 

s-u 

9-12 

11-15 

0.44 

0.98 

0.75 

0.75 

0.81 



0.74 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

0.77 

1.06 

0.68 

0.91 

0.85 



0.83 

520 

s-u 

13 + 

11-15 

3.50 

0.20 

0.18 

0.43 

0.27 

0.31 

0.29 

0.26 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 

0.34 

0.20 

0.20 

0.23 

0.24 

0.22 

0.24 

0.22 

2762 

s-u 

13 + 

21-25 

0.41 

0.33 

j 0.32 

0.33 

0.36 

0.34 

0.33 

0.32 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

0.69 

0.69 

0.49 

0.48 

0.45 



0.47 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

1.20 

0.98 

1.29 

1.08 

1.30 



1.19 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

0.80 

0.98 

0.83 

0.89 

1.04 



0.97 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

1.17 

0.93 

0.89 

0.86 




0.87 

375 

S-Jx 

13 + 

11-15 

0.10 

0.06 

0.09 

0.09 

0.08 



0.08 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

0.20 

0.27 

0.14 

0.20 

0.17 



0.16 

607 

S-PV 

13+ : 

11-15 

1.50 

0.75 

0.38 

0.43 

0.34 



0.36 

547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 

0.07 

0.28 

0.12 

0.26 

0.16 



0.16 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

0.17 

0.15 

0.14 

0.14 




0.17 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

0.20 

0.30 

0.09 

0.32 

0.29 

0.27 

0.20 

0.27 

1471 

S-Px 

13 + 

16-20 

0.22 

0.23 

0.19 

0.28 

0.28 

0.21 

0.26 

0.23 

1513 

S-Px 

13 + 

21-25 

0.28 

0.34 

0.30 

0.30 

0.32 

0.31 

0.31 

0.31 

1000 


Marital Intercourse 


M-U 

13 + 

21-25 

2.36 

2.58 

2.78 

2.43 

2.60 



2.58 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

12.46 

2.00 

2.24 

2.21 

2.24 



2.20 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

1.92 

1.92 

1.93 

1.92 




1.88 

438 


753 


{ Table continued on next page) 



754 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN IVIALE 


GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

AGE 

Median of Active Population: Size of 
Adequate Sample. Calculations with 
Samples of Various Sizes 

CASES IN 

LARGEST 



50 

100 

200 

300 

400 

600 

1000 

largest 

SAMPLE 

SAMPLE 


Total Extra-marital Intercourse 


M-U 

13+ 

21-25 

0.10 

0.08 

0.14 

0.10 

0.11 



0.12 

460 

M-U 

13+ 

26-30 

0.34 

0.23 

0.08 

0.09 

0.11 



0.10 

561 

M-U 

13+ 

31-35 i 

1 

0.10 

1 

0.21 

0.19 

0.21 




0.18 

438 


Homosexual Outlet 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

0.75 

0.46 

0.41 

0.34 

0.36 



0.42 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

0.50 

0.32 

0.35 

0.37 

0.33 



0.35 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

0.42 

0.30 

0.28 

0.26 

0.29 



0.32 

521 

S-U 

9-12 

16-20 

0.28 

0.30 

0.37 

0.49 

0.37 



0.35 

520 

S-U ! 

13+ 

11-15 

0.08 

0.09 

0.08 

0.09 

0.10 

0.10 

0.09 

0.09 

2708 

S-U 

13+ ! 

16-20 

0.08 

0.08 

0.19 

0.08 

0.08 

0.08 

0.08 

0.09 

2762 

S-U 

13 + 

21-25 

0.23 

0.30 

0.50 

0.28 

0.22 

0.17 

0.21 

0.26 

1844 

S-U 

13+ 1 

26-30 

0.75 

0.88 ! 

0.75 

0.65 

0.57 i 



0.75 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

0.75 

0.10 

0.44 

0.32 

0.34 



0.36 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

0.43 

0.46 

0.21 

0.30 

0.29 



0.30 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

0.88 

0.38 

0.23 

0.20 




0.29 

375 

S-Jx 

13 + 

11-15 

0.10 

0,08 

0.08 

0.09 

0.08 



0.09 

601 

S-Jx 

13 + 

16-20 

0.08 

1.25 

0.26 

0.07 

0.08 



0.09 i 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

0.09 

0.09 

0.09 

0.09 

0.09 



0.09 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

0.07 

0.08 

0.07 

0.08 

0.09 

i 


0.09 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 1 

0.30 

0.08 

0.10 

0.20 


i 


0.23 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

0.09 

0.09 

0.18 

0.13 

0.10 

0.10 

0.10 1 

0.09 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

0.15 

0.07 

0.09 

0.08 

0.08 

0.08 

0.08 

0.08 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

0.50 

0.38 

0.24 

0.25 

0.32 

0.22 j 

0.24 

0.24 

1000 


Table 158. Size of sample necessary to secure stable medians on active 

population 

Boldface figures designate calculations that lie within 5 per cent, plus or minus, of 
the calculations obtained from the largest sample. Medians are frequencies per week. 



APPENDIX ON SAMPLE SIZE 


755 






Incidence: Size of Adequate Sample 

GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

AGE 

Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes 



50 

100 

200 

300 

400 

600 

1000 

LARGEST 

sample 


CASES 

IN 

LARGEST 

SAMPLE 


Total Outlet 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

92.0 

90.0 

92.5 

92.3 

91.5 



92.4 

490 

s-u 

0-8 

16-20 

100.0 

98.0 

98.5 

98.3 

98.0 



97.7 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

92.0 

95.0 

97.0 

96.7 

; 95.7 



95.4 

1 521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

99.3 

1 99.7 



99.6 

i 520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

98.0 

97.0 

95.5 

96.3 

95.0 

94.8 

96.6 

95.8 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 

100.0 

99.0 

100.0 

100.0 

1 100.0 

100.0 

99.8 

99.7 

2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

1 100.0 

100.0 

99.9 

99.9 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 



100.0 

479 

M-U- 

13+ 

21-25 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 



100.0 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 



100.0 

561 

M-U 

13+ 

31-35 

100.0 

100.0| 

loo.o! 

100.0 


1 


100.0 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

94.0 

94.0 

96.0 

93.3 

93.7 



1 93.8 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

100.0 

98.0 

98.0 

99.0 

98.0 



! 98.2 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

90.0i 

94.0 

95.5 

95.7 




i 95.2 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

100.0 

96.0 

96.0 

97.0 

97.2 



96.0 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 



100.0 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

94.0 

94.0 

95.5 

96.0 

96.0 



95.4 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

100.0 

100.0 

99.5 

99.3 

100.0 



99.6 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

98.0 

99.0 

99.5 

100.0 




99.7 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

94.0 

96.0 

98.0 

93.7 

96.0 

96.5 

96.3 

96.0 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

99.5 

99.5 

99.8 

99.7 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

100.0 

100.0 

100.0 

99.3 

99.7 

99.8 

99.8 

99.8 

1000 


Masturbation 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

88.0 

86.0 

86.0 

85.0 

85.7 



86.7 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

92.0 

82.0 

85.5 

86.3 

84.2 



84.8 

486 

s-u 

9-12 

11-15 

86.0 

86.0 

92.5 

91.7 

90.7 



90.0 

521 

S-u 

9-12 

16-20 

92.0 

89.0 

92.0 

88.0 

90.0 



89.8 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

78.0 

85.0 

83.5 

83.3 

81.2 

81.2 

82.8 

82.0 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 

92.0 

88.0 

86.0 

88.3 

91.2 

90.7 

88.4 

88.7 

2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

94.0 

84.0 

86.5 

90.0 

87.0 

87.8 

87.3 

87.3 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

94.0 

89.0 

81.0 

84.0 

85.5 



85.4 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

88.0 

86.0 

92.5 

88.01 

88.5 



88.1 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

82.0 

83.0 

87.5 

85.7 

86.7 



85.8 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

86.0 

91.0 

90.5 

90.7 




90.4 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

86.0 

80.0 

78.0 

79.3 

80.0 



80.0 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

82.0 

94.0 

85.5 

88.7 

88.7 



88.1 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

82.0 

78.0 

80.5 

79.3 

81.7 



80.8 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

92.0 

82.0 

86.0 

83.7 

87.5 



85.6 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

86.0 

87.0 

84.5 

83.3 


I 


83.6 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

80.0 

84.0 

89.5 

; 81.0 

84,5 

85.5 

85.5 

1 84.7 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

92.0 

96.0 

93.5 

93.0 

93.0 

92.7 

92.0 

91.5 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

84.0 

88.0 

86.5 

90.0 

89.0 

90.5 

90.3 

90.3 

1 1000 


{Table continued on next page) 



756 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 



s-u 

0-8 

11-15 

28.0 

27.0 

27.5 

29.0 

25.2 



26.9 

490 

s-u 

0-8 

16-20 

68.0 

53.0 

59.0 

58.7 

59.5 



58.4 

486 

s-u 

9-12 

11-15 

40.0 

44.0 

45.0 

42.0 

40.7 



41.8 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

62.0 

69.0 

76.0 

72.3 

71.7 



70.6 

520 

s-u 

13 + 

11-15 

82.0 

74.0 

70.0 

70.7 

73.7 

73.2 

72.7 

71.4 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 

96.0 

87.0 

89.5 

91.0 

90.2 

90.8 

90.7 

90.8 

2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

88,0 

84.0 

87.5 

83.0 

86.7 

85.7 

86.3 

86.3 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

82.0 

84.0 

84.0 

86.7 

85.0 



84.6 

479 

M-U 

13 + 

21-25 

66.0 

68.0 

65.5 

67.3 

66.2 



66.3 

"460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

76.0 

78.0 

66.5 

70.3 

74.7 



72.4 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

74.0 

72.0 

65.5 

69.7 




68.7 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

28.0 

22.0 

25.0 

26.7 

25.5 



26.0 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

50.0 

56.0 

57.5 

57.7 

58.5 



56.6 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

42.0 

39.0 

43.5 

40.3 




40.0 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

72.0 

75.0 

75.5 

78.7 

74.7 



75.4 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

88.0 

86.0 

86.0 

87.0 

87.7 



86.2 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

70.0 

71.0 

69.0 

72.0 

70.0 



70.7 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

84,0 

91.0 

90.0 

94.7 

92.5 



92.5 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

94.0 

85.0 

88.0 

90.7 




90.1 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

70.0 

67.0 

64,0 

68.7 

66.0 

67.3 

66.6 

68.0 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

82.0 

90.0 

89.0 

89.0 

91.2 

91.5 

92.1 

91.7 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

86.0 

89.0 

81.5 

85.7 

86.7 

86.5 

86.9 

86.9 

1000 


Pre-marital Intercourse with Companions 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

66.0 

44.0 

54.5 

52.7 

50.0 



51.2 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

82.0 

88.0 

86.5 

82.7 

83.0 



83.1 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

42.0 

46,0 

48.0 

43.7 

44.0 



43.2 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

78.0 

71.0 

73.5 

65.0 

70.7 



70.4 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

6.0 

10.0 

11.5 

7.7 

8.2 

9.0 

7.3 

9.0 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 

42.0 

30.0 

39.5 

36.7 

41.5 

40.3 

38.6 

39.1 

2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

58.0 

59.0 

60.0 

63.3 

58.7 

54.5 

57.9 

57.0 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

52.0 

57.0 

54.0 

63.3 

58.7 



60.5 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

58.0 

56.0 

52.5 

50.0 

52.0 



52.4 

481 

S-Px I 

0-8 

16-20 

86.0 

88.0 

84.5 

90.3 

87.5 


1 

86.4 

493 

S-Px i 

9-12 

11-15 

28.0 

54.0 

42.0 

48.7 



i 

45.9 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

10.0 

.4.0 

5.0 

6.3 

7.7 



6.5 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

36.0 

44.0 

41.5 

43.0 

41.5 



41.5 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

10.0 

8.0 

7.0 

8.0 

7.5 



7.1 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

20.0 

23.0 

23.0 

25.0 

23.0 



23.9 

' 557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

16.0 

34.0 

32.5 

33.0 




32.6 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

12.0 

7.0 

11.5 

10.3 

10.7 

10.8 

9.7 

10.3 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

42.0 

38.0 

46.5 

42.7 

40.7 

39.5 

41.2 

41.1 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

64.0 

55.0 

54.5 

59.0 

56.7 

57.8 

58.6 

58.6 

1000 


(Table continued on next page) 


APPENDIX ON SAMPLE SIZE 


757 



M-U 

13+ 

21-25 

lOO.o' 

100.0 

99.5 

99.3 

99.5 



99.6 

460 

M-U 

13+ 

26-30 

100.0 

99.0 

99.0 

99.3 

99.0 



99.3 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

100.0 

99.0 

99.5 

99.7 




99.5 

438 


Total Extra-marital Intercourse 


M-U 

13+ 

21-25 

18.0 

14.0 

14.5 

16.7 

16.0 



16.3 

460 

M-U 

13+ 

26-30 

22.0 

23.0 

26.0 

24.0 

23.5 



25.0 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

26.0 

27.0 

32.5 

36.0 




34.0 

438 


Homosexual Outlet 


S-U 

0—8 

11-15 

38.0 

23.0 

26.5 

26.0 

28.0 



27.3 

490 

s-u 

0-8 

16-20 

38.0 

26.0 

1 33.0 

34.0 

31.5 



30.2 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

38.0 

30.0 

36.5 

37.7 

36.0 



36.5 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

44.0 

44.0 

42.0 

45.3 

45.0 



45.6 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

20.0 

18.0 

29.0 

23.0 

22.2 

21.2 

22.6 

21.9 

2708 

s - u . 

13+ 

16-20 

22.0 

22.0 

14,0 

13.0 

17.7 

16.3 

15.0 

16.0 

2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

10.0 

10.0 

9.5 

9.3 

11.2 

9.0 

11.2 

10.2 

1844 

s-u 

13-1- 

26-30 

20.0 

22.0 

17.0 

14.7 

17.7 



16.9 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

30.0 

22.0 

24.0 

23.3 

23,7 



22.5 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

20.0 

22.0 

25,5 

24.7 

25.0 



24.5 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

32.0 

30.0 

29.5 

33.3 




34.1 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

14.0 

16,0 

13.0 

16.7 

14.2' 



14.6 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

8.0 

8.0 

10.5 

12.3 

9.2 



10.7 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

16.0 

26.0 

22.0 

20.7 

“ 22.7 



23.8 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

18.0 

22.0 

17.0 

16.7 

18.2 



17.4 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

8.0 

7.0 

6.5 

6.0 




6.2 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

24.0 

31.0 

26.0 

25.3 

25.5 

28.2 

26.8 

25.6 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

20.0 

19.0 

19.0 

16.7 

21.0 

18.0 

19.2 

18.0 

1513 

S-Px 

13 + 

21-25 

14.0 

12.0 

13.0 

11.0 

12.2 

11.8 

11.5 

11.5 

1000 


Table 159. Size of sample necessary to secure stable incidence data 

Boldface figures designate calculations that lie within 2 per cent, plus or minus, of 
the calculations obtained from the largest sample. 



758 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

AGE 

Locus OF Mode: Size of Adequate Sample 
Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes 

cases 

IN 

LARGEST 

SAMPLE 

50 

100 

200 

300 

400 

600 

1000 

largest 

SAMPLE 

Total Outlet 

S-U 

0~8 

11-15 

2.0 

0.5 

1.0 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

2.5 

2.5 

2.0 


1.0 



1.0 

486 

s-u 

9-12 

11-15 

2.0 

3.5 

3.0 

1.0 




1.0 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

1„0 

1.5 

1.0 

1.5 




1.0 

520 

s-u 

13 + 

11-15 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

1.0 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

2708 

s-u 

13 + 

16-20 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

2.0 

2.0 

1.5 

1.0 

2762 

s-u 

13 + 

21-25 

2.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.5 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1844 

s-u 

13 + 

26-30 

1.5 

2.0 

0.5 

1.0 

1.0 



1.0 

479 

M-U 

13 + 

21-25 

3.0 

3.0 

3.0 

3.0 

3.5 



3.0 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

3.0 

2.0 

3.0 

3.0 

3.0 



3.0 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

2.0 

2.5 

2.0 

2.0 




2.0 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

1.5 

1.0 

1.0 

0.5 

0.5 



1.0 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

1.0 

2.5 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 



1.0 

' 493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 


2.0 

1.0 

1.0 




1.0 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

601 

S-Jx 

13 + 

16-20 


3.0 

1.5 

0.5 

2.5 



0.5 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 


0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 

3*5 

2.0 

1.0 

1.5 

1.0 



1.0 

557 

S-PV 

13 + 

21-25 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 




1.0 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 1 

1.0 

2.0 

3.5 

0.5 

0.5 1 

0.5 

3.5 

3.5 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 


2.0 

2.0 

1.5 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.5 

1513 

S-Px 1 

13+ 

21-25 

0.5 

0.5 i 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1000 


MRStiirbation 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

1.0 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

486 

s-u 

9-12 

11-15 

0.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 



1.0 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 

0.5 

0.5 

1.0 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

1.0 

1.0 

2762 

s-u 

13 + 

21-25 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

1.5 

0.5 

1.0 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 




1.0 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

« . i 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

0.5 



0.5 

2.0 



0.5 

607 

S-PV 

13 + 

11-15 


0.6 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

1 547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 


1 0.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 



1.0 

i 557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

0.5 

0,5 

0.5 

0.5 




0.5 

384 

S-Px 

13 + 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

2.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

! 0.0 

1471 

S-Px 

13 + 

16-20 

0.5 

4‘.0 

1.0 

1.0 

1.0 

0.5 

1.0 

1.0 

1513 

S-Px 

13 + 

21-25 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

1000 


Nncturnai Emissions 


S-U 

s-u 

S-U 

s-u 


0-8 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0,0 i 



0.0 

0-8 

16-20 

0.1 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 1 



0.0 

9-12 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

9-12 

16-20 

0.0 

0.5 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 



0.1 


490 

486 

521 

520 


{Table continued on next page) 



APPENDIX ON SAMPLE SIZE 


759 




Locus OF Mode : Size of Adequate Sample 



FDTIG. 

Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes 

CASES 

GROUP 


AGE 

IN 


LEVEL 


LARGEST 



50 100 200 300 400 600 1000 

SAMPLE 



SAMPLE 



Nocturnal Emissions {continued) 


s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

1844 

s-u 

13 + 

26-30 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

479 

M-U 

13 + 

21-25 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 



0.1 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 



0.1 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 

0.1 




0.1 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 




0.0 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

601 

S-Jx 

,13+ 

16-20 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 i 


0.5 

0.0 

0.5 

0.5 



0.5 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 j 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0,5 

0.5 



0.5 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 1 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 




0.5 

384 

S-Px 

13 + 

11-15 1 

0.0 

0.5 

0.0 

0.5 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

1471 

S-Px 

13 + 

16-20 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 

1000 


Pre-marital Intercourse with Companions 


S-U 

0—8 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

0,0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 


0.5 

0.5 

0,5 

0.5 1 



0.5 

486 

S-U ' 

9-12 

11-15 

0.0 

' 0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

521 

S-u 

9-12 

16-20 

0.5 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 1 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

2708 

s-u 

13 + 

16-20 

0.0 

0.0 j 

0.0 

0.0 ! 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

2762 

s-u 

13 + 

21-25 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

1844 

s-u 

13 + 

26-30 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 i 



0.0 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 j 



0.0 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

. . 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 ! 

0.5 



0.5 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 




0.0 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 



0.0 

557 

S-PV 

13 + 

21-25 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 




0.0 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

1 0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

1471 

S-Px 

13 + 

16-20 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

I 0.0 

0.0 

0.0 

1000 


Marital Intercourse 


M-U 

13+ 

21-25 

2.5 

2.0 

3.0 

3.0 

2.0 

2.0 

460 

M-U 

13+ 

26-30 

1.5 

2.0 

3.0 

2.0 

2.0 

1 2.0 

561 

M-U 

13+ 

31-35 

2.0 

2.0 

2.0 

2.0 


2.0 

438 


Table 160. Size of sample necessary to fix the locus of the mode 


Boldface figures designate calculations that lie within 5 per cent, plus or minus, of 
the calculations obtained from the largest sample. 



760 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE EIUMAN MALE 




Height of Mode: Size of Adequate Sample 


GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes 

age 

CASES IN 

LARGEST 


1 50 

100 

200 

300 

400 

600 

1000 

largest 

SAMPLE 

SAMPLE 


Total Outlet 


s-u 

0-8 

11-15 

140 

16.0 

12.0 

13.3 

12.3 



12.0 

490 

s-u 

0-8 

16-20 

14.0 

13.0 

11.5 

9.7 

11.0 



10.5 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

14.0 

13.0 

12.0 

10.7 

10.3 



9.6 

521 

S-U 

9-12 

16-20 

20.0 

13.0 

15.0 

10.7 

11.5 



11.2 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

18.0 

16.0 

12.5 

13.7 

12.3 

12.0 

12.7 

12.4 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 

18.0 

14.0 

15.0 

15.3 

11.0 

13.8 

12.3 

11.9 

2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

18.0 

15.0 

15.0 

15.0 

14.5 

15.3 

14.0 

14.8 

1844 

S-u 

13 + 

26-30 

16.0 

15.0 

17.0 

17.0 

16.0 



16.3 

479 

M-U 

13 + 

21-25 1 

20.0 

12.0 

15.0 

17.3 

13.8 



14.1 

' 460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

22.0 

19.0 

17.5 

13.3 

14.3 



13.9 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

20.0 

18,0 

18.5 

16.3 




17.1 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

14.0 

16.0 

16.0 

14.3 

12.5 



13.3 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

22.0 

13.0 

13.0 

12 3 

10.5 



11.1 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

10.0 

13.0 

11.5 

11.7 




10.9 

375 

S-Jx 

13 + 

11-15 

22.0 

14.0 

17.0 

16.0 

14.3 



13.8 

601 

S-Jx 

13 + 

16-20 

16.0 

14.0 

12.0 

12.0 

12.3 



11,2 

607 

S-PV 

13 + 

11-15 

12.0 

14.0 

12 0 

13,0 

14.0 



13.7 

547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 

16.0 

17.0 

15 0 

14.7 

14.5 



14.2 

557 

S-PV 

13 + 

21-25 

20.0 

17.0 

i 20.0 

18.7 




19.3 

384 

S-Px 

13 + 

11-15 

22.0 

15.0 

12.5 

10.3 

11.0 

11.0 

9.9 

9.9 

J471 

S-Px 

13 + 

16-20 

12.0 

17.0 

14.5 

11.7 

12.5 

11.3 

11.7 

11.8 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

16.0 

16.0 

17.0 

14.7 

15.5 

15.8 

14.7 

14.7 

1000 


Masturbation 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

18.0 

19.0 

18.5 

18.3 

17.5 



18.0 

49C 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

32.0 

29.0 

29.5 

28.3 

30.5 



29.2 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

14.0 

17.0 

18.0 

19.0 

18.3 



17.3 

521 

S-U 

9-12 

16-20 

28.0 

23.0 

25.5 

24.3 

26.0 



25.0 

520 

S-U 

13+ 

11-15 

22.0 

15.0 

16.5 

16.7 

18.8 

18.8 

17.2 

18.0 

2708 

S-U 

13+ 

16-20 

28.0 

20.0 

18.5 

19.3 

15.5 

18.8 

16.6 

16.5 

2762 

S-U 

13+ 

21-25 

44.0 

20.0 

22.0 

24.3 

23.3 

24.5 

24.7 

24.5 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

40.0 

26.0 

22.5 

25.3 

26.3 



26.5 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

14.0 

20.0 

20.0 

19.7 

18.0 



18.3 

481 

S.Px 

0-8 

16-20 

36.0 

27.0 

29.5 

31.3 

30.8 



31.2 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

20.0 

26,0 

22.0 

22.3 




21.3 

375 

S-Jx 

13 + 

11-15 , 

14.0 

20.0 

22.0 

20.7 

20.0 



20.0 

601 

S-Jx 

13 + 

16-20 

20.0 

13.0 

14.5 

16.0 

15.0 



15.3 

607 

S-PV 

13 + 

11-15 

18.0 

22.0 

19.5 

20.7 

18.3 



19.2 

547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 

16.0 

18.0 

20,0 

19,7 

18.5 



18.7 

557 

S-PV 

13 + 

21-25 

36.0 

25.0 

25.5 

26.0 




25.8 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

20.0 

16.0 

12.5 

19.0 

15.5 

14.5 

14.5 

15,3 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

20.0 

23.0 

19.5 

16.7 

18.5 

16.0 

17.4 

17.6 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

24.0 

21.0 

25.0 

24.3 

21.5 

23.3 

22.4 

22.4 

1000 


{Table continued on next page) 



APPENDIX ON SAMPLE SIZE 


761 


GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

AGE 

Height of Mode : Size of Adequate Sample 

Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes 

CASES IN 

LARGEST 

SAMPLE 

50 

100 

200 

300 

400 

600 

1000 

LARGEST 

sample 

Nocturnal Emissions 

S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

72.0 

73.0 

72.5 

71.0 

74.8 



73.1 

490 

s-u 

0-8 

16-20 

36.0 

47.0 

41.0 

41.3 

40.5 



41,6 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

60.0 

56.0 

55.0 

58.0 

59.3 



58.2 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

38.0 

32.0 

35.0 

32.7 

33.3 



32.7 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

42.0 

33.0 

35.0 

33.0 

30.5 

34.3 

31.7 

j 33.8 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 1 

48.0 

47.0 

40.5 

42.0 

47.0 

45.3 

44.6 

1 44.7 

2762 

s-u 

13 + 

21-25 

36.0 

42.0 

49.5 

45.0 

41.8 

44.5 

43.7 

43.5 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

46.0 

46.0 

42.0 

45.3 

42.5 



44.3 

479 

M-U 

-13+ 

21-25 

40.0 

42.0 

35.5 

34.0 

36.0 



35.2 

460 

M-U 

13+ 

26-30 

42.0 

43.0 

42.0 

42.0 

40.3 



40.8 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 1 

48.0 

37.0 

38.5 

40.3 




40.9 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

72.0 

78.0 

75.0 

73.3 

74.5 



74.0 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

50.0 

44.0 

42.5 

42.3 

41.5 



43.4 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

58.0 

61.0 

56.5 

59.7 




60.0 

375 

S-Jx 

13 + 

11-15 

34.0 

30.0 

29.5 

37.0 

31.8 



33.6 

601 

S-Jx 

13 + 

16-20 

42.0 

41.0 

39.0 

37.7 

40.5 



38.9 

607 

S-PV 

13 + 

11-15 

30.0 

35.0 

31.0 

37.3 

35.3 



35.8 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

56.0 

48.0 

43.5 

51.0 

49.0 



48.8 

557 

S-PV 

13 + 

21-25 

48.0 

35.0 

48.0 

47.7 




45.8 

384 

S-Px 

13 + 

11-15 

30.0 

37.0 

36.0 

32.3 

34.0 

32.7 

33.4 

32.0 

1471 

S-Px 

13 + 

16-20 

38.0 

45.0 

44.0 

44.7 

46.3 

44.7 

43,6 

45.3 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

38.0 

51.0 

41.5 

43.3 

45.0 

43.7 

44.5 

44.5 

1000 


Pre-marital Intercourse with Companions 


s-u 

0-8 

11-15 

34.0 

56.0 

45.5 

47.3 

50.0 



48.8 

490 

s-u 

0-8 

16-20 

18.0 

21.0 

19.0 

20.3 

19.5 



19.3 

486 

s-u 

9-12 

11-15 

58.0 

54.0 

52.0 

56.3 

56.0 



56.8 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

24.0 

29.0 

26.5 

35.0 

29.3 



29.6 

520 

s-u 

13 + 

11-15 

94.0 

90.0 

88.5 

92.3 

91.8 

91.0 

92,7 

91.0 

2708 

s-u 

13 + 

16-20 

58.0 

70.0 

60.5 

63.3 

58.5 

59.7 

61.4 

60.9 

2762 

s-u 

13 + 

21-25 

42.0 

41.0 

40.0 

36.7 

41.3 

45.5 

42.1 

43.0 

1844 

s-u 

13 + 

26-30 

48.0 

43.0 

46.0 

36.7 

41.3 



39.5 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

42.0 

44.0 

47.5 

50.0 

48.0 



47.6 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

20.0 

24.0 

24,0 

24.3 

22.0 



22.3 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

72.0 

46.0 

58.0 

51.3 




54.1 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

90.0 

96.0 

95.0 

93.7 

92.3 



93.5 

601 

S-Jx 

13 + 

16-20 

64.0 

56.0 

58.5 

57.0 

58.5 



58.5 

607 

S-PV 

13 + 

11-15 

90.0 

92.0 

93.0 

92.0 

92.5 



92.9 

547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 

80 0 

77.0 

77.0 

75.0 

77.0 



76.1 

557 

S-PV 

13 + 

21-25 

84.0 

66.0 

67.5 

67.0 




67.4 

384 

S-Px 

13 + 

11-15 

88.0 

93.0 

88.5 

89.7 

89.3 

89.2 

90.3 

89.7 

1471 

S-Px 

13 + 

16-20 

58.0 

62.0 

53.5 

57.3 

59.3 

60.5 

58.8 

58.9 

1513 

S-Px 

13 + 

21-25 

36.0 

45.0 

45.5 

41.0 

43.3 

42.2 

41.4 

41.4 

1000 


{Table continued on next page) 



762 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 



M-U 

13+ i 

21-25 

18.0 

13.0 

13.5 

13.3 

13.5 



13.5 

460 

M-U 

13+ 

26-30 

16.0 

17.0 

14.5 

14.0 

15.8 



16.4 

561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

18.0 

19.0 

24.5 

18.0 

1 



20.3 

438 


Total Extra-marital Intercourse 


M-U 

13+ 

21-25 

82.0 

86.0 

85.5 

83.3 

84.0 



83.7 

460 

M-U 

13+ 

26-30 

78.0 

77.0 

74.0 

76.0 

76.5 



75.0 

' 561 

M-U 

13 + 

31-35 

74.0 

73.0 

1 

67.5 

64.0 




66.0 

438 


Homosexual Outlet 


SrU 

0-8 

11-15 

62.0 

77.0 

73.5 

74.0 

72.0 



72.7 

490 

s-u 

0-8 

16-20 

62.0 

74.0 

67.0 

66.0 

68.5 



69.8 

486 

s-u 

9-12 

11-15 

62.0 

70.0 

63.5 

62.3 

64.0 



63.5 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

56.0 

56.0 

58.0 

54.7 

55.0 



54.4 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

80.0 

82,0 

71,0 

77.0 

77.8 

78.8 

77.4 

78.1 

2708 

s-u 

13 + 

16-20 

78.0 

78.0 

86.0 

87.0 

82.3 

83.7 

85.0 

84.0 

.2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

90.0 

90.0 

90.5 

90.7 

88.8 

91.0 

88.8 

89.8 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

80.0 

78.0 

83.0 

85.3 

82.3 



83.1 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

70.0 

78.0 

76.0 

76.7 

76.3 



77.5 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

80.0 

78.0 

74.5 

75.3 

75.0 



75.5 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

68.0 

70.0 

70.5 

66.7 




' 65.9 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

86.0 

84.0 

87.0 

83.3 

85.8 



85.4 

601 

S-Jx 

13 + 

16-20 

92.0 

92.0 

89.5 

87.7 

90.8 



89.3 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

84.0 

74.0 

78.0 

79.3 

77.3 



76.2 

547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 

82.0 

78.0 

83.0 

83.3 

81.8 



82.6 

557 

S-PV 

13 + 

21-25 

92.0 

93.0 

93.5 

94.0 




93.8 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

76.0 

69.0 

74.0 

74.7 

74.5 

71.8 

73.2 

74.4 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

80.0 

81.0 

81.0 

83.3 

79.0 

82.0 

80.8 

82.0 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

86.0 

88.0 

87.0 

89.0 

87.8 

88.2 

88.5 

88.5 

1000 


Table 161. Size of sample necessary to fix the height of the mode 

Boldface figures designate calculations that lie within 5 per cent, plus or minus, of 
the calculations obtained from the largest sample. 



APPENDIX ON SAMPLE SIZE 


763 


GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

AGE 

Range : Maximum Frequency Minus 1 : 

Size of Adequate Sample 
Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes 

cases 

in 

largest 

sample 




50 

100 

200 

300 

400 

600 

1000 

largest 

sample 


Total Outlet 


s-u 

0-8 

11-15 

14.0 

25.0 

18.0 

22.0 

22.0 



25.0 

490 

s-u 

0-8 

16-20 

9.0 

16.0 

16.0 

17.0 

26.0 



26.0 

486 

s-u 

9-12 

11-15 

11.0 

19.0 

23.0 

25.0 

25.0 



25.0 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

18.0 

26.0 

16.0 

26.0 

26.0 



26.0 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

14.0 

13.0 

15.0 

29.0 

18.0 

26.0 

26.0 

29.0 

2708 

S-U 

13 + 

16-20 

7.5 

12.0 

14.0 

20.0 

20.0 

17.0 

23.0 

24.0 

2762 

S-U 

13 + 

21-25 

6.5 

8.0 

17.0 

11.0 

14.0 

18.0 

21.0 

21.0 

1844 

s-u 

13 + 

26-30 

8.0 

16.0 

17.0 

17.0 

17.0 



17.0 

479 

M-U 

13+ 

21-25 

8.0 

16.0 

15.0 

15.0 

22.0 



22.0 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

10.0 

11.0 

12.0 

22.0 

22.0 



22.0 

561 

M-U 

13+ 

31-35 

8.0 

14.0 

20.0 

18.0 




20.0 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

17.0 

14.0 

25.0 

22.0 

25.0 



25.0 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

12.0 

17.0 

21.0 

26.0 

29.0 



29.0 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

11.0 

16.0 

19.0 

23.0 




1 23.0 

375 

S-Jx 

13 + 

11-15 

9.0 

26.0 

25.0 

25.0 

26.0 



: 29.0 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

7.5 

11.0 

12.0 

15.0 

20.0 



23.0 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

9.5 

8.0 

9.5 

12.0 

12.0 



13.0 

547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 

7.5 

7.5 

9.0 

9.5 

11.0 



11.0 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

6.5 

7.5 

8.0 

10.0 




11.0 

384 

S-Px ’ 

13+ 

11-15 

14.0 

10.0 

14.0 

22.0 

20.0 

15.0 

23.0 

25.0 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

13.0 

10.0 

11.0 

14.0 

14.0 

15.0 

14.0 

21.0 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

11.0 

11.0 

13.0 

14.0 

15.0 

15.0 

17.0 

17.0 

1000 


Masturbation 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

10.0 

12.0 

10.0 

14.0 

14.0 



14.0 

490 

s-u 

0-8 

16-20 

4.0 

7.0 

7.0 

7.0 

7.0 



8.0 

486 

s-u 

9-12 

11-15 

7.0 

10.0 

12.0 

12.0 

12.0 



12.0 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

4.0 

8.0 

8,0 

8.0 

8.0 



8.0 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

14.0 

10.0 

14.0 

14.0 

15.0 

15.0 

15.0 

29.0 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 

7.0 

10.0 

10.0 

20.0 

18.0 

16.0 

23.0 

23.0 

2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

5.0 

7.0 

11.0 

7.0 

10.0 

15.0 

15.0 j 

15.0 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

5.0 

7.0 

10.0 

10.0 

10.0 



10.0 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

7.0 

14.0 

14.0 

10.0 

14.0 



14.0 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

7.0 

7.0 

7.0 

7.0 

7.0 



8.0 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

10.0 

9.0 

12.0 

10.0 




12.0 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

7.0 

25.0 

25.0 

25.0 

25.0 



' 29.0 

601 

S-Jx j 

13+ 

16-20 

7.0 

8.5 

10.0 

15.0 

20.0 



23.0 

607 

S-PV 

13 + 

11-15 

7.0 

' 7.0 

1 7.5 

7.0 

8.0 



12.0 

547 

S-PV 

13 + 

16-20 

7.0 

5.5 

8.5 

9.0 

10.0 



10.0 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

' 21-25 

4.5 

5.0 

5.5 

6.0 




6.0 

384 

S-Px 

13 + 

11-15 

10.0 

10.0 

14.0 

14.0 

14.0 

14.0 

20.0 

20.0 

1471 

S-Px 

13 + 

16-20 

8.0 

10.0 

10.0 

14.0 

14.0 

14.0 

14.0 

14.0 

1513 

S-Px 

13 + 

21-25 

7.0 

10.0 

8.0 

11.0 

12.0 

12.0 

12.0 

12.0 

1000 


{Table continued on next page) 



764 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


GROUP 


EDUC. 

LEVEL 


AGE 


Range: Maximum Frequency Minus 1: 
Size of Adequate Sample 
Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes 


50 

100 

200 

300 

400 

600 

1000 

largest 

sample 


CASES 

IN 

largest 

sample 


Nocturnal Emissions 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

0.5 

2.0 

0.5 

2.0 

2.5 



2.5 

490 

s-u 

0-8 

16-20 

0.5 

1.5 

2.5 

2.5 

2.5 



2.5 

486 

s-u 

9-12 

11-15 

0.5 , 

1.0 

3.0 

4.0 

5.0 



5.0 

521 

s-u 

9-12 

16-20 

1.0 

2.0 

3.0 

3.0 

3.0 



3.0 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

2.5 

2.5 

3.0 

5.0 

6.0 

7.0 

6.5 

12.0 

2708 

s-u 

13+ 

16-20 

1.5 

2.5 

4.0 

3.0 

5.5 

5.0 

6.0 

6.5 

2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

1.5 1 

2.0 

3.0 

3.5 

5.0 

5.0 

6.5 

6.5 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

1.5 

2.0 

2.0 

4.0 

4.0 



4.0 

479 

M-U 

13+ 

21-25 

1.0 

1.5 

2.5 

4.0 

4.0 



4.0 

460 

M-U 

13+ 

26-30 

1.0 1 

1.5 

1.0 

3.0 

3.0 



3.0 

561 

M-U 

13+ 

31-35 

1.0 

1.0 

1.5 

3.0 




3.0 

438 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

0.5 

1.0 

1.0 

2.0 

2.5 



2.5 

481 

S-Px 

! 0-8 

16-20 

2.0 

2.5 

2.5 

2.5 

2.5 



2.5 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

1.5 

1.5 

2.0 

4.0 i 




4.0 

375 

S-Jx 

13 + 

11-15 

1.0 

3.5 

3.0 

3.5 

6.5 



6.5 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ i 

16-20 

2.5 

2.5 

4.5 

3.5 

6.0 



6.0 

607 

S-PV 

13 + 

11-15 

2.5 

1.5 

3.0 

5.0 

7.0 



7.0 

547 

S-PV 

! 13 + 

16-20 

1.5 

2.0 

2.5 j 

5.0 

3.5, 



5.0 

557 

S-Pv 

13 + 

21-25 

2.5 

2.5 

2.5 

5.0 




5.0 

384 

S-Px 

13 + 

11-15 

1.5 

3.0 

2.0 

5.0 

4.0 

6.0 

6.0 

7.0 

• 1471 

S-Px 

13 + 

16-20 

2.5 

2.5 

4.0 

3.0 

3.5 

5.5 

6.0 

6.0 

1513 

S-Px 

13 + 

21-25 

2.5 

2.0 

3.5 

4.0 

3.5 

6.0 

6.0 

6.0 

1000 


Pre-marital Intercourse with Companions 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

12.0 

20.0 

14.0 

15,0 

17.0 



20.0 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

7.0 

14.0 

12.0 

16.0 

20.0 



20.0 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

2.0 

12.0 

10.0 

25.0 

25.0 



25.0 

521 

S-U 

9-12 

16-20 

14.0 

20.0 

10.0 

12.0 

25.0 



25.0 

520 

s-u 

13+ 

11-15 

3.5 

3.0 

1.0 

3.0 

3.0 

9.5 

9.5 

10.0 

2708 

s-u 

13 + 

16-20 

2.0 

3.0 

6.0 

3.0 

12.0 

7.0 

12.0 

12.0 

2762 

s-u 

13+ 

21-25 

5.0 

5.5 

6.0 

9.0 

9.0 

8.5 

17.0 

17.0 

1844 

s-u 

13+ 

26-30 

5.5 

14.0 

9.5 

6.0 

14.0 



14.0 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

11.0 

6.0 

20.0 

15.0 

20.0 

i 


20.0 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

8.0 

16.0 

16.0 

20.0 

25.0 



25.0 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

3.5 

9.5 

10.0 

14.0 




14.0 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

0.5 

, 0.1 

0.5 

0.5 

0.5 



3.0 

601 

S-Jx ! 

13+ 

16-20 

2.0 

1.5 

5.0 

5.5 

5.5 


1 

7.0 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 

2.5 

2.5 

2.0 

3.0 

3.0 



4.5 

547 

S-PV 

13+ 

16-20 

0.5 

6.0 

2.5 

7.0 

6.0 



7.0 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 

0.5 

3.5 

6.0 

7.5 




7.5 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

0.5 

0.5 

3.0 

7.0 

5.0 

6.0 

6.0 

10.0 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

3.0 

2.5 

3.0 

6.5 

7.0 

7.0 

7.0 

9.0 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

3.0 

4.5 

5.5 

9.0 

8.0 

9.0 

9.0 

9.0 

1000 


{Table continued on next page) 



APPENDIX ON SAMPLE SIZE 


765 


GROUP 

EDUC. 

LEVEL 

AGE 

Range: Maximum Frequency Minus 1 : 

Size of Adequate Sample 
Calculations with Samples of Various Sizes 

cases 

in 




50 

100 

200 

300 

400 

600 

1000 

largest 

sample 

sample 


Marital Intercourse 


M-U 

13 + 

21-25 

8.0 

16.0 

15.0 

15.0 

20.0 



20.0 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

8.0 1 

10.0 

11.0 

20.0 

20.0 



20.0 

561 

M-U 

1 

13 + 

31-35 

7.0 

13.0 

18.0 

16.0 




18.0 

438 


Total Extra-marital Intercourse 


M-U 

13-p 

21-25 

2.0 

1.0 

2.5 

3.0 1 

3.0 



3.0 

460 

M-U 

13 + 

26-30 

1.0 

3.0 

3.5 

2.5 

3.5 



3.5 

561 

M-U 

' 13 + 

31-35 

2.5 

3.0 

3.5 

1 

4.5 




4.5 

438 


Homosexual Outlet 


S-U 

0-8 

11-15 

4.0 

6.0 

6.0 

7.0 

7.0 



7.0 

490 

S-U 

0-8 

16-20 

4.0 

2.5 

3.5 

5.5 

5.5 



6.0 

486 

S-U 

9-12 

11-15 

5.0 

4.5 

5.0 

7.0 

5.0 



7.0 

521 

S-U 

9-12 

16-20 

1.5 

11.0 

6.0 

11.0 

11.0 



11.0 

520 

S-U 

13+ 

11-15 

0.5 

0.5 

3.0 

4.0 

2.5 

5.0 

6.0 

6.0 

2708 

S-U 

13+ 

16-20 

1.0 

2.5 

2.0 

3.0 

2.5 

5.0 

2.5 

7.0 

2762 

S-U 

13+ 

21-25 

0.5 

1.0 

3.0 

3.5 

4.5 

4.5 

7.0 

7.0 

1844 

S-U ' 

13+ 

26-30 

3.5 

4.0 

4.5 

5.0 

5.0 



5.0 

479 

S-Px 

0-8 

11-15 

3.5 

3.0 

6.0 

6.0 

6.0 



6.0 

481 

S-Px 

0-8 

16-20 

1.0 

3.0 

6.0 

5.5 

6.0 



7.0 

493 

S-Px 

9-12 

11-15 

3.0 

4.5 

3.0 

5.0 




5.0 

375 

S-Jx 

13+ 

11-15 

0.5 

4.5 

1.0 

4.5 

4.5 



4.5 

601 

S-Jx 

13+ 

16-20 

0.1 

2.5 

2.5 

3.5 

3.0 



3.5 

607 

S-PV 

13+ 

11-15 i 

2.0 

2.5 

2.0 

2.0 

4.0 



4.0 

547 

s-pv 

13+ 

16-20 

0.5 

1.0 

4.0 

2.0 

4.0 



4.0 

557 

S-PV 

13+ 

21-25 1 

0.5 

0.5 1 

1.5 

1.5 




3.5 

384 

S-Px 

13+ 

11-15 

0.5 

2.0 

3.0 

3.0 

3.5 

4.0 

5.0 

5.0 

1471 

S-Px 

13+ 

16-20 

0.5 

0.5 

5.0 

2.0 

3.5 

5.0 

5.0 

5.0 

1513 

S-Px 

13+ 

21-25 

2.0 ! 

1 

1.0 

5.0 

6,5 

4,5 

5.0 

6.5 

6.5 

1000 


Table 162. Size of sample necessary to secure stable data on range of variation 

The figui'cs shown represent maximum frequencies minus one case. Boldface figures 
designate calculations that lie within 5 per cent, plus or minus, of the calculations 
obtained from the largest sample. 



BIBLIOGRAPHY 


■ The following bibliography includes items which are cited in the text 
of the present volume, and some additional references to the more im- 
portant general studies on sex. It does not purport to be a general bib- 
liography on human sexual behavior. 

Achilles, P. S. 1923. The effectiveness of certain social hygiene literature. New York, 
Amer. Soc. Hyg. Assoc., pp. 116. 

Ackerson, L. 1931. Children’s behavior problems. A statistical study based upon 5000 
children examined consecutively at the Illinois Institute for Juvenile Research. 
I. Incidence, genetic and intellectual factors. Chicago, University of Chicago 
Press, pp. xxi -{- 268. 

1942. Children’s behavior problems. A statistical study based upon 2113 boys and 
1181 girls examined consecutively at the Illmois Institute for Juvenile Re- 
search. n. Relative importance and interrelations among traits. Chicago, 
University of Chicago Press, pp. xix + 570. 

Adams, C. R., and Packard, V. O, 1946. How to pick a mate. The guide to a happy 
marriage. New York, E. P. Dutton & Co., pp. 215. 

Aiilstrom, E. H. 1937. Studies on variability in the genus Dinobryon (Mastigophora). 
Trans. Amer. Micro. Soc. 56:139-159. 

Allbutt, T. C. 1921. Greek medicine in Rome. London, Macmillan & Co., pp. xiv + 633. 

Allen, C. 1940. The sexual perversions and abnormalities. London, Oxford University 
Press, pp. xii -j- 193. 

Allen, E., et al. 1939. Sex and internal secretions. A survey of recent research. Baltimore, 
Williams & Wilkins Co., pp, xxxvi -}- 1346. 

Allen, F. L. 1931. Only yesterday. An informal history of the nineteen- twenties. New 
York, Bantam Books, pp. 413. 

Allport, G. W. 1942. The use of personal documents in psychological science. New York, 
Soc. Sci. Res. Council, Bull. 49, pp. xix -h 210. 

American Social Hygiene Association. 1930. From boy to man. Amer. Soc. Hyg. Assoc. 
Publ. 626:1-20. 

Ananga-Ranga (A.F.F. and B.F.R., transL). 1885. Ananga-Ranga; (stage of the bodiless 
one) or, the Hindu art of love. (Ars amoris indica.) Cosmopoh, Kama Shastra 
Soc,, pp. XVI + 144. 

Angus, S. 1925. The mystery-religions and Christianity. A study in the religious back- 
ground of early Christianity. New York, Charles Scribner’s Sons, pp. xvi 
-I- 359. 

Arey, L. B. 1924. Developmental anatomy. A text-book and laboratory manual of 
embryology. Philadelphia, W. B. Saunders Co., pp. vii -(- 433. 

1946. Developmental anatomy (5th edit.). A textbook and laboratory manual of 
embryology. Philadelphia, W. B. Saunders Co., pp. ix -|- 616. 

Armitage, R. B. 1913. Private lessons in the cultivation of sex force. Chicago, Franklin 
Publ., pp. 202. 

Asdell, S. A. 1946. Patterns of mammalian reproduction. Ithaca, N. Y., Comstock 
Publishing Co., pp. xi -f 437. 


Baldwin, B. T. 1916. A measuring scale for physical growth and physiological age. 
Yrbk. Nat. Soc. Study tduc. 1916:11-22. 

1928. The detemiination of sex maturation in boys by a laboratory method. J. 
Comp. Psych. 8:39-43. 


766 



BIBLIOGRAPHY 


767 


Baldwin, B. T., Busby, L. M., and Gatside, H. V. 1928. The anatomic growth of children. 

A study of some bones of the hand, wrist, and lower forearm, by means of 
roentgenograms. Univ. Iowa Studies in Child Welfare 4, pp. 88. 

Ball, J. 1937. Sex activity of castrated male rats increased by estrin administration. 
J. Comp. Psych. 24:135-144. 

1939. Male and female mating behavior in pre-pubertally castrated male rats 
receiving estrogens. J. Comp. Psych. 28:273-283. 

1940. The effect of testosterone on the sex behavior of female rats. J. Comp. Psych. 
29:151-165. 

Barton, G. A. 1925 (4th edit.). Archaeology and the Bible. Philadelphia, American Sun- 
day School Union, pp. xv + 561 -f 122 pi. 

Barton, R. F. 1938. Philippine pagans; the autobiographies of three Ifugaos. London, 
George Routledge & Sons, pp. xxi + 271. 

Bateson, W. 1894. Matenals for the study of variation treated with especial regard to 
discontinuity in the origin of species. London, New York, Macmillan Co., 
pp. xvi -b 598. 

Bauer, J. 1944. The male climacteric — a misnomer. J. Amer. Medic. Assoc. 126:914. 

Beach, F. A. 1937. The neural basis of innate behavior. 1. Effects of cortical lesions upon 
the maternal behavior pattern in the rat. J. Comp. Psych. 24:393-439. 

1938. Sex reversals in the mating pattern of the rat. J. Genet. Psych. 53:329-334. 

1939. The neural basis of innate behavior. III. Comparison of learning ability and 
instinctive behavior in the rat. J. Comp. Psych. 28:225. 

1940. Effects of cortical lesions upon the copulatory behavior of male rats. J. Comp. 
Psych. 29:193-245. 

1941. Copulatory behavior of male rats raised in isolation and subjected to partial 
decortication prior to the acquisition of sexual experience. J. Comp. Psych. 

31:457 ff. ' . . . r 

1941. Female mating behavior shown by male rats after administration of testos- 
terone propionate. Endocr. 29 :409-412. 

1942. Male and female mating behavior in prepuberally castrated female rats 
treated with androgens. Endocr. 31:673-678. 

1942. Copulatory behavior in prepuberally castrated male rats and its modification 

* by estrogen administration. Endocr. 31 :679-683. 

1942. Analysis of the stimuli adequate to elicit mating behavior in the sexually 

inexperienced male rat. J. Comp. Psych. 33:163-207. 

1942. Effects of testosterone propionate upon the copulatory behavior of sexually 
inexperienced male rats. J. Comp. Psych. 33:227-247. , . 

1942. Sexual behavior of prepuberal male and female rats treated with gonadal 
hormones. J, Comp. Psych. 34:285-292. ... . u 

1942. Comparison of copulatory behavior of male rats raised in isolation, cohabita- 
tion, and segregation. J. Genet. Psych. 60:121-136. 

1942. Execution of the complete masculine copulatory pattern by sexually receptive 
female rats. J. Genet. Psych. 60:137-142. . , , . . ^ 

1942. Central nervous mechanisms involved in the reproductive behavior oi verte- 
brates. Psych. Bull. 39:200-226. -r ^ 

1942. Analysis of factors involved in the arousal, mamtenance and manifestation 
of sexual excitement in male animals. Psychosom. Med. 4:1^-198. 

1943. Effects of injury to the cerebral cortex upon the display of masculine and 
feminme mating behavior by female rats. J. Comp. Psych. 36 a 69-199.^ 

1944. Effects of injury to the cerebral cortex upon ^exually-receptive behavior m the 

female rat. Psychosom. Med. 6:40-55. ^ i t 

1944. Experimental studies of sexual behavior m male mammals. J. Clm. Endoc . 


1944. Relative effects of androgen upon the mating behavior of male rats subjected 
’ to forebrain iniury or castration. J. Exp. Zool. 97 :249-295. 

1945. Hormonal induction of mating responses in a rat with congenital absence of 
gonadal tissue. Anat. Rec. 92:289-292. 

1945. Bisexual mating behavior in the male rat; efiects of castration and hormone 
administration. Physiol. Zool. 18:390-402. 



768 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


1947. A review of physiological and psychological studies of sexual behavior in 
mammals. Physiol. Rev. 27 :240-307. 

Beach, F. A., and Holz, A. M. 1946. Mating behavior in male rats castrated at various 
ages and injected with androgen. J. Exper. Zool. 101 :91-142. 

Beach, F. A., and Rasquin, P. 1942. Masculine copulatoiy behavior in intact and cas- 
trated female rats. Endocr. 31:393-409. 

Beers, C. W. 1908. A mind that found itself. New York, Longmans, Green & Co., pp. 
ix + 363. 

Bell, S. 1902. A preliminaiy study of the emotion of love between the sexes. Amer. 
J. Psych. 13:325-354. 

Benedict, R. 1934. Patterns of culture. New York, Penguin Books, pp. xiii + 272. 
Benjamin, H. 1939, Prostitution and venereal disease. Med. Rev. of Rev., Sept. 1935: 
5-40. 

1944. The sex problem in the armed forces. Urol. & Cutan. Rev. 48:231-244. 
Bigelow, M. A. 1916. Sex-education. New York, The Macmillan Co., pp. xi + 251. 
Bilder-Lexikon (Schidrowitz, L., edit.). 1928-1931. Bilder-Lexikon. Vol. 1: Kultur- 
geschichte, pp. 942. Vol. 2: Literatur und Kunst, pp. 944. Vol. 3: Sexual- 
wissenschaft, pp. 916. Wien & Leipzig, Verlag fiir Kulturforschung. 
Bingham, H. C. 1928. Sex development in apes. Comp. Psych. Monogr. 5 (1):1-165. 
Biskind, M. S., and Falk, H. C. 1943. Nutritional therapy of infertihty in the male, with 
special reference to the vitamin B complex and vitamin E. J. Clin.. Endocr. 
3:148-153. 

Blackwood, B. 1935. Both sides of Buka passage. An ethnographic study of social, 
sexual, and economic questions m the north-western Solomon Islands. 
Oxford, Clarendon Press, pp. xvi + 624. 

Blankenship, A. B. 1943. Consumer and opinion research. The questionnaire technique. 

New York and London, Harper & Brothers, pp. x + 238. 

Blanton, M. G. 1917. The behavior of the human infant durmg the first thirty days of life. 
Psych. Rev. 24:456-483. 

Bloch, 1. 1903. Beitrage zur Aetiologie der Psychopathia sexualis. (Second part.) Dresden, 
H. R. Dohm Verlag, pp. xviii + 400. 

1908. (ex 6th edit.). (Paul, M, E., transL). The sexual life of our time in its relations 
to modern civilization. London, Rebman, pp. xvi + 790. 

1909. Das Sexualleben unserer Zeit in seinen Beziehungen zur modemen Kultur. 
Berlin, L. Marcus Verlagsbuchhandlung, pp. xii + 850 + xix. 

1912. Die Prostitution. (Vol. 1). Berlin, Louis Marcus Verlagsbuchhandlung, 
pp. xxxvi + 870. 

1933. Anthropological studies in the strange sexual practises of all races in all ages. 
New York, Anthropol. Press, pp. ix + 246 •+ xxiii. 

1934. Sex life in England. New York, Panurge Press, pp. 356. 

Bloch, I., and Loewenstein, G. 1925. Die Prostitution. (Vol. II, pt. 1). Berlin, Louis 
Marcus Verlagsbuchhandlung, pp. viii + 728. 

Bios, P. A. 1941. The adolescent personality. A study of individual behavior. New York, 
D. Appleton-Century Co., pp. xiii + 517. 

Boas, F. 1932. Studies in growth. Human Biol. 4:307-350. 

Boas, F. (edit.). 1938. General anthropology. New York, D. C. Heath & Co., pp. xi 
+ 718. 

Bonnar, A. 1937 (1941). The Catholic doctor. New York, P. J. Kenedy & Sons, pp. 
xvii + 184. 

Bowman, H. A. 1942. Marriage for modems. New York and London, McGraw-Hill Co., 
pp. IX + 493. 

Boys’ Clubs of America. 1946. Social hygiene in a boys’ club. New York, Boys’ Clubs 
Amer., pp. 19. 

Boy Scouts of America. 1911. The official handbook for boys. Garden City, N. Y., 
Doubleday, Page & Co., pp. xiv + 400. 

Boy Scouts of America. 1945 (38th printing). Revised handbook for boys. New York, 
Boy Scouts Amer., pp. 570. 

Bridges, K. B. 1936. Le developpement des emotions chez le jeune enfant. J. Psychol. 
Norm, et Path. 33:45 ff. 



BIBLIOGRAPHY 


769 


Brifiault, R. 1927. The mothers. A study of the origins of sentiments and institutions. 

New York, The Macmillan Co., Vol. 1, op. xix + 781 ; Vol. 2, pp. xx + 789; 
Vol. 3, pp. XV + 841. 

BriU, A. A. 1944. Freud’s contribution to psychiatry. New York, W. W. Norton & Co., 
pp. 244. 

Brockman, F. S. 1902. A study of the moral and religious life of 251 preparatory school 
students in the United States. Pedag. Sem. & J. Genet. Psych. 9:255-273. 
Bromley, D. D., and Britten, F. H. 1938. Youth and sex. A study of 1300 college students. 

New York and London, Harper & Brothers, pp. xiii + 303. 

Brown, H. C. 1937. A mind mislaid. New York, E. P. Dutton & Co., pp. 219. 

Brown, J. F. 1940. The psychodynamics of abnormal behavior. New York and London, 
McGraw-Hill Book Co,, pp. xvi + 4-84. 

Bruckner, S. J. 1937. How to give sex instructions. A guide for parents, teachers and 
others responsible for the trainmg of young people. St. Louis, The Queen’s 
Work, pp. 64. 

Bryk, F. 1933. Voodoo eros. New York, priv. printed, Falstalf Press, pp. 251. 

Bryk, F. (Norton, A. J., transl.). 1944. Dark rapture. The sex-life of the African Negro. 

Forest Hills, N. Y., Juno Books, pp. xvi -f- 167. 

Budge, E. A. W. 1895. The book of the dead. The papyrus of Ani in the British Museum. 

The Egyptian text with interlinear transliteration and translation, a running 
. translation, introduction, etc. London, Brit. Mus., pp. civ -j- 377. 

Buhler, C. 1930. The first year of life. New York, John Day Co. 

1931. [Sexual development in children.] Ztschr. Kmderh. 51 *.612-642. 

1931. Kindheit und Jugend. Leipzig. 

Burgess, E. W., and Cottrell, L. S. 1939. Predicting success or failure in marriage. New 
York, Prentice-Hall, pp. xxiii -{- 472. 

Bums, H., and Watson, B. F. 1942. Annotated Indiana statutes 1933. 1942 replacement 
volume. Indianapohs, Bobbs-Merrill Co., pp. viii + 1155. 

Butterfield, O. M. 1939. Love problems of adolescence. New York, Emerson Books, 
pp. viii + 212. 


Calverton, V. F. 1926. Sex expression in literature. New York, Boni & Liveright, pp. 
xxviii + 337. 

Calverton, V. F. (edit.). 1931. The making of man. An outline of anthropology. New 
York, Modern Library, pp. xv -f 879. 

1937. The making of society. An outline of sociology. New York, Modern Library, 
pp. xviii -f 923. 

Calverton, V. F., and Schmalhausen, S. D. (edit.). 1929. Sex in civilization. New York, 
Macaulay Co., pp. 719. 

1930. The new generation. The intimate problems of modern parents and children. 
New York, Macaulay Co., pp. 717. 

Campbell, E. H. 1939. The social-sex development of children. Genet. Psych. Monogr. 
21 :461-552. 

Cantrfi, H. 1944. Gauging public opinion. Princeton, N. J., Princeton University Press, 
pp. xiv + 318. 

Carlson, E. R, 1941. Born that way. New York, John Day Co., pp. ix -f- 174. 
Carmichael, L. (edit.). 1946. Manual of child psychology. New York, John Wiley & 
Sons; London, Chapman & Hall, pp. viii + 1068. 

Carpenter, C. R. 1939. Behavior and social relationa of free-ranging primates. Sci. 
Month. 47:319-325. 

1940. A field study in Siam of the behavior and social relations of the gibbon 
(Hylobates lar). Comp. Psych. Monogr. 16:3-12. 

1942. Characteristics of social behavior in non-human primates. Trans. N. Y. Acad. 
Sci. (2) 4:248-258. 

1942. Sexual behavior of free rangmg Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). I. 
Specimens, procedures and behavioral characteristics of estrus. J. Comp. 
Psych. 33:113-142. 

1942. Sexual behavior of free ranging Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). 11. 



770 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Periodicity of estnis, homosexual, autoerotic and nonconformist behavior. 
U Comp. Psych. 33:143-162. 

1942. Societies of monkeys and apes. Biol. Sympos. 8:177-204. 

Carpenter, E. 1897. An unknown people. London, pp. 31. 

1911. Love’s coming-of-age. A series of papers on the relations of the sexes. New 
York and London, Mitchell Kennerley, pp. 199. 

1914. Intermediate types among primitive folk. A study in social evolution. New 
York, Mitchell Kennerley, pp. 185. 

1930. The intermediate sex. A study of some transitional types of men and women. 
London, Allen & Unwin, pp. 176. 

Carpenter, E. (edit.). 1902. lolaus. An anthology of friendship. London, Swan Sonnen- 
schein & Co.; Manchester, by the author; Boston, Chas. E. Goodspeed, 
pp. vi 4- 191. 

1935. An anthology of friendship. New York, A. and C. Bond, pp. vi + 295. 

Catholic Woman Doctor. 1939. Growing up. A book for girls. New York, Benziger 
Bros., pp. ix + 47. 

Chapin, F. S. 1933. The measurement of social status by the use of the social status scale 
1933. Minneapolis, University of Minnesota Press, pp. 16. 

Chappie, E., and Coon, C. 1942, Prmciples of anthropology. New York, Henry Holt 
<& Co., pp. xi -h 718. 

Chesire, L., Saffir, M[., and Thurstone, L. L. 1933. Computing diagrams for the tetra- 
choric correlation coefficient. Chicago, University of Chicago Bookstore, 
pp. 59, 

Olivers, W. R. (edit.). 1946. Successful marriage and family living. Atlanta, Ga., More- 
house College, pp. 47. 

Chlenov. 1907. [The sex life of Moscow students.] Trud. pirog. Syezd, Spb. (acc. to 
Willoughby, 1937). 

Chlenov (Feldhusen, M., transL). 1908. [The questionnaire on sex among the students 
of the University of Moscow. Rpt. to Dermat. and Hyg. Sect. 10th Pirogov 
Congress.] Ztschr. f. Bekampfung der Geschlechtskrankheiien 8:211-224, 
245-255. 

Clark, L. 1937. Emotional adjustment in marriage. St. Louis, C. V. Mosby Co., 

pp. 261. 

Clark, P. L., Jr. 1928, Sex education. Why? Where? When? By whom? Ithaca, N.Y., 
Rational Life Pubhshing Co., pp. 104. 

Clarke, E. L. 1938. Petting. Wise or otherwise? New York, Association Press, pp. 30. 

Cobb, M. 1947. Are Negroes oversexed? Our World, May 1947:18-20. 

Committee on Education and Labor, 1946. Hearing before a subcommittee of the Com- 
mittee on Education and Labor, United States Senate, Seventy-ninth Con- 
gress, second session on S. 1779. A bill to authorize the Federal Security 
administrator to assist the states m matters relating to social protection, and 
for other purposes. Washington, U. S. Govt. Print. Off., pp. 51. 

Committee on Judiciary. 1946. Hearings before Subcommittee No. 3 of the Committee 
on the Judiciaiy, House of Representatives, Seventy-ninth Congress, second 
session on H. R. 5234. A bill to authorize the Federal Security administrator 
to assist the states in matters relating to social protection, and for other 
purposes. Washington, U. S. Govt. Prmt. Off., pp. iv + 63. 

Conn, J. H. 1939. Factors influencing development of sexual attitudes and sexual aware- 
ness in children. Amer. J. Dis. Child. 58:738-745. 

1940. Sexual curiosity of children. Amer. J. Dis. Child. 60:1110-1119. 

Conwell, C. 1937. The professional thief. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, pp. 
xiii + 256. 

Cooper, C. R. 1939. Designs in scarlet. Boston, Little, Brown & Co., pp. 372. 

Coppens, C., and Spalding, H. S. 1921. Moral principles and medical practice. The basis 
of medical jurisprudence. New York, Benziger Bros., pp. 320. 

Comer, G. W. 1938. Attaining manhood. A doctor talks to boys about sex. New York, 
Harper & Brothers, pp. xi -f 67. 

1939. Attaining womanhood. A doctor talks to girls about sex. New York, Harper 
& Brothers, pp. xiii -p 95. 



BIBLIOGRAPHY 771 

1942. The hormones in human reproduction. Princeton, N. J., Princeton University 
Press, pp. xix + 265. 

Comer, G. W., and Landis, C. 1941. Sex education for the adolescent. Hygeia, July 1941, 
reprint pp. 18. 

Crampton, C. W. 1908. Physiological age — a fundamental principle. I. Amer. Physic. 
Educ. Rev. 13:141-147. 

1944. Physiological age — a fundamental principle. Child Develop. 15:3-52. 
Crampton, H. E. 1925. Studies on the variation, distribution, and evolution of the genus 
Partula. The species of the Mariana Islands, Guam and Saipan. Carnegie 
Inst. Wash., Publ. 228A, pp. viii -{- 116. 

Crisp, K. B. 1939. Growing into maturity. Chicago, J. B. Lippincott Co., pp. 38. 
Curry, E. T. 1940, The pitch characteristics of the adolescent male voice. Speech Monogr. 
7:48-62. 


Davis, H. 1946. Moral and pastoral theology. In four volumes. Vol. 1 : Human acts, 
law, sin, virtue, pp. xix 4- 361. Vol. 2: Commandments of God, precepts of 
the Church, pp. x 4- 463. Vol. 3: The sacraments in general, baptism, con- 
firmation, holy eucharist, penance, indulgences, censures, pp. xviii 4- 504. 
Vol. 4: Extreme unction, holy orders, marriage, the clerical state, the religious 
state, duties of lay people, pp. xiii 4- 432. New York, Sheed & Ward. 

Davis, K. B. 1929. Factors in the sex life of twenty-two hundred women. New York and 
London, Harper & Brothers, pp. xx + 430. 

Dearborn, W. F., and Rothney, J. W. M. 1941. Predicting the child’s development. 
Cambridge, Mass., Sci-Art. Publishers, pp. 360. 

Dice, L. R. 1932. Variation in a geographic race of the deermouse, Peromyscus manicu- 
latus bairdii. Univ. Mich. Mus. Zool., Occ. Papers 239:1-26. 

Dickerson, R. E. 1930. So youth may know. New York, Association Press, pp. x 
4-255. 

1933. Growing into manhood. New York, Association Press, pp. vii 4- 100. 

1937. Getting ready to fall in love. New York, Association Press, pp. 16. 

1937. Getting started in marriage. New York, Association Press, pp. 11. 

1937. When a couple are engaged. New York, Association Press, pp. 12. 

1944. Some problems in adolescence. Los Angeles, Amer. Inst. Family Rel., 
Home Study Course 5 :45-60. 

1944. Looking ahead to marriage. Los Angeles, Amer. Inst. Family Rel., Home 
Study Course 6:61-72. 

1946. Straight from the shoulder. New York, Association Press, pp. 32. 

Dickinson, R. L. 1933. Human sex anatomy. Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins Co., pp. 
xiii 4- 145 + 175 figs. 

Dickinson, R. L., and Beam, L. 1931. A thousand marriages. A medical study of sex 
adjustment. Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins Co., pp. xxv + 482. 

1934. The single woman. A medical study in sex education. Baltimore, Williams & 
Wilkins Co., pp. xix 4-469. 

Dimock, H. S. 1937. Rediscovering the adolescent. A study of personality development 
in adolescent boys. New York, Association Press, pp. xx 4- 287. 

Dobkowsky, T. 1923. Gebissuntersuchungen an homosexuellen Mannem. Ztschr. f. 
Sexualwiss. 10:199-200. 

Dobzhansky, T. 1937. Genetics and the origin of species. New York, Columbia Univer- 
sity Press, pp. xvi 4- 364. 

1941. (2nd edit.). Genetics and the origin of species. New York, Columbia Univer- 
sity Press, pp. xviii 4- 446. 

Du Bois, C. A. 1944. The people of Alor. A social-psychological study of an East Indian 
Island. Minneapolis, University of Minnesota Press, pp. xvi 4- 654. 

Dudycha, G. J. and M. M. 1933. Adolescents’ memories of preschool experiences. 
Pedag. Sem. & J. Genet. Psych. 42:468-480. 

Dyk, W. 1938. Son of Old Man Hat. A Navaho autobiography. New York, Harcourt, 
Brace & Co., pp. xiv 4“ 378. 



772 SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 

Eddy, S. 1928. Sex and youth. Garden City, N. Y., Doubleday, Doran & Co., pp. xii 
+ 338. 

1928. 1 Sex and youth. Garden City, N. Y., Doubleday, Doran & Co., pp. 102. 

Edson, N. W. 1936. Love, courtship and marriage. Amer. Soc. Hyg. Assoc., pp. 18, 
Publ. No. 932. 

Elder, J. H., and Yerkes, R. M. 1936. The sexual cycle of the chimpanzee. Anat. Rec. 
67:119-143. 

Elliott, G. L., and Bone, H. 1929. The sex life of youth. New York, Association Press, 
pp. xi + 142. 

Ellis, A. 1945. The sexual psychology of human hermaphrodites. Psychosom. Med. 
7:108-125. 

Ellis, H. 1910 (orig. 1900). Studies in the psychology of sex. Vol. 1 : The evolution of 
modesty. The phenomena of sexual periodicity. Auto-erotism. Philadelphia, 
F. A. Davis Co., pp. xv -f 352. 

1915 (orig. 1901). Studies in the psychology of sex. Vol. 2: Sexual inversion. 
Philadelphia, F. A. Davis Co., pp. xi + 391. 

1913 (orig. 1903). Studies in the psychology of sex. Vol. 3: Analysis of the sexual 
impulse. Love and pain. The sexual impulse m women. Philadelphia, F. A. 
Davis Co., pp. xii -j- 353. 

1905. Studies in the psychology of sex. Vol. 4: Sexual selection in man. I. Touch. II. 
Smell. HI. Hearing. IV. Vision. Philadelphia, F. A. Davis Co., pp.^xi + 270. 

1906. Studies in the psychology of sex. Vol. 5 : Erotic symbolism. The mechanism 
of detumescence. The psychic state in pregnancy. Philadelphia, F. A. Davis 
Co., pp. X + 285. 

1910. Studies in the psychology of sex. Vol. 6: Sex in relation to society. Phila- 
delphia, F. A. Davis Co., pp. xvi -f- 656. 

1936 (also 1897, 1901, etc.). Studies in the psychology of sex. New York, Random 
House, Vol. 1, pp. xxxix + 353, xii + 353, xi + 270, xi -F 391 ; Vol. 2, pp. 
X -f 285, vii + 539, xvi -f 750. 

Engle, E. T. (edit.). 1943 (1944). Proceedings of the conference on problems of human 
fertility. Sponsored by the National Committee on Maternal Health, January 
15-16, 1943, New York City. Menasha, Wis., Geo. Banta Publishing Co., 
pp. xi “f 182. 

1946. The problem of fertility. Proceedings of the conference on fertility held under 
the auspices of the National Committee on Maternal Health. Prmceton, N. J., 
Princeton University Press, pp. viii + 254. 

English, O. S., and Pearson, G. H. J, 1945. Emotional problems of living. Avoiding 
the neurotic pattern. New York, W. W. Norton & Co., pp. 438. 

Erickson, R. O. 1941. Mass collections: Camassia scilloides. Ami. Mo. Bot. Garden 
28:293-298. 

Exner, M. J. 1914. The rational sex life for men. New York and London, Association 
Press, pp. xiii -j- 95. 

1915. Problems and principles of sex education. A study of 948 college men. New 
York, Association Press, pp. 39. 

1932. The sexual side of marriage. New York, W. W. Norton and Co., pp. 187. 
Reprint by Eugenics Press, N. Y. 


Family Life Education (Popenoe, P., edit.). 1946. Divorce does not solve the problem! 

Los Angeles, Amer. Inst. Fanuly Rel., 6 (7): 1-10. 

Fehlinger, H. (Herbert and Herbert transl.) . 1945. Sexual life of primitive people. New 
York, United Book Guild, pp. 133. 

Fere, C. S. 1904. The evolution and dissolution of the sexual instinct. Paris, Chas. 
Carrington, pp. xxiv -f- 358. 

Fer6, C. S. (Horst, U. van der, transl.). 1932. Scientific and esoteric studies in sexual 
degeneration in mankind and in animals. New York, Anthropol. Press, pp. 
xix + 325. 

Finger, F. W. 1947. Sex beliefs and practices among male college students. J. Abn. & 
Soc. Psych. 42:57-67. 



BIBLIOGRAPHY 773 

Fleege, U. H. 1945. Self-revelation of the adolescent boy. A key to understanding the 
modern adolescent. Milwaukee, Bruce Publishing Co., pp. xiv -f- 384. 

Folsom, J. K. (edit.). 1938. Plan for marriage. An intelligent approach to marriage and 
parenthood. Proposed by members of the staff of Vassar College. New York, 
Harper & Brothers, pp. xii + 305. 

Forbush, W. B. 1919. The sex-education of children. New York and London, Funk & 
Wagnalls Co., pp. 224. 

Ford, C. S. 1941. Smoke from their fires. New Haven, Conn., Yale University Press, 
pp. xiii -{- 248. 

1945. A comparative study of human reproduction. New Haven, Conn., Yale 
University Press, pp. 111. 

Ford, F. R., and Guild, H. 1937. Precocious puberty following measles, encephalomyel- 
itis and epidemic encephalitis. Bull. Johns Hopkins Hosp. 60:192-203. 

Forel, A. (Marshall, C. F., transl.). 1906 (1922 edit.). The sexual question. A scientific, 
psychological, hygienic and sociological study. Brooklyn, N. Y., Physicians 
and Surgeons Book Co., pp. xv -f 536. 

Fortune, R. F. 1932. Sorcerers of Dobu. The social anthropology of the Dobu Islanders 
of the Western Pacific. New York, E. P. Dutton & Co., pp. xxviii + 318. 
London, George Routledge & Sons. 

Frank, H. 1941. A guide for confession. Huntington, Ind., Our Sunday Visitor Press, 
. pp. 40. 

Frank, R. 1946. Personal counsel. A supplement to morals. New York, Informative 
Books, pp. 306. 

Freud, S. (Brill, A. A., transl.). 1938. The basic writings of Sigmund Freud. New York, 
Modem Library, pp. vi + 1001. 

Freud, S. 1945. Die Traumdeutung. Wien, F. Deuticke, pp. vii + 478. 


Gallichan, W. M. 1916. The great unmarried. New York, Frederick A. Stokes Co., 
pp. 224. 

Gallup, G. H. 1944. A guide to public opinion polls. Princeton, N. J., Princeton Univer- 
• sity Press, pp. xviii + 103. 

Gallup, G. H., and Rae, S. F. 1940. The pulse of democracy. New York, Simon & 
Schuster, pp. 335. 

Gantt, W. H. 1944. Experimental basis for neurotic behavior. Origin and development 
of artificially produced disturbances of behavior in dogs. New York and 
London, Paul B. Hoeber, pp. xv + 211. 

Gartland, F. E. 1945. Fight first: Marry later. Advice to men in the service and to their 
fiancees. Huntington, Ind., Our Sunday Visitor Press, pp. 29. 

1946. Boy meets girl the Christian way. An exhortation to chaste Christian court- 
ship. Huntington, Ind., Our Sunday Visitor Press, pp. 20. 

Geis, R. (Bmehl, C., transl.). 1930. Principles of Catholic sex morality. New York, 
Joseph F. Wagner, pp. xix + 105. 

Geschlecht und Gesellschaft (Vanselow, K., edit.). 1906-1914. Volumes 1-9. Berlin, 
Leipzig, Wien, Verlag der Schonheit. 

Gilbert, J. G. 1941. Memory loss in senescence. J. Abn. & Soc. Psych. 36:73-86. 

GiUis, J. M. 1930. The moral law (In the heart of man and on the tablets of stone). 
Huntington, Ind., Our Sunday Visitor Press, pp. 85. 

Glass, S. J., Deuel, H. J., and Wright, C. A. 1940. Sex^hormone studies in male homo- 
sexuality. Endocr. 26:590-594. 

Glass, S. J., and Johnson, R. H. 1944. Limitations and complications of organotherapy 
in male homosexuality. J. Clin. Endocr. 1 1 :540-544. 

Glass, S. J., and Makennon, B. J. 1937. The hormonal aspects of sex reversals. Western 
J. Surg. 45:467-473. 

Goldschmidt, R. 1916. Die biologischen Gmndlagen der kontraren Sexualitat und des 
Hermaphroditismus beim Menschen. Arch. f. Rassen- und Gesellschaftsbiol. 
12:1-14. 

Goodland, R. 1931. A bibliography of sex rites and customs. An annotated record of 



774 SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 

books, articles, and illustrations in all languages. London, George Routledge 
& Sons, pp. V +752. 

Greulich, W. W., et al. 1938. A handbook of methods for the study of adolescent 
children. Washington, Soc. Res. Child Develop., Nat. Res. Counc., Monogr. 
15, pp. xvii + 406. 

Greulich, W. W. 1938. Some anatomical aspects. In Greulich, W. W., et al. : A handbook 
of methods for the study of adolescent children. Washington, Soc. Res. 
Child Develop., Nat. Res. Counc., Monogr. 15:1-70. 

GrifiBn, L. F. 1945. The Cathohc boy examines his conscience. Huntington, Ind., Our 
Sunday Visitor Press, pp. 22. 

1946. The Catholic girl examines her conscience. Huntington, Ind., Our Sunday 
Visitor Press, pp. 24. 

Groves, E. R. 1940. The family and its social functions. Chicago, Philadelphia, and New 
York, J. B. Lippincott and Co., pp. xvi + 631. 

Gruenberg, B. C., and Kaukonen, J. L. 1940. High schools and sex education. Washing- 
ton, U. S. Govt. Print. Off., pp. xix + 110. 

Guttman, L. 1942. A revision of Chapin's social status scale. Amer. Soc. Rev. 7 :3 62-3 69. 


Haire, N., Costler, A., Willy, A., et al. 1937. Encyclopaedia of sexual knowledge. New 
York, Eugenics Publishing Co., pp. xx + 567. 

Hall, G. S. 1904. Adolescence. Its psychology and its relations to physiology, anthro- 
pology, sociology, sex, crime, religion and education. New York, D. Appleton 
and Co., Vol. 1, pp. xx + 589; Vol. 2, pp. vi + 784. 

Hall, W. S. 1907. The biology, physiology and sociology of reproduction. Also sexual 
hygiene with special reference to the male. Chicago, Wynnewood Publishing 
Co,, pp. 149. 

1909. From youth into manhood. New York, Association Press, pp. 106. 

1920. Sex training in the home. Chicago, Midland Press, pp. 128. 

Halverson, H. M. 1938. Infant sucking and tensional behavior. Pedag. Sem. & J. Genet. 
Psych. 53:365-430. 

1940. Genital and sphincter behavior of the male infant. Pedag. Sem. & J.* Genet. 
Psych. 56:95-136. 

Hamilton, G. V. 1914. A study of sexual tendencies in monkeys and baboons. J. Anim. 
Behav. 4:295-318. 

1929. A research in marriage. New York, A. and C. Boni, pp. xiii + 570. 
Hamilton, G. V., and MacGowan, K. 1929. What is wrong with marriage. New York, 
A. and C. Boni, pp. xxi + 319. 

Hamilton, J. B. 1937. Treatment of sexual underdevelopment with synthetic male 
hormone substance. Endocr. 21 :649-654. 

1937. Induction of penile erection by male hormone substances. Endocr. 21: 

744-749. 

Hamilton, V. C. (transl.). 1940. Report on the sex question by the Swedish population 
commission. Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins Co., pp. xx + 182. 

Harper, R. F. 1904 (edit. 2). The code of Hammurabi, King of Babylon. Chicago, 
University of Chicago Press, pp. xv + 192 + 103 pi. 

Hartland, E. 1931. Motherright. In Calverton, V., 1931, The making of man. New York, 
Modern Library, pp. xv + 879. 

Hatt, E. D. 1931. Turi’s book of Lappland. London, Jonathan Cape, pp. 293. 
Hattendorf, K. W. 1932. A study of the questions of young children concerning sex: 

A phase of an experimental approach to parent education. J. Soc. Psych. 
3:37-65. 

Heidel, A. 1946. The Gilgamesh epic and Old Testament parallels. Chicago, University 
of Chicago Press, pp. ix + 269. 

Heller, C. G., and Myers, G. B. 1944. The male climacteric, its symptomology, diagnosis 
and treatment. Use of urinary gonadotropins, therapeutic test with testos- 
terone propionate and testicular biopsies in delineating the male climacteric 
from psychoneurosis and psychogenic impotence. J. Amer. Medic. Assoc. 
126:472-477. 



BIBLIOGRAPHY 775 

Heller, C. G., Nelson, W. O., and Roth, A. A. 1943. Functional prepuberal castration 
in males. J. Clin. Endocr. 3:573-588. 

Henry, G. W. 1938. Essentials of psychiatry. Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins Co., pp. 
xii + 465. 

1941. Sex variants. A study of homosexual patterns. New York and London, 
Paul B. Hoeber, 2 vols., pp. xx + vi + 1179. 

Henry, N. B., et al. 1944. The forty-third yearbook of the National Society for the Study 
of Education. Part 1, Adolescence. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 
pp. X 4“ 358. 

Hildebrand, D. V. 1935 (orig. 1927). In defence of purity. New York, Sheed & Ward, 
pp. 196. 

Hile, R. 1937. Morphometry of the cisco, Leucichthys artedi (Le Seur), in the lakes of 
Northeastern Highlands, Wisconsin. Int. Rev. ges. Hydrobiol. u. Hydrogr. 
36:57-130. 

Hillyer, J. 1927. Reluctantly told. New York, The Macmillan Co., pp. xvii + 205. 

Himes, N. E. 1940. Your marriage. A guide to happiness. New York, Farrar & Rinehart, 
pp. xvi + 430. 

Hirschfeld, M. (edit.). 1899-1922. Jahrbuch fiir sexuelle Zwischenstufen. Unter beson- 
derer Beriicksichtigung der Homosexualitat. Leipzig, Max Spohr, Volumes 
1 - 22 . 

Hirschfeld, M. 1903. Ursachen und Wesen des Uranismus. Jahrb. f. sex. Zwisch. 5:1-193. 

1910. Die Transvestiten, eine Untersuchung iiber den erotischen Verkleidungstrieb, 
mit umfangreichem casuistischen und historischen Material. Berlin, Alfred 
Pulvermacher & Co., pp. vi -f 562. 

1912. Naturgesetze der Liebe. Eine gemeinverstandliche Untersuchung iiber den 
Liebes-Eindruck, Liebes-Drang und Liebes-Ausdruck. Berlin, Verlag A. 
Pulvermacher & Co., pp. 281. 

1916, 1921, 1920. Sexualpathologie. Ein Lehrbuch fiir Arzte und Studierende. 
Part 1: Geschlechtliche Entwicklungsstorungen mit besonderer Beruck- 
sichtigung der Onanie. Part 2: Sexuelle Zwischenstufen. Das mannliche Weib 
und der weibliche Mann. Part 3 : Stbrungen im Sexualstotfwechsel mit beson- 
derer Beriicksichtigung der Impotenz. Bonn, Marcus & Webers Verlag, 

* Vol. 1, pp. XV -f 211; Vol. 2, pp. x 279; Vol. 3, pp. xi 4* 340. 

1920 (edit. 2). Die Homosexualitat des Mannes und des Weibes. Berlin, L. Marcus 
Verlagsbuchhandlung, pp. xvii + 1067. 

1926-1930. Geschlechtskunde auf Grund dreiszigjahriger Forschung und Erfahrung 
bearbeitet. Vol. 1 (1926): Die Korperseelischen Grundlagen, pp. xv -f 638. 
Vol. 2 (1928) : Folgen und Folgerungen, pp. 659. Vol. 3, pp. 755. Vol. 4 (1930) : 
Index volume. Stuttgart, J. Piittmann Verlagsbuchhandlung. 

1935. Men and women. The world journey of a sexologist. New York, G. P. Put- 
nam’s Sons, pp. xix + 325. 

1936. Magnus Hirschfeld. In: Robinson, Encyclopaedia sexualis, pp. 317-321. 
New York, Dingwall-Rock. 

1936. Homosexuality. In: Robinson, Encyclopaedia sexualis, pp. 321-334. New 
York, Dingwall-Rock. 

1940. Sexual pathology. A study of derangements of the sexual instinct. New York, 
Emerson Books, pp. xii + 368. 

1944. Sexual anomalies and perversions. Physical and psychological development 
and treatment. London, Francis Aldor Publ., pp. 630. 

Hirschfeld, M., and Gotz, B. 1929. Sexualgeschichte der. Menschheit. Berlin, P. Langen- 
scheidt, pp. vii + 385. 

Hodaim, M. 1928. Sexualelend und Sexualberatung. Briefe aus der Praxis. Rudolstadt, 
Greifenverlag (W. Geissler), pp. 302. 

Hoddan, M. (Gibbs, J., transl.). 1932. Sex life in Europe. A biological and sociological 
survey. New York, Julian Press, pp. 252. 

Hohman, L. B., and Schaffner, B. 1947. The sex lives of unmarried men. Amer. J. Soc. 
52:501-507. 

HoUingshead, A. B. 1939. Human ecology and the social sciences. In: Park, R. E. et al., 
An outline of the principles of sociology. New York, Barnes & Noble, pp. 
65-168. 



776 SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 

Hoopes, G. G. 1939. Out of the running. Springfield, 111., Charles C Thomas, pp. 
xvii -f 158. 

Hotchkiss, R. S. 1944. Fertility in men. Philadelphia, J. B. Lippincott Co., pp. xiii + 216. 

Hubbs, C. L., Hubbs, L. C., and Johnson, R. E. 1943. Hybridization in nature between 
species of catostomid fishes. Contrib. Lab. Vert. Biol. 22:1-76. 

Hubbs, C. L., and Miller, R. R. 1942. Mass hybridization between two genera of cyprinid 
fishes in the Mohave Desert, California. Papers Mich. Acad. Sci., Arts, 
Letters 28:343-378. 

Hughes, W. L. 1926. Sex experiences of boyhood. J. Soc. Hyg. 12:262-273. 

Hunt, J. McV., et al. 1944. Personality and the behavior disorders. A handbook based 
on experimental and clinical research. New York, Ronald Press Co., 2 vols.: 
pp. xii + V + 1242. 

Huxley, J. (edit.). 1940. The new systematics. Oxford, Clarendon Press, pp. viii + 583. 

Hyman, H. T. 1946-47. An integrated practice of medicine. A complete general practice 
of medicine from differential diagnosis by presenting symptoms to specific 
management of the patient. Philadelphia, W. B. Saunders Co., 4 Vols. + 
Index. 


Inmate, Ward 8. 1932. Behind the door of delusion. New York, The Macmillan Co., 
pp. xvi + 325. 

Isaacs, S, 1933. Social development of young children. A study of beginnings. London, 
George Routledge & Sons, pp. xii + 480. 


Jackson, C. M. 1925. The effect of inanition and malnutrition upon growth and structure. 
Philadelphia, P. Blakiston’s Son & Co., pp. xii 4- 616. 

Jahrbuch fiir sexuelle Zwischenstufen. 1899-1922. Unter besonderer Beriicksichtigung 
der Homosexualitat. (Hirschfeld, M., edit.) Volumes 1-22. Leipzig, Max 
Spohr. 

Jefferis, B. G. and Nichols, J. L. 1912. Search lights on health. Light on dark comers. 

A complete sexual science and a guide to purity and physical manhood. 
Advice to maiden, wife, and mother. Love, courtship, and marriage. ’Naper- 
ville, III, J, L. Nichols, pp. 487. 

Jerome, E. K. 1937. Change of voice in male adolescents. Quart. J. Speech 23 :648-653. 

Johnson, W. 1930. Because I stutter. New York and London, D. Appleton & Co., 
pp. XV + 127. 

Jones, H. E. 1944, The development of physical abilities. In \ Henry, N. B., Adolescence, 
pp. 100-122. Chicago, Nat. Soc. Study Educ. (University of Chicago Press). 

Judge Baker Foundation. 1922. 20 case studies. Boston, Judge Baker Fnd. [no contin. 
paginat,]. 

Jung, F. T., and Shafton, A, L. 1935. The mammary gland in the normal adolescent 
male. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 33:455-458. 


Kahn, F. 1939. Our sex hfe. A guide and coxmsellor for everyone. New York, Alfred A. 
Knopf, pp. xxxviii + 459. 

Karpman, B. 1935. The individual criminal; studies in the psychogenetics of crime. 
Vol. 1, cases 1-5. Washington, Nerv. & Ment. Dis. Publ. Co. 

1944. Case studies in the psychopathology of crime. A reference source for research 
in criminal material. Vol. Il.Washington, Medic. Sci. Press, pp. viii -b 738. 
Katz, D. 1941. The public opinion polls and the 1940 election. Publ. Opin. Quart. 
5:52-78. 

Kellogg, W. N., and Kellogg, L. A. 1933. The ape and the child. A study of environ- 
mental influence upon early behavior. New York and London, Whittlesey 
House, pp. xiv -}- 341. 

KeDy, G. 1941. Modern youth and chastity. St. Louis, The Queen’s Work, pp. 104. 
Kempf, E. J. 1917. The social and sexual behavior of infra-human primates with some 
comparable facts in human behavior. Psychoanal. Rev. 4:127-154. 



BIBLIOGRAPHY 777 

Kinsey, A. C. 1941. Criteria for a hormonal explanation of the homosexual. J. Clin. 
Endocr. 1:424-428. 

1942. Isolating mechanisms in gall wasps. Biol. Symposia 6:251-269. 

1947. Sex behavior in the human animal. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 47 :635-637. 

Kirkendall, L. A. 1940. Building a program of sex education for the secondary school. 
J. Soc. Hyg. 26:305-311. 

1940. Sex adjustments of young men. New York and London, Harper & Brothers, 
pp. xiii + 215. 

Kirsch, F. M. 1930. Sex education and training in chastity. New York, Benziger Bros., 
pp. xxxix -h 540. 

1935. The sex problem. A challenge and an opportunity. New York, Paulist Press, 
pp. 32. 

1945. Training in chastity. Huntington, Ind., Our Sunday Visitor Press, pp. 27. 

Klemer, D. H. 1939. The other sex. A frank statement, addressed to both boys and girls, 
of the essential facts that young people want and need to know about sex. 
New York, Association Press, pp. 42. 

Koch, R. A., and Wilbur, R. L. 1944. Promiscuity as a factor in the spread of venereal 
disease. J. Soc. Hyg. 30:517-529. 

Krafift-Ebing, R. (Rebman, F. J., transl.). 1922 (ex edit. 12). Psychopathia sexualis, with 
especial reference to the antipathic sexual instinct. A medico-forensic study. 
Brooklyn, N. Y., Physicians and Surgeons Book Co., pp. xiii -j- 617. 

Krafft-Ebing, R. (Moll, A., edit.). 1924 (edit. 16-17). Psychopathia sexualis. Mit beson- 
derer Beriicksichtigung der kontraren Sexualempfindung. Eine medizinisch- 
gerichtliche Studie fiir Arzte und Juristen. Stuttgart, Ferdinand Enke, pp. 
V -f 832. 

Krauss, F. S. (edit.). 1904-1913. Anthropophyteia. Jahrbucher fiir folkloristische Erhe- 
bungen und Forschungen zur Entwicklunggeschichte der geschlechtlichen 
Moral. Leipzig, Deutsche Verlagsactiengeseilschaft & ethnologischer Verlag. 
Volumes 1-10. 

Kubitschek, P. E, 1932. Sexual development of boys with special reference to the appear- 
ance of the secondary sexual characters and their relationship to structural 
and personality types. J. Nerv. & Ment. Dis. 76:425-451. 


Landes, R. 1938. The Ojibwa woman. New York, Columbia University Press, pp. 
viii + 247. 

Landis, C., et al. 1940. Sex in development. A study of the growth and development of 
the emotional and sexual aspects of personality together with physiological, 
anatomical, and medical information on a group of 153 normal women and 
142 female psychiatric patients. New York and London, Paul B. Hoeber, 
pp. XX -h 329. 

Landis, C., and BoUes, M. M. 1942. Personality and sexuality of the physically handi- 
capped woman. New York and London, Paul B. Hoeber, pp. xii + 171. 

1946. Textbook of abnormal psychology. New York, The Macmillan Co., pp. 
xii + 576. 

Laton, A. D., and Bailey, E. W. 1940. Suggestions for teaching selected material from 
the field of sex responsiveness, mating and reproduction. New York, Bur. 
Publ. Teachers Coll. Columbia Univ., Monogr. 2, pp. xviii 4* 118. 

Legrain, L. 1936. Ur excavations. Vol. Ill: Archaic seal-impressions. Publ. Brit. Mus. 
and Univ. Pa. Mus., pp. viii 4- 51 -}- 58 pj. 

Levey, H. B. 1939. Critique of the theory of sublimation. Psychiatry 2:239-270. 

Levy, D. M. 1940. “Control-situation” studies of children’s responses to the difference 
in genitalia. Amer. J. Orthopsychiat. 10:755-762. 

Licht, H. 1925, 1926, 1928. Sittengeschichte Griechenlands. Dresden, Zurich, Paul 
Aretz Verlag. Vol. 1: Die griechische Gesellschaft, pp. 319. Vol. 2: Das 
Liebesleben der Griechen, pp. 263. Vol. 3: Erganzungsband. Die Erotik in 
der griechischen Kunst. Erganzungen zu Band I und II, pp. viii 4- 279. 

Ocht, H. (Freese, J. H., transl.). 1932. Sexual life in ancient Greece. London, George 
Routledge & Sons, pp. xv -f 557. 



778 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Lieber, E. 1920. From boyhood to manhood. Indianapolis, Ind. State Bd. Health, pp. 8, 
Liedennan, E. E. 1926. Sexual guidance. New York, E. E. Liederman, pp. 172. 
Linderman, F. B. 1930. American, the life stoiy of a great Indian. New York, John Day 
Co., pp. xi + 313. 

1932. Red mother. New York, John Day Co., pp. 256. 

Lindsey, B. B., and Evans, W. 1929. The companionate marriage. Garden City, N. Y., 
Garden City Publishing Co., pp. xxxiv -f- 396. 

Linton, R. 1936. The study of man. An introduction. New York, D. Appleton-Century 
Co., pp. ix + 503. 

Lips, J. 1938. Government. In: Boas, F., 1938, General anthropology. New York, D. C. 
Heath & Co., pp. xi + 718. 

Lisser, H., and Curtis, L. E. 1943. Testosterone therapy of male eunuchoids. IV. Results 
from methyl testosterone linguets. J. Clin. Endocr. 3:389-399. 

Lisser, H., Escamilla, R. F., and Curtis, L. E. 1942. Testosterone therapy of male 
eunuchoids. III. Sublingual administration of testosterone compounds. 
J. Clin. Endocr. 2:351-360. 

Lorand, S. (edit.). 1944. Psychoanalysis today. New York, International Universities 
Press, pp. xvi -f 404. 

1946. Technique of psychoanalytic therapy. New York, International Universities 
Press, pp. viii + 251. 

Lorge, I. 1942. The “last school grade completed” as an index of intellectual level. 
School and Society 56 :529-532. 

Lovell, P. M. 1940. Sex and you. Los Angeles, Lovell Publ., pp. 98. 

Lowry, O. 1938. A virtuous woman. Sex life in relation to the Christian life. Grand 
Rapids, Mich., Zondervan Publishing House, pp. 160. 

Lundberg, G. A., and Friedman, P. 1943. A comparison of three measures of socio- 
economic status. Rural Soc. 8 :227-242. 


Malamud, W., and Palmer, G. 1932. The role played by masturbation in the causation 
of mental disturbances. J. Nerv. & Ment. Dis. 76:220-233, 366-379. 
Malinowski, B. 1929. The sexual life of savages in North-western Melanesia. An ethno- 
graphic account of courtship, marriage and family life among the natives of 
the Trobnand Islands, British New Guinea. New York, Halcyon House, pp. 
xxviii + 603. 

Mantegazza, P. (Putnam, S., transl.). 1935. The sexual relations of mankind. New York, 
Eugenics Publishing Co., pp. xvi + 335. 

Mantegazza, P. (Alexander, H., transl). 1936. Physiology of love. New York, Eugenics 
Publishing Co., pp. xviii + 237. 

Martindale, C. C. 1925. The difficult commandment. New York, P. J. Kenedy & Sons, 

pp. 80. 

Maslow, A. H. 1940. A test for dominance-feeling (self-esteem) in college women. 
J. Soc. Psych. 12:255-270. 

1942. Self-esteem (dominance-feeling) and sexuality in women. J. Soc. Psych. 
16:259-294. 

1942. Manual for social personality inventory for college women. Stanford Univer- 
sity, Stanford University Press. 

Maslow, A. H., Hirsh, E., Stein, M., and Honigmann, 1. 1945. A clinically derived test 
for measuring psychological security-insecurity. J. Gen. Psych. 33:21-41. 
May, G. 1931. Social control o£ sex expression. New York, Wilham Morrow & Co., 
pp. xi + 307. 

Mayr, E. 1942. Systematics and the origin of species. From the viewpoint of a zoologist. 

New York, Columbia Umversity Press, pp. xiv + 334. 

McGiU, M. E. 1946. Company keeping: when is it a sin? Huntington, Ind., Our Sunday 
Visitor Press, pp. 27. 

^ 1946. Does that man love you? Huntington, Ind., Our Sunday Visitor Press, pp. 27. 

McGraw, M. B. 1935. Growth, A study of Johnny and Jimmy. New York, D. Appleton- 
Centuiy Co., pp. xxi + 319. 

McLean, D., and Hubbell, C. 1934. Male impotence. Publ. by author, pp. 16. 



BIBLIOGRAPHY 


779 


McNemar, Q. 1940- Sampling in psychological research. Psych. Bull. 37:331-365. 

1946. Opinion-attitude methodology. Psych. Bull. 43 :289-374. 

McPartland, J. 1947. Sex in our changing world. New York, Rinehart & Co., pp. 
280. 

Mead, M. 1939. From the South Seas. Studies of adolescence and sex in primitive 
societies. New York, William Morrow & Co., pp. xxxv + iii + 304 + 384 -\- 
xiv + 335. 

Mead, W. R., and Stith, R. 1940. Male climacteric. J. So. Car. Medic. Assoc. 36:222-226. 

Meagher, J. F. W. 1924. A study of masturbation and the psychosexual life. Baltimore, 
William Wood & Co. 

Meagher, J. F. W., and Jelliffe, S. E. 1936. A study of masturbation and the psycho- 
sexual life. Baltimore, William Wood & Co., pp. xii + 149. 

Menzies, K. 1921. Autoerotic phenomena in adolescence. An analytical study of the 
psychology and psychopathology of onanism. New York, Paul B. Hoeber, 

pp. 100. 

Meredith, H. V. 1935. The rhythm of physical growth. A study of eighteen anthropo- 
metric measurements on Iowa City white males ranging in age between birth 
and eighteen years. Iowa City, Univ. Iowa Studies in Child Welfare ll.T- 
128. 

1939. Stature of Massachusetts children of North European and Italian ancestry. 

. Amer. J. Physic. Anthrop. 24:301-346. 

Merrill, L. 1918. A summary of findings in a study of sexualism among a group of one 
hundred delinquent boys. J. Juv. Res. 3 :255-267. 

Meyer, F. 1927. Plain talks on marriage. Cincinnati, St. Francis Book Shop, pp. 181. 

1927. Youth’s pathfinder. Heart to heart chats with Catholic young men and 
women. Cincinnati, St. Francis Book Shop, pp. 421. 

1929. Helps to purity. A frank, yet reverent instruction on the intimate matters of 
personal life for adolescent girls. Cincinnati, St. Francis Book Shop, pp. 90. 

1929. Safeguards of chastity. A frank, yet reverent instruction on the intimate 
matters of personal life for young men. Cincmnati, St. Francis Book Shop, 
pp. vii + 84. 

1934. I’m keeping company now! Cincinnati, St. Francis Book Shop, pp. 32. 

1935. On or off with the dance? Cincinnati, St. Francis Book Shop, pp. 16. 

Miles, W. R. 1919. The sex expression of men hving on a lowered nutritional level. 

J. Nerv. & Ment. Dis. 49:208-224. 

1942. Psychological aspects of ageing. In: Cowdry, E. V., 1942. Problems of ageing, 
pp. 756-784. 

Miner, A. H. 1941. Speciation in the avian genus Junco. Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool. 44: 
173-434. 

Miller, G. S. 1928. Some elements of sexual behavior in primates and their possible 
influence on the beginnings of human social development. J. Mammal. 
9:273-293. 

1931. The primate basis of human sexual behavior. Quart. Rev. Biol. 6:379^10. 

Moffett, M. 1942. Youth looks at marriage. A guide for the study of marriage and family 
life. New York, Association Press, pp. 48. 

Moll, A. 1893 (2d edit.). Die kontrare Sexualempfindung. Mit Benutzung amtlichen 
Materials. Berlin, Fischer’s Medicin. Buchhandlung, pp. xiv 394. 

1893. Les perversions de I’instinct genital. Etude sur I’inversion sexuelle basee sur 
des documents oflficiels. Pans, G. Carre, pp. ix 327. 

1897. (edit. 6). Les perversions de I’instinct gdnital. Etude sur I’inversion sexuelle 
basee sur des documents ofiiciels. Paris, G* Carr6 et C. Naud, pp. xxxvi -f- 
ix + 327. 

1898. Untersuchungen fiber die Libido sexualis. Berlin, Fischer’s Medicin. Buch- 
handlung, pp. XV + 872. 

1899 (edit. 3). Die kontrare Sexualempfindung. Berlin, Fischer’s Medicin. Buch- 
handlung, pp. xvi + 652. 

1912. The sexual life of the child. New York, The Macmillan Co., pp. xv -f 339. 

1933. Libido sexualis. Studies in the psychosexual laws of love verified by clinical 
sexual case histories. New York, Amer. Ethnol. Press, pp. 384. 



780 SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 

Moll, A. (edit.). 1921. Handbuch der Sexualwissenschaften mit besonderer Beruck- 
sichtigung der kulturgeschichtlichen Beziehungen. Leipzig, F. C. W. Vogel, 
pp. xxiv + 1046. 

Moll, A. (Popkin, M., transL). 1931. Perversions of the sex instinct. A study of sexual 
inversion based on clinical data and official documents. Newark, N. J., 
Julian Press, pp. 237. 

Moore, C. R. 1942. Comparative biology of testicular and ovarian hormones. Biol. 
Symposia 9:3-10. 

1942. The physiology of the testis and application of male sex hormone. J. Urol. 
47:31-44. 

1943. Sexual differentiation in the opossum after early gonadectomy. J. Exper. 
Zool. 94:415-461. 

1944. Gonad hormones and sex differentiation. Amer. Nat. 78:97-130. 

1944. Sex endocrines in development and prepuberal life. J. Clin. Endocr. 4:135-141. 
Moore, T. V. 1943. The nature and treatment of mental disorders. New York, Grune & 

Stratton, pp. viii + 312. 

1943. Principles of ethics. Philadelphia, J. B. Lippincott Co., pp. xii + 405. 

1945. The pathogenesis and treatment of homosexual disorders: A digest of some 
pertinent evidence. J. Personality 14:47-83. 

Morgan, W. H., and Morgan, M. 1. 1943. Planning for marriage. Outlines for discussion 
by young men and women. New York, Association Press, pp. 85. 

Morley, S. G. 1946. The ancient Maya. Stanford IJniversity, Stanford University Press; 

London, Oxford University Press, pp. xxxii + 520. 

Moses, J. 1922. Konstitution und Erlebnis in der Sexualpsychologie und -pathologic 
des Kindesalters. Ztschr. f. Sexualwiss. 8:305-319. 

Mowrer, O. H., and Kluckhohn, C. 1944. Dynamic theory of personality. Jn: Hunt, 
J. MqV., 1944, Personality and the behavior disorders. New York, Ronald 
Press Co., Vol. 1:69-135. 

Murdock, G. 1934. Our primitive contemporaries. New York, The Macmillan Co., 
pp. xxii + 614. 


Nacke, P. 1908. Beitrage zu den sexuellen Traumen. Archiv f. Kriminalanthropologie 
20:366. 

National Research Council Conference on Mammalian Sex Behavior. 1943. Conference 
on patterns and problems of primate sex-behavior. New York, mimeo. rpt., 

pp. 6. 

Neugebauer, F. L. von. 1908. Hermaphroditismus beim Menschen. Leipzig, Werner 
■^inkhardt, pp. vii + 748. 

Norbury, E. P. 1934. The climacteric period from the viewpoint of mental disorders. 
Med. Rec. 140:605-609, 657-662. 

Northcote, H, 1916. Christianity and sex problems. Philadelphia, F. A. Davis Co., 
pp. xvi + 478. 

Nowlis, V. 1941. Companionship preference and dominance in the social interaction of 
young chimpanzees. Compar. Psychol. Monogr. 17:1-57. 


Painter, T. 1941. Male homosexuals and their prostitutes in contemporary America. 
New York, unpubl. ms., 2 vols. 

Park, R. E., et al. 1939. An outline of the principles of sociology. New York, Barnes & 
Noble, pp. viii + 353.* 

Patten, B. M. 1946. Human embryology. Philadelphia, TheBlakiston Co., pp. xv + 776. 
Pearl, R. 1925 (revis. 1930). The biology of population growth. New Yor£ Alfred A. 
Knopf, pp. xiv + 260. 

1940. Introduction to medical biometry and statistics. Philadelphia, W. B. Saunders 
Co., pp. XV + 537. 

1946. Man the animal. Bloomington, Ind., Piincipia Press, pp. ix + 128. 

Peck, M. W., and WeUs, F. L. 1923. On the psycho-sexuaUty of coUege graduate men 
Ment. Hyg. 7:697-714. ^ ^ ^ 



BIBLIOGRAPHY 781 

1925. Further studies in the psycho-sexuality of college graduate men. Ment. Hyg. 
9:502-520. 

Pedrey, C. P. 1945. A study of voice change in boys between the ages of eleven and 
sixteen. Speech Monogr. 12:30-36. 

Peterson, K. M. 1938. Early sex information and its influence on later sex concepts. 

Unpublished thesis in library of College of Education, University of Colo- 
rado, pp. vii 4- 136. 

Plattner, K. 1930; Eros ini Zuchthaus. Sehnsuchtsschreie gequalter Menschen nach 
Liebe. Eine Beleuchtung der Geschlechtsnot der Gefangenen bearbeitet 
auf der Grundlage von Eigenerlebnissen, Beobachtungen und Mitteilungen 
in achtjahriger Haft. Haimover, P. Witte Verlag, pp. 226. 

Popenoe, P. 1936. Betrothal. Amer. Soc. Hyg. Assoc., Publ. 972:1-15. 

1938. If your daughter pets. Your Life 1938 '.(reprint pp. 1-6). 

1940. Eugenics and family relations. J. Hered. 31 :532-536. 

1940. Modem marriage. A handbook for men. New York, The Macmillan Co., 
pp. xi 4“ 299. 

1943. Marriage before and after. New York, Wilfred Funk, pp. xiv 4” 246. 

1944. Building sex into your life. Los Angeles, Amer. Inst. Family Rel., pp. 22. 
1946. Sexual inadequacy of the male. A manual for counselors. Los Angeles, Amer. 

Inst. Family Rel., pp. 41. 

Popenoe, P. (edit.). 1946. Family life education. Los Angeles, Amer. Inst. Fam. Rel. 6, 

pp. 10. 

Pratt, J. P. 1942. A personal note on methyl testosterone in hypogonadism. J. Clin. 
Endocr. 2:460-464. 

Prince, M. 1905. The dissociation of a personality. A biographical study in abnormal 
psychology. London, Longmans, Green, & Co., pp. x 4- 569. 


Radin, P. 1920. The autobiography of a Winnebago Indian, Univ. Calif. Publ, Amer. 
Archaeol. Ethnol. 16:381^73. 

1926. Crashing Thunder. The autobiography of an American Indian. New York, 
D. Appleton and Co., pp. xxv 4- 203. 

Rado*, S. 1940. A critical examination of the concept of bisexuality. Psychosom. Med. 
2:459-467. 

Ramsey, G. V. 1943. The sexual development of boys. Amer. J. Psych. 56:217-234. 

1943. The sex information of younger boys. Amer. J. Orthopsychiat. 13:347-352. 

Ratzel, F. (Butler, A. J., transl.). 1896. The histoiy of mankind. New York, The Mac- 
millan Co., Vol. 1, pp. xxiv 4- 486. 

Redemptorist Father. 1946. Aids to purity. Huntington, Ind., Our Sunday Visitor Press, 
pp. 32. 

Reich, W. 1942. The function of the orgasm. Sex-economic problems of biological energy. 

(Running title: The discovery of the orgone). New York, Orgone Inst. Press, 
pp. xxxvi 4“ 368. 

Reichard, G. 1938. Social life. In: Boas, F., 1938, General anthropology. New York, 
D. C. Heath & Co., pp. xi 4- 718. 

Rice, T. B. 1933. How life goes on and on. A story for girls of high school age. Chicago, 
American Medical Association, pp. 39. 

1933. In training, for boys of high school age. Chicago, American Medical Associa- 
tion, pp. 48. 

1933. The age of romance. Chicago, American Medical Association, pp. 44. 

1933. The venereal diseases. Chicago, American’ Medical Association, pp. 39. 

1946. Sex, marriage and family. Philadelphia, J. B. Lippincott Co., pp. 272. 

Richey, H. G. 1931. The blood pressure in boys and girls before and after puberty. Its 
relation to growth and to maturity. Amer. J. Dis. Child. 42:1281-1330. 

Robie, W. F. 1924. Sex and life. What the experienced should teach and what the in- 
experienced should learn. Ithaca, N. Y., Rational Life Publishing Co., 
pp. 424. 

1925. The art of love. London, Medic. Res. Soc., pp. vi 4- 386. 

1927. Rational sex ethics and further investigations. A psychological and physio- 



782 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


logical study of the sex lives of normal men and women. Ithaca, N. Y., 
Rational Life Publishing Co., pp. 333. 

Robinson, S. 1938. Experimental studies of physical fitness in relation to age. Arbeits- 
physiol. 10:251-323. 

Robinson, V. (edit.). 1936. Encyclopaedia sexualis. New York, Dingwall-Rock, pp. 
XX + 819. 

Robinson, W. J. 1933. Medical and sex dictionary. New York, Eugenics Publishing Co., 
pp. 187. 

1936, Sexual ethics. In: Robinson, V., Encylopaedia sexualis, pp. 209-221. New 
York, Dingwall-Rock. 

Rockwood, L. D., and Ford, M. E. N. 1945. Youth, marriage, and parenthood. The 
attitudes of 364 university juniors and seniors toward courtship, marriage, 
and parenthood. New York, John Wiley & Sons, pp. xiii -f 298. 

Rogers, C. R. 1942. Counseling and psychotherapy. Newer concepts in practice. New 
York, Houghton Mifflin Co., pp. xiv 4- 450. 

Rohleder, H. 1902 (edit. 2). Die Masturbation. Eine Monographic fiir Arzte, Padagogen 
und gebildete Eltem. Berlin, Fischer’s Medicin. Buchhandlung, pp. xxiii -f- 
336. 

1907. Vorlesungen fiber Geschlechtstrieb imd gesamtes Geschlechtsleben des 
Menschen. Vol. 1 : Das normale, anormale und paradoxe Geschlechtsleben. 
Berlin, Fischer’s Medicin. Buchhandlung, pp. xvi -f 600. 

1907. Vorlesungen fiber Geschlechtstneb und gesamtes Geschlechtsleben des 
Menschen. Vol. 2: Das perverse Geschlechtsleben des Menschen, auch vom 
Standpunkte der lex lata und der lex ferenda. Berlin, Fischer’s Medicin. 
Buchhandlung, pp. vi + 545. 

1918. Normale, pathologische und kfinstliche Zeugung beim Menschen. Volume I 
of: Monographien fiber die Zeugung beim Menschen. Leipzig, G. Thieme 
Verlag, pp, xvi -1-317. 

1913. Die Funktionsstdrungen der Zeugung beim Manne (Samenflfisse, Impotenz, 
Sterilitat). Volume III of: Monographien fiber die Zeugung beim Menschen. 
Leipzig, G. Thieme Verlag, pp. ix -f 235. 

1914. Die libidinosen Funktionsstorungen der Zeugung beim Weib. Volume IV of: 
Monographien fiber die Zeugung beim Menschen. Leipzig, G. Thieme Verlag, 
pp. viii -f 99. 

1921. Die kfinstliche Zeugung (Befruchtung) im Tierreich. Volume VII (supple- 
mentary volume) of: Monographien fiber die Zeugung beim Menschen. 
Leipzig, G. Thieme Verlag, pp. x -|- 128. 

1921 (4th edit.). Die Masturbation. Eine Monographie ffir Arzte, Padagogen und 
gebildete Eltem. Berlin, Fischer’s Medicin. Buchhandlung, pp. xxvii -{- 384. 

1923. Sexualphilosophie und Sexualethik. Volume IV of Monographien zur Sexual- 
wissenschaft. Leipzig, E. Oldenburg Verlag, pp. 104. 

1923-1925. Vorlesungen fiber das gesamte Geschlechtsleben des Menschen. Bd. 1. 
1923. Das normale, anormale und paradoxe Geschlechtsleben. Bd. 2. 1923. 
Die normale und anormale Kohabitation und Konzeption (Befruchtung). 
Pp. xiii -{- 357. Bd. 3. 1925. Das perverse heterosexuelle und automonosex- 
uelle Geschlechtsleben. Pp. xv -f 421. Bd, 4. 1925. Die homosexuellen 
Perversionen des Menschen auch vom Standpunkt der lex lata und lex 
ferenda. Pp. xii -f 403. Berlin, Fischer’s Medicin. Buchhandlung. 

Rosanoff, A. J. 1938. Manual of psychiatry and mental hygiene. New York, John Wiley 
& Sons, pp. xviii + 1091. 

Rosenzweig, S., and Hoskins, R.'G. 1941. A note on the ineffectualness of sex-hormone 
medication in a case of pronounced homosexuality. Psychosom. Med. 
3:87-89. 

Ruland, L. (Rattler, T. A., transL). 1934. Pastoral medicine. St. Louis and London, 
B. Herder Book Co., pp. viii -p 344. 


Sadler, W. S., and Sadler, L. K. 1944 (orig. 1938). Living a sane sex life. New York and 
Chicago, Wilcox & FoUett, pp. xii + 344. 



BIBLIOGRAPHY 783 

Schapera, I. 1941. Married life in an African tribe. New York, Sheridan House, pp. 
xvii + 364. 

Schapiro, B. 1943, Premature ejaculation. A review of 1130 cases. J. Urol. 50:374-379. 

Schonfeld, W. A. 1943. Management of male pubescence. J. Amer. Med. Assoc. 121 : 
177-182. 

1943. Primary and secondary sexual characteristics. Study of their development in 
males from birth through maturity, with biometric study of penis and testes. 
Amer. J. Dis. Child. 65:535-549. 

‘ Schonfeld, W. A., and Beebe, G. W. 1942. Normal growth and variation in the male 
genitalia from birth to maturity. J. Urol. 48 :759-777. 

Sears, R. R. 1943. Survey of objective studies of psychoanalytic concepts. Soc. Sci. Res. 
Council Bull. 51, pp. xiv + 156. 

Selective Service System. 1941. Analysis of reports of physical examination. Summary of 
data from 19,923 reports of physical examination. Washington, Select. 
Serv. Syst., Med. Statis. Bull. 1, pp. 31. 

1943. Causes of rejection and incidence of defects. Local board examinations of 
Selective Service registrants m peacetime. An analysis of reports of physical 
examination from 21 selected states. Washington, Select. Serv. Syst., Medic. 
Statis. Bull. 2, pp. 41 . 

1944. Physical examinations of Selective Service registrants during wartime. An 
analysis of reports for the continental United States and each state. April 
1942-December 1943. Washington, Select. Serv. Syst., Medic. Statis. Bull. 3, 
pp. iv 4“ 137. 

1946. Physical examinations of Selective Service registrants in the final months of 
the war. An analysis of national and state data. January 1944-August 1945. 
Washington, Select. Serv. Syst., Medic. Statis. Bull. 4, pp. 102. 

Severinghaus, E. L., and Chornyak, J. 1945. A study of homosexual adult males. Psy- 
chosom. Med. 7 :302-305. 

Seward, G. H. 1946. Sex and the social order. New York, McGraw-Hill Book Co., 
pp. ix -f- 301. 

Sewell, W, H. 1942. The development of a sociometric scale. Sociometry 5 :279-297. 

Shaw, C. R. 1930. The jack-roller. A delmquent boy’s own story. Chicago, University of 
' Chicago Press, pp. xv -f 205. 

Shuttleworth, F. K. 1937. Sexual maturation and the physical growth of girls age six to 
nineteen. Washington, Soc. Res. Child Develop., Nat. Res. Council 12, 
pp. XX 4- 253. 

1939. The physical and mental growth of girls and boys age six to nineteen in 
relation to age at maximum growth. Washington, Soc. Res. Child Develop., 
Nat. Res. Council 22, pp. vi 4- 291. 

1941. Sampling errors involved in incomplete returns to mail questionnaires. 
J. Appl. Psych. 25:588-591. 

Simmons, L. W. 1942. Sun Chief. The autobiography of a Hopi Indian. New Haven, 
Conn., Yale University Press, pp. xi 4- 460. 

Snedecor, G. W. 1946. Statistical methods applied to experiments in agriculture and 
biology. Ames, Iowa State College Press, pp. xvi + 485. 

Snow, W. F. 1933, 1937, 1941. Marriage and parenthood. Amer. Soc. Hyg. Assoc., 
Publ. No. A-54, A-329, pp. 15. 

1937, 1941. Health for man and boy. New York, Amer. Soc. Hyg. Assoc., pp. 16. 

Sokolowsky, A. 1923. The sexual life of the anthropoid apes. Urol. & Cutan. Rev. 
27:612-615. 

Sorokin, P. 1927. Social mobility. New York, Harper & Brothers, pp. xvii + 559. 

Spott, R., and Kroeber, A. L. 1942. Yurok narratives. Univ. Calif. Publ. Amer. Archaeol. 
&Etlinol. 35:143-256. 

Squier, R. 1938. The medical basis of intelligent sexual practice. In: Folsom, J. K., Plan 
for marriage. New York, Harper &. Brothers, pp. 113-137. 

Stekel, W. 1920* Onanie und Homosexualitat. (Die homosexuelle Neurose). Volume II 
of: Storungen des Trieb- und Affektlebens. (Die parapathischen Erkran- 
kungen). Berlin und Wien, Urban & Schwarzenberg, pp. xii 4- 527. 

1920. Die Gesdilechtskalte der Frau. (Eine Psychopathologie des weiblichen 



784 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


Liebeslebens). Volume III of: Stomngen des Trieb- und Affektlebens. (Die 
parapathischen Erkrankungen). Berlin und Wien, Urban & Schwarzenberg, 
pp. xii + 402. 

1920. Die Impotenz des Mannes. (Die psychischen Storungen der niannlichen 
Sexualfunktion). Volume IV of: Storungen des Trieb- und Affektlebens. 
(Die parapathischen Erkrankungen). Berlin, Urban & Schwarzenberg, pp. 
xi -f 484. 

1922. Psychosexueller Infantilismus. (Die seelischen Kinderkrankheiten der 
Erwachsenen). Volume V of: Storungen des Trieb- und Affektlebens. (Die 
parapathischen Erkrankungen), Berlin und Wien, Urban & Schwarzenberg, 
pp. xii + 616. 

1927. Zwang und Zweifel (fiir Arzte und Medizmer dargestellt). First part. Volume 
IX of: Storungen des Trieb- und Affektlebens. (Die parapathischen Erkran- 
kungen). Berlin und Wien, Urban & Schwarzenberg, pp. 633. 

1928. Zwang und Zweifel (fiir Arzte und Mediziner dargestellt). Second part. 
Volume X of: Storungen des Trieb- und Affektlebens. (Die parapathischen 
Erkrankungen). Berlin und Wien, Urban & Schwarzenberg, pp. 672. 

Stekel, W. (van Teslaar, J. S., transl.). 1922. Bi-sexual love. The homosexual neurosis. 
Boston, R. G. Badger, pp. viii -f 359. 

1924. Peculiarities of behavior. Wandering mania, dipsomania, cleptomania, 
pyromania and allied acts. New York, Liveright Publishing Co., Vol. 1, 
pp. xiv + 328; Vol. 2, pp. x -f 341. 

1926. Frigidity in woman in relation to her love life. New York, Liveright Publish- 
ing Co., Vol. 1, pp. viii 4- 304; Vol. 2, pp. vii 4-314. 

Stekel, W. (Boltz, O. H., transl.). 1927. Impotence in the male. The psychic disorders 
of sexual functions m the male. New York, Liveright Publishing Co., Vol. 1, 
pp. viii 4- 329; Vol. 2, pp. viii 4- 350. 

Stekel, W. (Brink, L., transl.). 1929. Sadism and masochism. The psychology of hatred 
and cruelty. New York, Liveright Pubhshing Co., Vol. 1, pp. xvii -j- 441 ; 
Vol. 2, pp. xiii 4- 473. 

Stekel, W. (Parker, S., transl.), 1930. Sexual abberations. The phenomena of fetishism 
in relation to sex. New York, Livenght Publishing Co., Vol. 1, pp. ix 4- 369; 
Vol. 2, pp- vi + 355. 

Stekel, W. (Tannenbaum, S. A., transl.). 1932. Twelve essays on sex and psychoanalysis. 
New York, Eugenics Publishing Co., pp. 320. 

Stephan, F. F., Deming, W. E., and Hansen, M. H. 1940. The sampling procedure of 
the 1940 population census. J. Amer. Stat. Assoc. 35:615-630. 

Stone, C. P. 1924. A note on “feminine” behavior in adult male rats. Amer. J. Physiol. 
68:39. 

1926. The effects of cerebral destruction on the sexual behavior of male rabbits. 
III. The frontal, parietal, and occipital regions. J. Comp. Psych. 6:435-448. 

1932. The retention of copulatory activity in male rabbits following castration. 
Pedag. Semin. & J. Genet. Psych. 40:296-305. 

1939. Sex drive. ln‘ Allen, E., Sex and internal secretions. Baltimore, Williams & 
Wilkins Co,, pp. 1213-1262. 

Stone, C. P,, Tomilin, M. I., and Barker, R. G. 1935. A comparative study of the sexual 
drive in adult male rats as measured by direct copulatory tests and by the 
Columbia obstruction apparatus. J. Comp. Psych. 19:215-241. 

Stone, C. P., and Ferguson, L. W. 1940. Temporal relationships in the copulatory acts 
of adult male rats. J. Comp. Psych. 30:419-433. 

Stone, H. M., and Stone, A. 1937. A marriage manual. A practical guide-book to sex 
and marriage. New York, Simon & Schuster, pp. xi 4 334. 

Strain, F. B. 1934. New patterns in sex teaching. The normal sex interests of children 
and their guidance from infancy to adolescence. New York, D. Appleton- 
Century Co., pp. xvi 4 242. 

Strakosch, F. M. 1934. Factors in the sex life of seven hundred psychopathic women. 
Utica, New York, State Hospitals Press, pp. 102. 



Bibliography 


785 


Talmud (Rodkinson, M. L., edit.). 1896, 1899, 1903, 1916. New edition of the Baby- 
lonian Talmud. Original text, edited, corrected, formulated, and translated 
into English. Boston, Talmud Soc., Vols. 1-10. 

Talmud (Epstein, L, and Simon, M. edit.). 1936 edit. The Babylonian Talmud. Seder 
Nashim. Gittin. London, Soncino Press, pp. xv + 474. 

Talmud (Epstein, I., Daiches, S., and Slotki, I. W., edit.). 1936 edit. The Babylonian 
Talmud. Seder Nashim. Kethuboth. London, Soncmo Press, Vol. I, pp. 
xvii -I- 432; Vol. II, pp. 433-771. 

Talmud (Epstein, I., and Freedman, H., edit.). 1936 edit. The Babylonian Talmud. 
Seder Nashim. Kiddushin. London, Soncino Press, pp. xiii + 463. 

Talmud (Epstein, I., and Klien, B. D., edit.). 1936 edit. The Babylonian Talmud. Seder 
Nashim. Nazir. London, Soncino Press, pp. xiii -{- 300. 

Talmud (Epstein, I., and Freedman, H., edit.). 1936 edit. The Babylonian Talmud. Seder 
Nashim. Nedarim. London, Soncino Press, pp. xiv -1-312. 

Talmud (Epstein, I., edit., Slotki, I. W., transl.). 1936. The Babylonian Talmud. Seder 
Nashim. Yebamoth. London, Soncino Press, Vol. 1, pp. xlviii -f 440; Vol. 2, 
pp. 441-930. 

Tanner, P. 1946. Youth and chastity. (Adapted from “Marriage and the family” by Dr. 
Jacques Leclercq). Huntington, Ind., Our Sunday Visitor Press, pp. 24. 

Taylor, W. S. 1933. A critique of sublimation in males. A study of forty superior single 
• men. Genet, Psych. Monogr. 13:1-115. 

Terman, L. M. 1930. Talent and genius in children. In: Calverton, V. F. The new genera- 
tion. New York, Macaulay Co., pp. 405-423. 

Terman, L. M., and Miles, C. C. 1936. Sex and personality. Studies in masculinity and 
femininity. New York, McGraw-Hill Book Co., pp. xi 600. 

Terman, L. M., et al. 1938. Psychological factors in marital happiness. New York, 
McGraw-Hill Book Co., pp. xiv -1- 474. 

Thomas, W. I., and Znaniecki, F, 1918-1920. The Polish peasant in Europe and America. 
Boston, Gorham Press, Vols. 1-5. 

1927. The Polish peasant in Europe and America. New York, Alfred A. Knopf. 
Vol. 1, pp. XV -f 1115; Vol. 2, pp. vi 4- 1116-2250. 

Thorndike, E. L. 1940. Human nature and the social order. New York, The Macmillan 
Co., pp. XX -f 1019. 

Thorndike, E. L., Bregman, E. O., Tilton, J. W., and Woodyard, E. 1928. Adult learning. 
New York, The Macmillan Co., pp. x -f 335. 

Thornton, N. 1946. Problems in abnormal behaviour. Philadelphia, The Blakiston Co., 
pp. X -{- 244. 

Thumwald, R. 1931. Banaro society. In: Calverton, V., The making of man. New York, 
Modem Library, pp. xv -f- 879. 


Underhill, R. M. 1936. The autobiography of a Papago woman. Menasha, Wis., Amer. 
Anthrop. Assoc. Memoirs 46:1-64. 

U. S. Census Bureau. 1943. Sixteenth census of the U. S.: 1940. Population. Vol. IV: 

Characteristics by age, marital status, relationship, education, and citizenship. 
Pt. 1: U. S. Summaiy. Washington, U. S. Govt. Print. Off., pp. xii -f- 183. 
1943. Sixteenth census of the U. S.: 1940. Population. Special report on institu- 
tional population 14 years old and over. Characteristics of inmates in penal 
institutions, and in institutions for the delinquent, defective, and dependent. 
Washington, U. S. Govt. Print. Off., pp..iv -f- 361. 

1946. Prisoners in state and federal prisons and reformatories. 1943. Washington, 
U. S. Govt. Print. Off., pp. v 4- 72. 

U. S. Department of Labor. 1939. Dictionary of occupational titles. Part I: Definitions 
of titles. Washington, U. S. Govt. Print. Off., pp. xxxii -f- 1287. 

1939. Dictionary of occupational titles. Part II : Group arrangement of occupational 
titles and codes, Washington, U. S. Govt. Print. Off., pp. xxvi 4- 330. 

1945. Dictionary of occupational titles. Supplement. Edition III. Washington, 
U. S. Govt. Print. Off., pp. viii 4- 747. 

U. S. Navy Department 1940. Regulations governing the admission of candidates into 



786 


SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN THE HUMAN MALE 


the United States Naval Academy as midshipmen and sample examination 
papers. Washington, U. S. Govt. Print. Off., pp. 101. . 

U, S. Public Health Senice. 1921. Keeping fit (for boys). Indianapolis, Ind., State Board 
of Health, pp. 16. 

1934. Keeping fit. A pamphlet for adolescent boys. Washington, tl. S. Publ. Health 
Serv., pp. 15. 

1937. Man-power. Washington, U. S. Publ. Health Serv., V. D. Pamphl. 6: pp. 15. 

Van de Velde, T. H. 1930. Ideal marriage. Its physiology and technique. New York, 
Covici, Friede, pp. xxvi + 336. 

Vatsyayana (Burton, R. F., and Arbuthnot, F. F., transl.). 1883-1925. The Kama Sutra 
of Vatsyayana. Translated from the Sanscrit by The Hindoo Kama Shastra 
Society. Benares-New York, Printed for Society of Friends of India, priv. 
circulat. [Guy dTsere], pp. xxi + 175. 

Vecki, V. G. 1901. The pathology and treatment of sexual impotence. Philadelphia and 
London, W. B. Saunders Co., pp. 329. 

1920. Sexual impotence. Philadelphia and London, W. B. Saunders Co., pp. viii + 


Walker, K., and Strauss, E. B. 1939. Sexual disorders in the male, -Baltimore, Williams 
& Wilkins Co., pp, xiv + 248. 

Warner, W. L., and Lunt, P. S. 1941. The social life of a modem community. New 
Haven, Conn., Yale University Press, pp. xx + 460. 

1942, The status system of a modern community. New Haven, Conn., Yale Univer- 
sity Press, pp. XX -f 246. 

Warner, W. L., and Srole, L. 1945. The social systems of American ethnic groups. New 
Haven, Conn., Yale University Press, pp. xii + 318. 

Washbume, H. C. 1940. Land of the good shadows. The life story of Anauta, an Eskimo 
woman. New York, John Day Co., pp. xix 4- 329. 

Weatherhead, L. D,, and Greaves, M. 1932. The masteiy of sex through psychology and 
religion. New York, The Macmillan Co., pp. xxv + 246. 

Wechsler, D.-1935. The range of human capacities. Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins Co., 
pp. ix -f 159. 

Weinberger, L. M., and Grant, F, C. 1941, Precocious puberty and tumors of the hypo- 
thalamus. Arch. Int. Med. 67:762-792. ^ 

Weisman, A. 1. 1941, Spermatozoa and sterility. A clinical manual. New York, Paul B. 
Hoeber, pp. xvi + 314. 

Weiss, E., and English, O. S. 1943. Psychosomatic medicine. The clinical application of 
psychopathology to general medical problems. Philadelphia, W. B. Saunders 
Co., pp. xxiii + 687. 

Weissenberg, S. 1924. Das Geschlechtsleben der russischen Studentinnen. Ztschr. f. 
Sexualwiss. 11:7-14. 

1924. Das Geschlechtsleben des russischen Studententums der Revolutionszeit. 
Ztschr. f. Sexualwiss. 11:209-216. 

Werner, A. A. 1945. The male climactenc: Report of fifty-four cases. J. Amer. Medic 
Assoc. 127:705-710. 

Westermarck, E. 1922. The history of human marriage. New York, Allerton Book Co 

PP* PP* Vol. 3, pp. viii 4- 587. 

1934. Three essays on sex and marriage. London, Macmillan & Co., pp. ix + 353. 

1936. The future of marriage in western civilization. New York, The Macmillan 
Co., pp. xiv 4- 281. 

Whelpton, P. K., and Kiser, C. V. 1943, Social and psychological factors affecting 
fertihty. I. Differential fertility among 41,498 native-white couples in Indian- 
apolis. Milbank Mem. Fnd. Quart. 21 :221-280. 

1944. Social and psychological factors affecting fertility. IL Variations in the size 
of completed families of 6551 native-white couples in Indianapolis. Milbank 
Mem. Fnd, Quart. 22:72-105. 

1945. Social and psychological factors affecting fertility. HI. The completeness and 



BIBLIOGRAPHY 787 

accuracy of the household survey of Indianapolis. Milbank Mem. Fnd. 
Quart. 23:254-296. 

Wile, 1. S. 1941. Concerning a pamphlet on sex hygiene. Report to 1941 Nat. Inter- 
fraternity Conf. 

1941. Sex in terms of personal and social hygiene. Nat. Interfraternity Conf., pp. 32. 
Willoughby, R. R. 1937. Sexuality in the second decade. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child 
Develop. 10, pp. iv + 57. 

Wissler, C. 1922. (edit. 2). The American Indian. An introduction to the anthropology 
of the New World. New York, Oxford University Press, pp. xxi + 474. 
Witschi, E;, and Mengert, Y/. F. 1942. Endocrine studies on human hermaphrodites 
and their bearing on the interpretation of homosexuality. J. Clin. Endocr. 
2:279-286. 

Wolf, C. 1934. Die Kastration bei sexuellen Perversionen und SittUchkeitsverbrechen 
des Mannes. Basel, Benno Schwabe & Co. Veiiag, pp. xii + 300. 

Wolfe, T. P. 1942. [Review of] P. Schilder “Types of anxiety neuroses.” Inter. J. Psycho- 
anal., 1941. Inter. J. Sex-econ. and Orgone-Res. 1 : 185-1 86. 

Wright, C. A. 1935. Endocrine aspects of homosexuality. Med. Rec. 142:407-410. 

1938. Further studies of endocrine aspects of homosexuality. Med. Rec. 147: 
449-452. 

1939. The sex offender’s endocrines. Med. Rec. 149 :399-402. 

1941. Results of endocrine treatment in a controlled group of homosexual men. 
Med. Rec. 154:60-61. 

1942. Endocrine treatment in homosexual men. Therap. Notes, Feb. 1942:65. 
Wright, R. 1945. Blackboy; a record of childhood and youth. Cleveland, World Publish- 
ing Co., pp. viii + 228. 

Wulffen, E. 1913. Das Kind. Sem Wesen imd seine Entartung. Berlin, P. Langenscheidt 
Verlag, pp. xxiv -f 542. 

Yerkes, R. M. 1939. Sexual behavior in the chimpanzee. Human Biol. 11 :78-lll. 

1939. Social dominance and sexual status in the chimpanzee. Quart. Rev. Biol. 
14:115-136. 

1940. Social behavior of chimpanzees: dominance between mates, in relation to 
• sexual status. J. Comp. Psych. 30:147-186. 

1941. Conjugal contrasts among chimpanzees. J. Abn. & Soc. Psych! 36:175-199. 

1943. Chimpanzees. A laboratory colony. New Haven, Conn., Yale University 
Press, pp. XV 4- 321. 

Yerkes, R. M., and Elder, J. H. 1936. Oestrus, receptivity, and mating in chimpanzee. 
Compar. Psych. Monogr. 13 (5): 1-39. 

1936. The sexual and reproductive cycles of chimpanzee. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 
22:276-283. 

Yerkes, R. M., and Yerkes, A. W. 1929. The great apes. A study of anthropoid life. 
New Haven, Conn., Yale University Press, pp. xix -f 652. 

1935. Social behavior in infrahuman primates. Handbk. Soc. Psych, pp. 973-1033. 
Young, H. H. 1937. Genital abnormalities, hermaphroditism and related adrenal dis- 
eases. Baltimore, William & Wilkins Co., pp. xli + 649. 

Young, K. 1940. Personality and problems of adjustment. New York, F. S. Crofts & Co., 

pp. X + 868. 

Young, W. C., and Orbison, W. D. 1944. Changes in selected features of behavior in 
pairs of oppositely sexed chimpanzees during the sexual cycle and after 
ovariectomy. J. Comp. Psych. 37:107-143. 

Young, W. C., and Rundlett, B. 1939. The hormonal'induction of homosexual behavior 
in the spayed female guinea pig. Psychosom. Medic. 1 :449-460. 

Zeitschrift fiir Sexualwissenschaft (Eulenburg, A., and Bloch, I., edit.). 1914-1932. 

Vols. 1-18. Bonn, Marcus & Webers Verlag. 

Zuckerman, S, 1933. Functional affinities of man, monkeys, and apes. A study of the 
bearings of physiology and behaviour on the taxonomy and phytogeny of 
lemurs, monkeys, apes, and man. New York, Harcourt, Brace and Co., 
pp. xviii 4- 203. 




INDEX 


All numbers refer to pages. Bold face entries refer to more extended treatments 
of each subject. Names of authors cited are italicized. 


Abnormal, defined, 7, 37, 199-203, 385- 
386, 572-581 

Abstinence, 205-213, 297-298, 319-326, 
528-529 

Accumulative incidence 
Animal contacts, 669-673 
Calculating, 114-119 
Extra-marital intercourse, 584-588 
Homosexual, 100-101, 140-141, 145- 
146, 317, 321, 402-403, 617-631, 623, 
650-651, 653-655 
Marital coitus, 253, 281, 564-567 
Marriage, 318-319 

Masturbation, 96-97, 136-137, 144-145, 
311, 317, 398-399, 408-409, 499-502 
Nocturnal emissions, 96-97, 137-138, 
144-145, 398-399, 408-409, 519-522 
Oral contacts, 370-371, 373 
Petting, 97-98, 406-409, 533-537 
Pre-marital intercourse, 139, 141, 145- 
146, 397, 400-401, 404-405, 549-551 
Prostitute intercourse, 100-101, 116- 
H18, 139, 141, 402-403, 597-600 
Total intercourse, 98, 101, 137-138, 
316-317, 396, 400-401, 404-405, 565- 
567 

Achilles, P. S., 23, 28, 499, 519, 552, 766 
Acker son, L., 53, 766 
Active incidence. See Incidence, active. 
Adams, C. R., 546, 560, 766 
Adolescence, 182-192, 297-326 
Defined, 182-183, 298-301 
Female, 183, 187 
Mean age, 187-189 

Physical developments, 130-131, 184- 
185 

Precocious, 185-186 
School grade, 186-187 
Sexual activity, 219-225, 506-507, 523 
Variation, 182-192 
Adolescence, age at onset vs. 

First intercourse, 315-317 
Homosexual, 312, 315, 317, 320-321, 
630 

Marital intercourse, 306-308, 569 
Marriage, 315, 318-319 
Masturbation, 310-313, 317, 324, 507 
Nocturnal emissions, 315, 322, 523 
Onset of activity, 298-303 


Adolescence, age at onset vs.. Continued 
Pre-marital intercourse, 312-315, 324, 
553 

Social level, 188-189 
Source of first ejaculation, 300-301, 303 
Total outlet, 302-308, 324 
Age, determination of, 76-77 
Age, old, 226-227, 235-238, 319-326 
Aging, 218-262, 319-325 
Animal contacts, 260-262, 671-673 
Erotic response, 229-230 
Extra-marital intercourse, 248, 250, 257, 
259, 568, 587-588 

Homosexual, 258-261, 290-292, 629- 
630, 636 

Marital intercourse, 252-257, 278-281, 
285, 567-569 

Masturbation, 238-243, 270-272, 506- 
507 

Nocturnal emissions, 242-245, 274-276, 
521, 523 

Petting, 244-249, 539 
Post-marital outlets, 262-292, 294-296 
Pre-marital intercourse, 248-253, 277- 
288, 552-553 

Prostitute intercourse, 250-253, 281, 
285-288, 601 
Total activity, 218-262 
Total outlet, 219-221, 226-230, 266-267 
Allbutt, T. C., 297, 766 
Allen, C., 207, 514, 766 
Allen, F. L., 39, 766 

American Social Hygiene Association, 514, 
527, 560, 591 

Anal contacts, children, 170-171 
Anal eroticism, 170-171, 579 
Angus, S., 202, 263, 465, 766 
Ammal contacts, 667-678 
Accumulative incidence, 669-673 
Active incidence, 260-262, 362-363, 
46'l-464, 670 

Aging effects, 260-262, 671-673 
Data in interview, 65, 70 
Frequencies, 260-262, 293, 362-363, 
461-464, 671-673 
Marital status, 289, 293, 673 
Pre-adolescent, 174, 671, 673 
Psychologic basis, 675-678 
Rural-urban, 459-464, 670-674 


789 



790 


INDEX 


Animal contacts, Continued 
Significance in total outlet, 378, 488- 
491, 669-670 

Social levels, 362-363, 671 
Variation, 234, 671, 674 
Anthropoids, 205, 222, 510, 549 
Apathy, sexual, 157, 207, 209, 544-546 
Apes, 205, 222, 510, 549 
Appendix on sample size, 736-765 
Arey, L. B., 659, 766 
Armed forces, 197, 388-389, 416-417, 552, 
617, 621-622, 664 
Armitage, R. B., 560, 591, 766 
Army. See Armed forces. 

Artists, 207 

Asceticism, 206, 263, 487 
Assyrian code, 465, 547, 583 
Athletes, sexual activities, 206 
Athletics, data in interview, 64 
Attitudes 

Development in children, 440-447 
Pre-marital intercourse, 364, 381-383, 
559-562 

Auto-eroticism. See Masturbation. 


Babylonian code, 465, 547, 583 
Bailey, E. W., 514, 546, 777 
Balance, heterosexual-homosexual, 636- 
659 

Baldmn, B, T., 183, 185, 766 
Ball, 613, 615 , 167 
Barton, G, A,, 465, 547, 583, 668, 767 
Basal metabolic effects, 204, 309, 313 
Bauer, 227, 767 

Beach, F, A., vii, 181, 369, 371, 373, 510, 
589, 613, 615, 668, 767-768 
Beam, L., 24, 28, 30, 499, 771 
Bell, S,, 163, 768 
Benedict, R., 202, 16% 

Benjamin, H., 591, 606, 768 
Benzedrine, 49 
Bible, 473, 483, 583, 610, 668 
Bibliography, 766-787 
Bigelow, M. A,, 514, 529, 560, 606, 768 
Bingham, H, C„ 167, 205, 222, 373, 768 
Biologic factors of variation, 203-205, 
309-313, 327, 660-663, 678 
Bisexual, 615, 637-638, 656-659 
Biskind, M. S., 204, 768 
Blackwood, B., 547, 583, 768 
Blanton, M, G., 163, 768 
Bloch, L, 34, 513, 768 
Boarding schools, sex in, 225 
Boas, F., 183, 768 
BoHes, M, 27, 29, 497, 514, 111 
Bone, H., 514, 528, 541, 543, 560, 772 
Bowman, H. A., 543, 560, 606, 768 
Boy Scout Manual, 297, 513 


Boys Clubs of America, 514, 530, 543, 560 
Breakdown, twelve-way, 75-82, 92 
Breast 
Knots, 185 

Sensitivity in males, 575 
Stimulation, 367-371, 540, 574-575 
Bridges, K. B., 181, 768 
Brill, A, A., 206-207, 769 
Britten, F. H., 24, 29, 31-33, 499, 533, 552, 
597, 769 

Brockman, F. S„ 207, 499, 769 
Bromley, D. D., 24, 29, 31-33, 499, 533, 
552, 597, 769 
Brown, J. R, 207, 769 
Bruckner, S. J„ 530, 543, 560, 769 
Bryk, F., 377, 547, 559, 583, 769 
Budge, E. A, W., 465, 769 
Bidder, C., 181, 769 
Burgess, E. W., 560, 769 
Business executives, defined, 79 
Butterfield, O. M., 514, 541, 552, 560, 769 


Calendars, sex, 74 
Campbell, E. H„ 163, 769 
Carmichael, L,, 497, 769 
Carpenter, C. R., 205, 510, 769-770 
Casanova, 213 
Case histoiy data, 63-70 
Case history methods. See Interviewing 
techniques. 

Catholic sex patterns, 465-487 
Catholic woman doctor, 543, 560, 770' 
Celibacy, 213, 528 
Chapin, F. 5., 77, 331, 770 
Chappie, E., 547, 583, 770 
Children 

Anal contacts, 170-171 
Anim.al contacts, 174, 671, 673 
Coitus, 162, 166, 169-170, 173-174 
Compamons, 168 
Development of attitudes, 440-447 
Eroticism, 175-181 
Interviewing, 58-59 
Oral contacts, 170-174 
Orgasm, 159-161, 175-181 
Sex play, 66, 68-69, 162-181, 497, 499- 
506 

Sex play, ages, 162, 167, 169, 171-173 
Sex play, heterosexual, 162, 166-174 
Sex play, homosexual, 162, 167-172 
Sex play, incidence, 162, 165-174 
Chivers, W. R,, 560, 770 
Chlenov, 34, 770 
Clark, L., 591, 770 
Clark, P, L., 560, 770 
Clarke, E. L., 543, 560, 606, ' 

Climacteric, male. 111, 235 
Climax. See Orgasm. 



INDEX 


791 


Clinical tables, 679-735 
Married white males, 710-729 
Previously married white males, 730- 
735 

Single white males, 686-709 
Clinics, as sources of data, 201-202 
Clitoris, 575-576, 659 
Cobb, M., 389, 770 
Coding data, 44-45, 71-73 
Coefficients of correlation, 114 
Coital positions, 372-374, 578-579, 604 
Coital techniques, 67, 365-374, 540-542, 
571-582, 604-605 
Coitus, extra-marital, 583-594 
Accumulative incidence, 584-588 
Active incidence, 257, 259, 282, 284, 288, 
348, 350, 352, 412, 431, 456, 585-587 
Aging effects, 248, 250, 257, 259, 568, 
587-588 

Data on histories, 67 
Frequencies, 257, 259, 282-288, 348, 
350, 352, 354, 410, 431, 456, 586-589 
Religious background, 481, 589 
Rural-urban, 456, 588-589 
Significance in total outlet, 489, 492- 
493, 587-588, 590 

Social level, 348, 350-354, 380, 382-383, 
431, 586-588, 592-593 
Two generations, 410, 412, 414 
Variation, 234, 586-589 
Coitus, first, 315-317, 565-566 
Coitus, marital, 563-582 
Active incidence, 252-254, 281, 355- 
356, 412, 432, 459, 482, 564-565 
Age at adolescence, 306-308, 569 
Aging effects, 252-257, 278-281, 285, 
567-569 

Data on histories, 66 
Frequencies, 126-128, 252-257, 278- 
280, 355-356, 410, 432, 459, 482, 569- 
571 

Marital outlet, 252-257, 278-281 
Religious background, 479-482, 569, 
571 

Rural-urban, 455, 459, 569 
Significance in total outlet, 380, 382, 
489, 492-493, 564-568 
Social level, 355-357, 380, 432, 569-570 
Techniques, 365-374, 571-582 
Two generations, 410, 412, 414 
Variation, 234, 569-571 
Coitus, oldest depiction, 374 
Coitus, post-marital, 67, 278-280, 282- 
284, 286-288, 295, 489 
Coitus, pre-adolescent, 162, 166, 169-170, 
173-174 

Coitus, pre-marital, 547-562 
Accumulative incidence, 139, 141, 145- 
146, 397, 400-401, 404-405, 549-551 


Coitus, pre-marital, Continued 
Active incidence, 248-251, 278, 280- 
288, 313-315, 347-352, 430-431, 434- 
435, 456, 458, 480-481, 552-556 
Age at adolescence, 312-315, 324, 553 
Aging effects, 248-253, 277-288, 552- 
553 

Attitudes on, 364, 381-383, 559-562 
Data on histories, 66 
Frequencies, 248-253, 278-288, 314, 
347-355, 430-431, 434-435, 456, 458, 
480-481, 552-556 
Marital status, 277-288 
Religious background, 477-481, 553 
Rural-urban, 455--456, 458, 462, 553, 
556 

Significance in total outlet, 376, 378, 
489-491 » 

Social level, 347-355, 364, 376,381-383, 
426-427, 430-436, 442-445, 549-559 
Social significance, 559-562 
Two generations, 396, 400-401, 404- 
405, 410-413, 556-557 
Variation, 234, 324, 553-555 
Coitus, prostitutes, 595-609 
Accumulative incidence, 100-101, 116- 
118, 139, 141, 402-403, 597-600 
Active incidence, 250-251, 281, 285- 
288, 351-353, 434-435, 458, 600-603 
Aging effects, 250-253, 281, 285-288, 
601 

Data on histories, 67-68 
Frequencies, 250-251 , 285-288, 352-354, 
410-413, 434-435, 442, 458, 601-603 
Marital status, 277-288, 600-601 
Rural-urban, 455, 458, 462 
Significance in total outlet, 376, 378, 
489-493, 597-601 

Social level, 351-355, 376, 434-435, 442, 
588, 598-602 

Social significance, 605-609 
Techniques, 604-605 
Two generations, 396, 402-403, 410- 
413, 603-604 
Variation, 234, 601-603 
Coitus, total 

Accumulative incidence, 98, 101, 137- 
138, 316-317, 396, 400-401, 404-405, 

565- 567 

Aging effects, 256 

Frequencies, 256, 278-280, 348-349, 
358, 410-411, 430-431, 456, 480-481 
Social level, 316, 348-349, 358, 400,404, 
410-411, 413, 430-431, 456, 480-481, 

566- 567 

Two generations, 396, 400-401, 404- 
405, 410 

Common law, Anglo-American, 202-203, 
263-265, 465 



792 


INDEX 


Companions, pre-adolescent, 168 
Compulsions, 202 

Conditioning, psychologic, 157, 204, 216- 
217, 309, 327-328, 440-447, 509, 515- 
518, 523, 529, 569-571, 661, 676-678 
Confidence of record, 44-47 
Conn, J, H., 163, 770 
Conservation, sexual, 297-326, 512-516 
Contacts, animal. See Animal contacts. 
Contacts, homosexual. See Homosexual. 
Contacts, oral, 170-172, 368-373, 540, 
573-578, 605-607, 614-616 
Contraception, techniques, 67 
Contrary to nature, 201-203, 389, 572- 
581, 604, 677-678 
Convulsions, in orgasm, 160-161 
Coon, C., 547, 583, 770 
Cooper, C. R., 395, 770 
Coppens, C., 513, 560, 606, 770 
Copulation. See Coitus. 

Corner, G, W„ 514, 530, 546, 770-771 
Correlation coefficients, 114 
Cottrell, L, S., 560, 769 
Courts, 4, 223-225, 237-238, 263-265, 
389-393, 437, 447, 572, 577-578, 583, 
601, 603, 605, 655, 664-666, 677 
Crampton, C. W., 130, 131, 183, 771 
Crisp, K. B., 513, 530, 560, 606, 771 
Cross-checks, in interviewing, 54-55, 128- 
130 

Cunnilingus, 170-174, 368-373, 540, 573- 
578 

Curry, E. T„ 185, 771 
Curtis, L. k, 204, 778 
Customs. See Mores. 


Daily sexual activity, 205 
Dark and light, preferences for, 581 
Davis, H., All, 503, 543, 560, 771 
Davis, K. B., vii, 24, 28, 34, 771 
Day laborers, 78, 426-427 
Dearborn, W. F., 183, 771 
Deep kissing, 368-369, 540, 574 
Defects, physical, 209 
Delayed activity, 210, 301-303 
Delayed orgasm, 580-581 
Delinquency, contributing to, 237-238 
Delinquency, juvenile, 163-174, 223-225, 
263-265, 389-393, 437, 664-666 
Dependents, defined, 78 
Diaries, sex, 74 

Dickerson, R. R, 297, 514, 530, 543, 546, 
560, 606, 771 

Dickinson, R. L., viii, 24, 28, 31, 34, 499, 
659, 771 

Differential impotence, 517 
Dimock, H. S., 130-131, 183, 771 
Diodes, 297 


Divorce, 295, 544-546, 578, 591-594 
Dream content, 525-527 
Dreams, wet. See Nocturnal emissions. 
Drugs, 49, 521 
Dudycha, G, 163, 771 


Ecclesiastic law, 202, 465 
Economic data, 63-64, 333-334 
Eddy, S„ 514, 528, 543, 560, 591, 597, 606, 
111 

Edson, N. W., 543, 772 
Educational data, 63 

Educational level, 327-417. See also Social 
level. 

Adolescence, age at, 188-189 
Ammal contacts, 362-363, 671 
Coital positions, 312-314, 578-579, 604 
Coital techniques, 365-374, 540-542, 
571-582, 604-605 
Defimtion, 77, 329-331 
Erotic responsiveness, 345, 363-364 
Extra-marital intercourse, 348, 350-354, 
380, 382-383, 431, 586-588, 592-593 
Homosexual, 357-362, 376, 378, 382- 
384, 624-631, 634-636, 640-654 
Marital intercourse, 355-357, 380, 569- 
570 

Masturbation, 339-343, 375-377, 380, 
500-509 

Nocturnal emissions, 342-345, 376, 378, 
380, 520-525 

Nudity, 365-367, 372, 386, 574, 581, 604 
Oral eroticism, 170, 368-373, 574-578, 
605 

Petting, 345-347, 365-373, 376-381, 
534-539 

Pre-marital intercourse, 347-355, 364, 
376, 381-383, 549-559 
Prostitute intercourse, 351-355, 376, 
598-602 

Sources of first ejaculation, 190-192 
Sources of total outlet, 490-493 
Total intercourse, 316, 358, 400, 404, 
410-411, 413, 456, 480-481, 566-567 
Total outlet, 335-339 
Efferz, 291 

Egyptian sex code, 465 
Ejaculation 
Delayed, 580-581 
First, 183-186 

Multiple, 179-180, 215-216, 232-233, 
579 

Nature of, 157-161 
Premature, 580-581 
Source of first, 190-192, 300-303 
Speed of, 178-179, 231, 579-581 
Spontaneous, 190-192, 517-518 
Without orgasm, 159 



INDEX 

Ejaculatoiy impotence, 209 
Elder, /. H,, 205, 373, 772 
Elliott, G. L., 514, 528, 541, 543, 560, 
111 

Ellis, Havelock, 34, 74, 110, 514, 518, 525, 
526, 527, 530, 591, 606, 618-619, 651, 
111 

Emissions, nocturnal. See Nocturnal 
emissions. 

Emotions and sex, 164-165, 192 
Empedocles, 297 

Enforcement of sex law, 4, 223-225, 263- 
265, 389-393, 437, 447, 572, 577, 583, 
601-603, 605, 655, 664-666, 677 
English, O. S., 506, 514, 517, 525, 542, 546, 
111 

Erection 

Aging effects, 230-231 
Angle of penis, 230-231 
Impotence, 209, 236-238, 297, 323, 545 
Morning, 230, 235, 237 
Nature of, 157-161 
Erogenous zones, 573 
Erotic response, 65, 69, 157-161, 216-217, 
517-518, 521, 525-527 
Aging effects, 229-230 
Pre-adolescent, 175-181 
Social level, 210, 345, 363-364, 521 
Erotica, 23, 65 

European patterns, 515, 559, 593 
Evans, W., 591, 778 
Executives, business, defined, 79 
Exhibition, genital, 165-173, 501 
Exner, M. J., 24, 28, 31-33, 499, 527, 529, 
552, 560, 606, 111 
Extra-marital intercourse, 583-594 
Accumulative incidence, 584-588 
Active incidence, 257, 259, 282, 284, 288, 
348, 350, 352, 412, 431, 456, 585-587 
Aging effects, 248, 250, 257, 259, 568, 
587-588 

Data on histories, 67 
Frequencies, 257, 259, 282-288, 348, 
350, 352, 354, 410, 431, 456, 586-589 
Religious background, 481, 589 
Rural-urban, 456, 588, 589 
Significance in total outlet, 489, 492- 
493, 587-588, 590 

Social level, 348, 350-354, 380, 382-383, 
431, 586-588, 592-593 
Two generations, 410, 412, 414 
Variation, 234, 586-589 
Extra-marital petting, 532 


Falk, H. a, 204, 768 
Family, basis of, 563 
Fantasy, in dreams, 525-527 
Fantasy, in masturbation, 510-511, 523 


793 

Fantasy, in social levels, 210, 345, 363-364, 
521 

Faris, R, E. L., 606, 776 
Fatigue, physical, 206 
Feeble-minded, 49, 53, 55, 608 
Fehlinger, H., 547, 559, 111 
Fellation, 170-172, 368-373, 540, 573-578, 
616 

Fellation, self, 510 
Female 

Adolescence, 183, 187 
Comparison with male, 157, 167, 183, 
187, 216, 223, 544-546, 571, 575-576, 
581, 589-590 
Masturbation, 576 
Pre-adolescent play, 157, 167 
Pre-marital activity, 223, 544-546 
Prostitution, 61, 67-68, 216, 595-609 
Unresponsiveness, 157, 207, 209, 544- 
546 

Feminine behavior, 613-615 
Fertility, adolescent, 185 
Fetish, 607 

Finger, F. W,, 15, 19, 31, 33, 499, 552, 621, 
111 

Flagellation, 607 

Fleege, U. H, 503, 543, 773 

Folsom, J, K,, 618, 773 

Forbush, W. B., 543, 560, 773 

Ford, C. S,, 547, 583, 773 

Ford, F. R., 186, 773 

Ford,M.E. N., 591, 782 

Forel, A., 513, 591, 606, 773 

Fortune, R. F, 547, 583, 773 

Frank, H,, 543, 560, 773 

Frank, R„ 514, 528, 541, 543, 560, 773 

Frequencies 

Animal contacts, 260-262, 293, 362- 
363, 461-464, 671-673 
Daily, 205 

Excessive, defined, 199-203, 213-217 
Extra-marital intercourse, 257, 259, 
282-288, 348, 350, 352, 354, 410, 431, 
456, 586-589 
High, 197-203, 213-217 
Homosexual, 258-261, 290-293, 320, 
357, 359-361, 410, 438-439, 442, 460, 
484, 631-636 
Low, 205-213 

Marital intercourse, 126-128, 252-257, 
278-280, 355-356, 410, 432, 459, 482, 
569-571 

Masturbation, 238-243, 270-273, 310- 
313, 339-341, 422-423, 450, 470-471, 
504-509 

Nocturnal emissions, 242-245, 274-277, 
315, 322, 342-345, 406-407, 410, 424- 
425, 442, 452-453, 474-476, 521-525 
Personality correlations, 325-326 



794 


INDEX 


Frequencies, Continued 
Petting, 244-249, 345-347, 407, 410, 
428-429, 442, 453-454, 477-478, 533, 
538-539 

Pre-marital intercourse, 248-253, 278- 
288, 314, 347-355, 430-431, 434-435, 
456, 458, 480-481, 552-556 
Prostitute intercourse, 250-251, 285- 
288, 352-354, 410-413, 434-435, 442, 
458, 601-603 

Total intercourse, 256, 278-280, 348- 
349, 358, 410-411, 430-431, 456, 480- 
481 

Total outlet, 193-217, 220-221, 226, 
266-269, 273, 302-307, 336-338, 410, 
420-421, 448, 466-467 
VaHdity of data, 123-126, 135, 143, 147, 
149, 152-153 

Frequency curve. 111, 198-201 
Frequency statistics, 110-114 
Freud, 5., 34, 163, 180-181, 206-207, 263, 
497, 525 

Frigidity, female, 157, 207, 209, 544-546 


Gallichan, W, M., 560, 773 
Gartland, F. E., 543, 560, 773 
Generations, older vs. younger, 150, 368, 
372, 394-417 

Genital data on histories, 64-65 
Gemtal stimulation, 367-373, 540-541, 
575-578 

Geographic breakdown, 81 
Gerontology, 226-227, 235-238, 319-325 
Gillis, J, M., 560, 773 
GI patterns, 197, 388-389, 416-417, 437, 
552 , 617, 621-622, 664 
Girls. See Female. 

Goldschmidt, R., 657-659, 661-662, 773 
Grade in school, vs. adolescence, 186- 
187 

Grant, F, C., 186, 786 

Greece, 202, 465, 526, 613, 660 

Greulich, W, W., 183, 774 

Griffin, L, F., 543, 560, 774 

Group activity, 67, 169-170, 501, 607, 675 

Group frequencies, 110-114 

Growth in height, 183-185 

Gruenberg, B. C., 514, 530, 774 

Guild, H., 186, 773 

Gynandromorph, 658 


Hair, pubic, 130-131, 183-185 
Haire, N., 499, 514, 517, 606, 618, 774 
Hall, G. S., 513, 529, 774 
Hall, W, S., 291, 513, 527, 528-560, 606, 
774 

Halverson, H. M., 163-164, 774 


Hamilton, G. V., vii, 25, 28, 32-34, 167, 
175, 204, 222, 499, 519, 533, 552, 585, 
591, 621, 774 

Handicapped persons, 608 
Harper, R. F., 465, 547, 583, 774 
Hartland, E., 583, 774 
Hattendorf, K, W., 163, 774 
Health, affecting sexual activity, 64-65, 
204-206, 209 

Hebrews. See Bible, Talmud. 

Height, development, 183-185 

Heller, C. G., 204, 227, 774 

Henry, G. W., 34, 207, 514, 515, 612, 775 

Heredity, sexual, 203-204, 661-663 

Hermaphrodite, 526, 658 

Hermaphroditism, psychosexual, 612 

Heterosexual. See the following: 

Coital positions 
Coital techniques 
Coitus, extra-marital 
Coitus, marital 
Coitus, post-marital 
Coitus, pre-adolescent 
Coitus, pre-mantal 
Coitus, prostitute 
Coitus, total 
Erotic response 
Female 
Incest 
Kissing 
Oral eroticism 
Petting 

Pre-adolescent sex play, heterosexual 
Heterosexual-homosexual balance, 636- 
659 

Heterosexual-homosexual rating scale, 
638-641 

High frequencies of outlet, 195, 197-198, 
213-217 

Hildebrand, D, V., 560, 775 
Himes, N. K, 514, 546, 775 
Hirschfeld, M., 34, 618-620, 651, 775 
Hirsh, E., 103, 778 
History, items covered, 63-70 
History of study, 3-34 
Hittite code, 465, 547, 583, 668 
Hohman, L, 5., 26, 29, 499, 552, 621-622, 
115 

Hollingshead, A. B., 77, 331, 775 
Homosexual, 610-666 
Accumulative incidence, 100-101, 140- 
141, 145-146, 317, 321, 402-403, 617- 
631, 623, 650-651, 653-655 
Active incidence, 629-631, 640-652, 
650-652 

Age at adolescence, 312, 315, 317, 320- 
321, 630 

Aging effects, 258-261, 290-292, 629- 
630, 636 



INDEX 


795 


Homosexual, Continued 
Data on histories, 65, 68-70 
Defined, 577, 612-617, 623 
Etiologic factors, 660-663 
Frequencies, 258-261, 290-293, 320, 
357, 359-361, 410, 438-439, 442, 460, 
484, 631-636 
Inheritance, 661-663 

- Institutional, 129-130, 210, 224, 529, 
617, 647, 664 

Marital status, 285, 289-292, 631 
Neuroses, 201-203, 660 
Oral contacts, 370 
Post-marital, 290-292, 295-296 
Pre-adolescent, 162, 167-172 
Prison, 129-130, 210, 224, 529, 617, 647, 
664 

Prostitution, 70, 216, 596 
Re-direction, 608, 660 
Religious background, 482-484, 631, 
636- 

Rural-urban, 455-460, 630-631, 636 
Significance in total outlet, 376, 378, 
382, 488-493, 610 

Social level, 357-362, 376, 378, 382-384, 
438-439, 442, 624-631, 634-636, 640- 
654 

Social significance, 663-666 
Two generations, 396, 402-403, 410, 
412-414, 631 

Validity of data, 90, 100-101, 123, 134- 
135, 140-147, 618-622, 625-627 
Variation, 234, 633-636 
Homosexual-heterosexual ratmg scale, 
638-641 

Honigmann, L, 103, llS 
Hormone, male, 204, 615, 659 
Hormone, thyroid, 204 
Hotchkiss, R, S., 527, 776 
Hughes, W. L., 26, 28, 499, 519, 776 
Hundred percent samples, 93-102, 
133 

Hunt, J. McV., 497, 606, 776 
Hyman, H. T., 514 , 116 
Hypersexual, defined, 199 
Hypospadia, 659 


Impotence, 158, 209, 236-238, 297, 323, 
517, 545 
Differential, 517 
Ejaculatory, 209 
Premature ejaculation, 580-581 
Incest, 558 

Incidence, accumulative. See Accumula- 
tive incidence. 

Incidence, active 

Animal contacts, 260-262, 362-363, 
461-464, 670 


Incidence, active, Continued 
Extra-marital intercourse, 257, 259, 282, 
284, 288, 348, 350, 352, 412, 431, 456, 
585-587 

Homosexual, 629-631, 640-652,650-652 
Marital intercourse, 252-254, 281, 355- 
356, 412, 432, 459, 482, 564-565 
Masturbation, 238-241, 270-273, 277, 
310, 313, 339-340, 422-423, 450, 470- 
471, 507-509 

Nocturnal emissions, 242-243, 274-277, 
322, 342-344, 412, 424-425, 452, 474- 
475, 519-525 

Petting, 244-246, 345-346, 412, 428- 
429, 454, 478, 533, 539 
Pre-adolescent sex play, 162, 165-174 
Pre-marital intercourse, 24^251, 278, 
280-288, 313-315, 347-352, 43(M31, 
434-435, 456, 458, 480-481, 552-556 
Prostitute intercourse, 250-251, 281, 
285-286, 351-353, 434-435, 458, 600- 
603 

Incidence data, validity, 121-124, 135-143, 
147, 149, 152 

Individual variation. See Variation, indi- 
vidual. 

Industrial problems, 386-387 
Infants, sexual response, 160, 163-164, 
175-181, 497-506, 542-543 
Infidelity, marital. See Coitus, extra-mar- 
ital. 

Inheritance, sexual, 203-204, 661-663 
Inhibitions, 211-213, 364-367, 369-374, 
381-382, 472, 483-487, 542-546, 561, 
573-578 
Institutions 

Homosexual in, 129-130, 210, 224, 529, 
617, 647, 664 

Mental, 49, 53, 55, 197, 224, 608, 617, 
647, 664 

Penal, 129-130, 197, 210, 224-225, 386, 
390-393, 528-529, 617, 647, 664 
Intercourse, extra-marital, 583-594 
Accumulative incidence, 584-588 
Active incidence, 257, 259, 282, 284, 288, 
348, 350, 352, 412, 431, 456, 585-587 
Aging effects, 248, 250, 257, 259, 568, 
587-588 

Data on histories, 67 
Frequencies, 257, 259, 282-288, 348, 
350, 3B2, 354, 410, 431, 456, 586-589 
Religious background, 481, 589 
Rural-urban, 456, 588-589 
Sigmficance in total outlet, v, 489, 492- 
493, 587-588, 590 

Social level, 348, 350-354, 380, 382-383, 
431, 586-588, 592-593 
Two generations, 410, 412, 414 
Variation, 234, 586-589 



796 


INDEX 


Intercourse, first, 315-317, 565-566 
Intercourse, marital, 563-582 
Active incidence, 252-254, 281, 355- 
356, 412, 432, 459, 482, 564-567 
Age at adolescence, 306-308, 569 
Aging effects, 252-257, 278-281, 285, 
567-569 

Data on histories, 66 
Frequencies, 126-128, 252-257, 278- 
280, 355-356, 432, 459, 482, 569-571 
Marital outlet, 252-257, 278-281 
Religious background, 479-482, 569, 
571 

Rural-urban, 455, 459, 569 
Significance in total outlet, 380, 382, 
489, 492-493, 564-568 
Social level, 355-357, 380, 432, 569- 
570 

Techniques, 365-374, 571-582 
Two generations, 410, 412, 414 
Variation, 234, 569-571 
Intercourse, oldest depiction, 374 
Intercourse, positions, 372-374, 578-579, 
604 

Intercourse, post-marital, 67, 278-280, 
282-284, 286-288, 295, 489 
Intercourse, pre-adolescent, 162, 166, 169- 
170, 173-174 

Intercourse, pre-marital, 547-562 
Accumulative incidence, 139, 141, 145- 
146, 397, 400-401, 404-405, 549-551 
Active incidence, 248-251, 278, 280- 
288, 313-315, 347-352, 430-431, 434- 
435, 456, 458, 480-481, 552-556 
Age at adolescence, 312-315, 324, 553 
Aging effects, 248-253, 277-288, 552- 
553 

Attitudes on, 364, 381-383, 559-562 
Data on histories, 66 
Frequencies, 248-253, 278-288, 314, 
347-355, 430-431, 434-435, 456, 458, 
480-481, 552-556 
Marital status, 277-288 
Religious background, 477-481, 553 
Rural-urban, 455-456, 458, 462, 553, 
556 

Significance in total outlet, 376, 378, 

489-491 

Social level, 347-355, 364, 376, 381-383, 
426-427, 430-436, 442-445, 549-559 
Social significance, 559-562 
Two generations, 396, 400-401, 404- 
405, 410-413, 556-557 
Variation, 234, 324, 553-555 
Intercourse, prostitute, 595-609 
Accumulative incidence, 100-101, 116- 
118, 139, 141, 402-403, 597-600 
Active incidence, 250-251, 281, 285- 
288, 351-353, 434-435, 458, 600-603 


Intercourse, prostitute, Continued 
Aging effects, 250-253, 281, 285-288, 
601 

Data on histories, 67-68 
Frequencies, 250-251, 285-288, 352- 
354, 410-413, 434-435, 442, 458, 601- 
603 

Marital status, 277-288, 600-601 
Rural-urban, 455, 458, 462 
Significance in total outlet, 376, 378, 
489-493, 597-601 

Social level, 351-355, 376, 434-435, 442, 
588, 598-602 

Social significance, 605-609 
Techniques, 604-605 
Two generations, 396, 402-403, 410- 
413, 603-604 
Variation, 234, 601-603 
Intercourse, techniques, 67, 365-374, 571- 
582, 604-605 
Intercourse, total 

Accumulative incidence, 98, 101, 137- 
138, 316-317, 396, 400-401, 404-405, 

565- 567 

Aging effects, 256 

Frequencies, 256, 278-280, 348-349, 
358, 410-411, 430-431, 456, 480-481 
Social level, 316, 348-349, 358, 400, 404, 
410-411, 413, 430-431, 456, 480-481, 

566- 567 

Two generations, 396, 400-401, 404- 
405, 410 

Intersex, 612, 657-659 
Interspecific mating, 668 
Interview, data on 
Animal contacts, 65, 70 
Heterosexual relations, 65-68 
Homosexual, 65, 68-70 
Marital history, 64 
Masturbation, 65-66 
Nocturnal emissions, 65 
Physical and physiologic, 64-65 
Sex education, 64 

Socio-economic background, 63-64 
Interviewers, comparisons of data, 133- 
143 

Interviewers, training, 59-62 
Interviewing techniques, 35-62 
Children, 58-59 
Coding, 44-45, 71-73 
Confidence of record, 44-47 
Cross-checks, 54-55, 128-130 
Making contacts, 3^1 
Rapport, 41-44, 48 
Recording data, 44-45, 50, 71-73 
Sequence of topics, 48-49 
Validity, 54-55 
Inversion, 612, 614-615 
Isaacs, S„ 163, 776 



INDEX 


797 


Jackson, C. M., 204, 776 
Jefferis, B. G., 513, 527, 543, 560, 776 
Jelhffe, S.E., 497, 560, 779 
Jerome, E. K.,\S5,116 
Jewish sex patterns, 465-487. See also 
Bible, Talmud. 

Job classification, 334 
Jones, H. E., 183, 776 
Jung, F. T., 185, 776 
Juvenile court, 224-225 
Juvenile delinquency, 163-174, 223-225, 
263-265, 389-393, 437, 664-665 


Kahn, F., 215, 776 
Kaukonen, J. L., 514, 530, 774 
Kelly, G., 530, 543, 560, 776 
Kempf, E. J., 222, 776 
Kirkendall, L, A„ 514, 527, 543, 560, 606, 
111 

Kirsch, F, M., 503, 527, 543, 560, 111 
Kiser, C V., 92-93, 786-787 
Kissing, 368-369, 531, 540, 573-578 
Deep, 368-369, 540, 574 
Klemer, D, H., 530, 111 
Kluckhohn, C,, 497, 780 
Knots, breast, 185 
Koch, R. A., 606, 111 
Krafft-Ebing, R„ 34, 175, 111 
Kubitschek, P. E,, 183, 111 


Laborers 
Day, 78, 426-427 
Semi-skilled, 78, 426-427 
Skilled, 78, 427, 433 
Landes, R., 547, 111 

Landis, C., vii, 9, 26-27, 29, 497, 514, 530, 
546, 111 

Laton, A. D., 514, 546, 111 
Law, ecclesiastic, 202, 465 
Law, sex, 4, 223-225, 237-238, 263-265, 
389-393, 437, 447, 572, 577-578, 583, 
601-603, 605, 655, 664-666, 677 
Legman, G,, 612, 775 
Legrain, L., 374, 111 
Lesbian, 613 
Levels. See Social level. 

Levels, educational. See Educational level. 
Levels, occupational. See Occupational 
class. 


Levy, D. M., 163, 111 

Licht, if., 526, 660, 111 

Lieber, L., 513, 527, 778 

Liederman, E, E., 297, 529, 543, 560, 778 

Light and dark, preferences for, 581 

Lindsey, J?. .0., 591, 778 

Linton, R., SAl, 583, 778 

Lips, J., 583, 778 


Lisser, H., 204, 778 

Literature, sex, 22-23 

Lorand, S,, 497, 525, 778 

Lordosis, 613 

Lorge, /., 329, 778 

Lovell, P. M., 514, 517, 530, 778 

Low frequencies of outlet, 205-213 

Lower white collar group, 78, 433 

Lowry, O., 560, 778 

Lunt, P. S., 77, 331, 786 


MacGowan, K., 25, 774 
Male hormone, 204, 615, 659 
Male menopause, 227, 235 
Malinowski, B., 4, 222, 373, 547, 559, 583, 
778 

Mantegazza, P., 34, 778 
Marcuse, M., 34 
Marihuana, 49 
Marines. See Armed forces. 

Marital intercourse, 563-582 
Active incidence, 252-254, 281, 355-356, 
412, 432, 459, 482, 564-565 
Age at adolescence, 306-308, 569 
Aging effects, 252-257, 278-281, 285, 
567-569 

Data on histories, 66 
Frequencies, 126-128, 252-257, 278- 
280, 355-356, 432, 459, 482, 569-571 
Marital outlet, 252-257, 278-281 
Religious background, 479-482, 569, 
571 

Rural-urban, 455, 459, 569 
Significance in total outlet, 380, 382, 
489, 492-493, 564-568 
Social level, 355-357, 380, 432, 569-570 
Techniques, 365-374, 571-582 
Two generations, 410, 412, 414 
Variation, 234, 569-571 
Marital maladjustment, 544-546, 578, 
591-594 

Mantal status, 263-296 
Animal contacts, 289, 293, 673 
Defined, 76 

Homosexual, 285, 289-292, 631 
Marital outlet, 252-257, 278-281 
Masturbation, 270-273, 277, 507-508 
Nocturnal emissions, 274-277, 519, 521 
Petting, 277, 532 
Pre-marital intercourse, 277-288 
Prostitute intercourse, 277-288, 600-601 
Total outlet, 266-273 
Marriage 

Accumulative incidence, 318-319 
Adjustments, 542-546, 561-562, 571, 
578, 591-593 

Age at adolescence, 315, 318-319 
Choice of mate, 197 



798 


INDEX 


Marriage, Continued 
Counseling, 386-387, 543, 545 
Data on histories, 64 
Functions, 563 

Martindale, C. C., 560, 606, 778 
Masculine behavior, 613-614 
Masculinity-femininity scale, 637-638 
Maslow, A. H., 103-104, 778 
Masochism, 60, 510, 526, 607 
Masturbation, 497-516 
Accumulative incidence, 96-97, 136- 
137, 144-145, 311, 317, 398-399, 408- 
409, 499-502 

Active incidence, 238-241, 270-273, 
277, 310, 313, 339-340, 422-423, 450, 
470-471, 507-508 

Age at adolescence, 310-313, 317, 324, 
507 

Aging effects, 238-243, 270-272, 506- 
507 

Animal, 675-676 
Data on histones, 65-66 
Defined, 497-498 
Exhibitionistic, 169, 501 
Fantasies, 510-511, 523 
Female, 576 

Frequencies, 238-243, 270-273, 310- 
313, 339-341, 422-423, 450, 470-471, 
504-509 

Harmful effects, 437, 512-516 
Infant, 163, 497-506 
Mantal status, 270-273, -277, 507-508 
Mutual, 170, 616 
Post-marital, 270-272, 294 
Religious background, 470-473, 476, 
509 

Rural-urban, 450, 453, 462, 509 
Significance in total outlet, 376, 378, 
380, 382, 488-493, 506-509 
Social level, 339-343, 375-378, 380, 
422-423, 442, 444, 500-509 
Techniques, 509-511 
Two generations, 396-399, 408-412, 
508-509 

Variation, 91, 234, 324, 504-506 
Mating, interspecific, 668 
Mating, sources of attaction, 667-668 
May, G., 4, 202, 263, 297, 465, 483, 778 
McGill, M. E., 543, 560, 778 
McNemar, Q,, 32, 42, 52, 779 
McPartland, 395, 606, 619, 779 
Mead, M., 227, 547, 559, 583, 779 
Meagher, J, F. W., 497, 560, 779 
Means, defined, 112 
Medians, defined, 113-114 
Memory, immediate vs, remote, 148-150 
Memory, vahdity, 120-153, 148-150, 183- 
185 

Menarche, 187 


Menopause, male, 227, 235 
Menstruation, 65, 187 
Mental institutions, 49, 53, 55, T97, 224, 
608, 617, 647, 664 
Meredith, H. V., 183, 185, 779 
Merrill, L., 27, 28, 31, 175, 499, 779 
Metabolic level, 204, 309, 313 
Method of sex studies, 23-34, 35-119 
Meyer, F., 513, 525, 543, 560, 591, 606, 779 
Miles, W, R., 204, 618, 637, 779 
Miller, G. S., 563, 779 
Mobility, vertical, 417-439 
Moffett, M„ 543, 779 
Moll, A,, 34, 175, 525, 779-780 
Moore, C. R., 204, 780 
Moore, T. V., 181, 202, 513, 543, 560, 618, 
780 

Moral interpretations, 5, 384-386, 678 
Mores 

Origifi, 202-203, 263-265, 465, 468, 
483-487, 547-548 
Transmission, 440-447 
Morgan, W. H., 543, 780 
Morley, S, G., 547, 559, 780 
Mormng erections, 230, 235, 237 
Moses, J,, 175, 780 

Mouth eroticism, 170-171, 368-373, 540, 
573-578, 605-607, 614-616 
Mouth-genital contacts, 170-174, 368- 
373, 510, 540, 573-578, 616 
Mowrer, O. H., 497, 780 
Mucus, pre-coital, 230-233 
Multiple orgasm, 179-180, 215-216, 232- 
233 579 

Murdock, G., 547, 559, 583, 780 
Myers, G. B., Ill, 11 A 


Ndcke, P., 526, 780 
Narcotics, 49, 521 

National Research Council, v, vii, ix, 510, 
668 

Natural law, 202-203, 385-386 
Naval Academy, U. S., 264, 513 
Navy. See Armed forces. 

Negro, histones, 217, 235, 237, 388, 393 
Nelson, W. O., 204 

Nichols, J. L., 513, 527, 543, 560, 776 
Nocturnal emissions, 517-530 
Accumulative incidence, 96-97, 137- 
138, 144-145, 398-399, 408-409, 519- 

522 

Active incidence, 242-243, 274-277, 
322, 342-344, 412, 424-425, 452, 474- 
475, 519-525 

Age at adolescence, 315, 322, 523 
Agmg effects, 242-245, 274-276, 521, 

523 

Data on histories, 65 



INDEX 


799 


Nocturnal emissions, Continued 
Frequencies, 242-245, 274-277, 315, 
322, .342-345, 406-407, 410, 424-425 
442, 452-453, 474-476, 521-525 
Marital status, 274-277, 519, 521 
Post-marital, 274-276, 294-295 
Religious background, 474-476, 525, 
527-528 

Rural-urban, 452-453, 462, 525 
Significance in total outlet, 376, 378, 
380, 382, 488-493 

Social level, 342-345, 376, 378, 380, 382, 
424-425, 442, 444, 520-525 
Two generations, 396-399, 408-410, 
412, 525 

Variation, 234, 521, 524 
Non-marital intercourse. See: 
Extra-marital intercourse 
Post-marital intercourse 
Pre-marital intercourse 
Prostitute intercourse 
Non-sexual stimuli, 164-165 
Norbury,E,P„ 221, 780 
Normal, defined, 7, 37, 199-203, 385-386, 
572-581 

Northcote, H., 465, 780 
Nowlis, K., viii, 222, 373, 780 
Nudity, social level and, 365-367, 372, 
386, 574, 581, 604 
Nutrition, 187, 204 
Nymphomania, defined, 199-203 


Occupational class, 330-362, 417-447. See 
also Social level. 

Definition, 77-79, 330-335, 418-442 
Erotic responsiveness, 345, 363-364 
Extra-marital intercourse, 354, 382-383, 
431, 588 

Homosexual, 357, 361-362, 438 t439, 
442 1 

Marital intercourse, 356-357, 432 
Masturbation, 341, 343, 422-423, 442, 

444 

Nocturnal emissions, 343, 345, 424-425, 
442 444 

Petting, 346-347, 428-429, 442 
Pre-marital intercourse, 349, 351, 353- 
355, 381-383, 426-427, 430-436, 442- 

445 

Prostitute intercourse, 353-355, 434- 
435, 442, 601 

Total intercourse, 348-349, 430-431 
Total outlet, 338-339, 420-421, 442 
Oedipus complex, 315 
Ogive, 115-117 

Old age, 226-227, 235-238, 319-325 
Older vs. younger generations, 150, 368, 
372, 394-417 


Oncers, 383 

Onset of sexual activity, vs. age at ado- 
lescence, 298-303 
Oral contacts 

Accumulative incidence, 370-371, 373 
Homosexual, 370 
Marital, 576-578 
Pre-adolescent, 170-171, 174 
Oral eroticism, 170-171, 368-373, 510, 
540, 573-578, 605-607, 614-616 
Orbison, JV. D., 205, 787 
Orgasm 

Age at first, 175-181, 183-186 
Capacity to delay, 580-581 
Multiple, 179-180, 215-216, 232-233, 
579 

Nature, 157-161, 573-576 
Pre-adolescent, 159-161, 175-181 
Premature, 580-581 

Source of first, 164-165, 190-192, 300- 
303 

Speed, 178-179, 231, 579-581 
Spontaneous, 190-192, 517-518 
Variation, 160-161, 178-180 
Without ejaculation, 159 
Orgastic pleasure, 159 
Outlet, regularity of, 192 
Outlet, total. See Total outlet. 

Over-sexed, defined, 199-203 


Painter, T., 618, 780 

Parole, sexual problems, 225, 391-393. 

See also Law, sex. 

Partial samples, 94-104 
Patten, B, M., 659, 780 
Patterns of behavior, 374-447 
Stability, 394-447 

Pearl, Raymond, 27-28, 76, 89, 193, 213, 
221, 449, 569, 780 

Pearson, G. H. J., 514, 542, 546, 111 
Pearsonian r, 114, 122-123, 126 
Peck, M, W,, vii, 27-28, 31, 33, 499, 519, 
780-781 

Pedrey, C. P., 185, 781 
Penal institutions, 129-130, 197, 210, 224- 
225, 386, 390-393, 528-529, 617, 647, 
664 

Penis, erect angle, 230-231 
Percents of outlet 
Group,' 114 
Individual, 114 
Periodicity, 74, 110 

Personality, vs. sexual frequencies, 325- 
326 

Perversions, defined, 201-203, 264-265, 
389, 572-581, 604, 677-678 
Peterson, K. M., 29-30, 31-33, 499, 519, 
552, 781 



800 


INDEX 


Petting, 531-546 

Accumulative incidence, 97-98, 406- 

409, 533-537 

Active incidence, 244-246, 345-346, 
412, 428-429, 454, 478, 533, 539 
Aging effects, 244-249, 539 
Data on histories, 66-67 
Defined, 531 
' Extra-marital, 532 

Frequencies, 244-249, 345-347, 407, 

410, 428-429, 442, 453-454, 477-478, 
533, 538-539 

Marital status, 277, 532 
Pre-coital, 572-573 
Religious backgrounds, 477-478, 540 
Rural-urban, 453-454, 462, 539 
Significance in total outlet, 376, 378, 
488-491 

Social level, 345-347, 365-373, 376-381, 
428-429, 442, 534-539, 541 
Social significance, 542-546 
Techniques, 367-373, 540-542, 571-581 
Two generations, 397, 406-407, 408- 
410, 412, 537-539, 541 
Variation, 234, 533, 538-539 
Petting to climax. See Petting. 

Physical defects, 209 

Physical exercise, affecting sex, 206 

Pimp, 61 

Pin-point sampling, 82-83 
Pioneers, sexual patterns of, 457, 459, 630- 
631 

Pituitary, 204 
Plato, 297* 

Plattner, K., 528, 781 
Pleasure, orgastic, 159 
Popenoe, P., 514, 543, 546, 560, 591, 606, 
781 

Positions in intercourse, 312-374, 51%- 
579, 604 

Post-marital outlets, 294-296 
Agmg effects, 262-292, 294-296 
Homosexual, 290-292, 295-296 
Intercourse, 67, 278-280, 282-284, 286- 
288, 295, 489 

Masturbation, 270-272, 294 
Nocturnal emissions, 274-276, 294-295 
Total outlet, 266-267 
Pratt, J,P., 204, 781 
Pre-adolescents, 157-181 
Anal contacts, 170-171 
Animal contacts, 174, 671, 673 
Coitus, 162, 166, 169-170, 173-174 
Companions, 168 
Development of attitudes, 440-447 
Erotic response, 175-181 
Interviewing, 58-59 
Oral contacts, 170-171, 174 
Orgasm, 159-161, 175-181 


Pre-adolescents, Continued 
Sex play, 66, 68-69, 162-181, 497, 499- 
506 

Sex play, ages, 162, 167, 169, 171-173 
Sex play, female, 157, 167 
Sex play, heterosexual, 162, 166-174 
Sex play, homosexual, 162, 167-172 
Sex play, incidence, 162, 165-174 
Pre-coital mucus, 230-233 
Pregnancy, fear of, 364 
Pre-mar ital adjustment, 222-223, 544-546 
Pre-mantal intercourse, 547-562 
Accumulative incidence, 139, 141, 145- 
146, 397, 400-401, 404-405, 549-551 
Active incidence, 248-251, 278, 280- 
288, 313-315, 347-352, 430-431, 
434-435, 456, 458, 480-481, 552-556 
Age at adolescence, 312-315, 324, 553 
Aging effects, 248-253,277-288, 552-553 
Attitudes on, 364, 381-383, 559-562 
Data on histones, 66 
Frequencies, 248-253, 278-288, 314, 
347-355, 430-431, 434-435, 456, 458, 
480-481, 552-556 
Pre-marital outlet, 277-288 
Religious background, 477-481, 553 
Rural-urban, 455-456, 458, 462, 553, 
556 

Significance in total cutlet, 376, 378, 
489-491 

Social level, 347-355, 364, 376, 381-383, 
426-427, 430-436, 442-445, 549-559 
Social significance, 559-562 
Two generations, 396, 400-401, 404- 
405, 410-413, 556-557 
Variation, 234, 324, 553-555 
Premature ejaculation, 580-581 
Prime of life, 222 

Primitive sex codes, 465, 547, 583, 668 
Prison. See Penal institution. 

Professional group, 79, 436 
Promiscuity, 589-590, 632 
Prostitutes, intercourse with, 595-609 
Accumulative incidence, 100-101, 116- 
118, 139, 141, 402-403, 597-600 
Active incidence, 250-251, 281, 285- 
288, 351-353, 434-435, 458, 600-603 
Aging effects, 250-253, 281, 285-288, 
601 

Data on histories, 67-68 
Frequencies, 250-251, 285-288, 352- 
354, 410-413, 434-435, 442, 458, 601- 
603 

Marital status, 277-288, 600-601 
Rural-urban, 455, 458, 462 
Significance in total outlet, 376, 378, 
489-493, 597-601 

Social level, 351-355, 376, 434-435, 442, 
588, 598-602 



INDEX 


801 


Prostitutes, intercourse with, Continued 
Social significance, 605-609 
Techniques, 604-605 
Two generations, 396, 402-403, 410- 
413, 603-604 
Variation, 234, 601-603 
Prostitution, heterosexual, 61, 67-68, 216, 
415-416, 595-609 

•Prostitution, homosexual, 70, 216, 596 
Protestant sex patterns, 465-487 
Psychiatrists, histories of, 21 1 
Psychologic factors, 157, 204, 216-217, 
309, 327-328, 440-447, 509, 515-5X8, 
523, 529, 569-571, 661, 676-678 
Psychologists, histories of, 211 
Psychoneuroses, 201-203, 660 
Pubic hair, development, 130-131, 183-185 
Public opinion, fear of, 364 
Public opinion polls, 20, 33 

Questionnaire, 63-70 


Race-cultural groups, 75-76 
Ramsey, G. K, viii, 29-30, 31, 32, 164, 183, 
185, 298, 499, 533, 621,781 
Range of variation. See Vanation, indi- 
vidual. 

Rape, 237-238 

Rat, sexual behavior, 613-615 
Rating scale, heterosexual-homosexual, 
638-641 

Rattler, T. A., 503, 528, 560, 591, 606, 782 
Ratzel, R, 547, 781 

Recall, immediate vs. remote, 148-150 

Recreational data on histories, 63-64 

Redemptorist Father, 543, 560, 781 

Regular outlet, 192 

Reich, W„ 159, 781 

Reichard, G., 547, 559, 583, 781 

Reitman, B., 591 

Reliability of data, 54-55, 120-153. See 
also Statistical analyses. 

Religion 

Classification, 79, 81, 468 
Effect on sex behavior, 465-487 
Source of sex mores, 202-203, 263-265, 
465, 468, 483-487, 547-548 
Religious background, 465-487 
Extra-marital intercourse, 481, 589 
Homosexual, 482-484, 631, 636 
Marital intercourse, 479-482, 569, 571 
Masturbation, 470-473, 476, 509 
Nocturnal emissions, 474-476, 525, 

Petting, 477-478, 540 
Pre-marital intercourse, 477-481, 553 
Total outlet, 466-469, 472 


Responsiveness, erotic. See Erotic re- 
sponse. 

Re-takes, 121-125 

Rice, T. B., 297, 395, 514, 530, 541, 543, 
546, 560, 591, 606, 781 
Richey, H. G„ 309, 781 
Roaring twenties, 396 
Robinson, S,, 227, 309, 782 
Robinson, V., 514, 591, 597, 606, 782 
Robinson, W. J., 297, 782 
Rockefeller Foundation, v, vii, ix 
Rockwood, L. D., 591, 782 
Rohleder, H., 34, 175, 190, 499, 782 
Roman ascetic cults, 202, 465 
Romer, 619 

Rosanoff, A. 514, 606, 618, 782 
Roth, A. A., 204 
Rothney, J. W, M„ 183, 771 
Ruland, L., 503, 528, 560, 591, 606, 782 
Rural-urban, 449-464 
Animal contacts, 459-464 
Classification, 79, 451 
Extra-marital intercourse, 456, 588-589 
Homosexual, 455-460, 630-631, 636 
Marital intercourse, 455, 459, 569 
Masturbation, 450, 453, 462, 509 
Nocturnal emissions, 452-453, 462, 525 
Patterns, 449-464 
Petting, 453-454, 462, 539 
Pre-marital intercourse, 455-456, 458, 
462, 553, 556 

Prostitute intercourse, 455, 458, 462 
Total outlet, 448, 451-453 


Sadler, W, S, andL, K., 514, 530, 543, 560, 
606, 782 

Sampling, 82-105 
Diversification, 92-93 
Hundred percent, 93-102, 133 
Order, 104-105 
Partial samples, 94-104 
Size of sample, 82-92, 737-764 
Techniques, 82-105 
U. S. synthesis, 105-109 
Sappho, 613 

Satyriasis, defined, 199-203 
Schajfner, B,, 26, 29, 499, 552, 621-622, 
775 

Schapera, /., 547, 583, 783 
Schedule 6f questions, 63-70 
Schonfeld, W.A., 130-131, 183, 783 
School, boarding, sex in, 225 
School grade, vs. age at adolescence, 186- 
187 

School teachers, 223, 387 
Sears, R. R,, 163, 164, 783 
Selective Service, 621-622, 783 
Self fellation, 510, 614 



802 


INDEX 


Semen, source, 527-528 
Semi-skilled laborers, 78, 426-421 
Sequence, in interviewing, 48-49 
Seward, G. H., 514, 533, 591, 783 
Sex drive 

High, 178-180, 195, 197-198, 213-217 
Low, 205-213 

Sex education, 64, 197, 223, 440-447, 514- 
516, 542-546 
Validity of data, 124 
Sex hormone, 204, 615, 659 
Sex offenses, 4, 223-225, 237-238, 263- 
265, 389-393, 437, 447, 572, 577-578, 
583, 601-603, 605, 655, 664-666, 677 
Sex organs, 573-574 

Sex play, pre-adolescent, 66, 68-69, 162- 
181, 497, 499-506 
Sexual activity, maximum, 219-221 
Sexual capacity, maximum, 179-180, 221 
Sexual response. See Erotic response. 
Sexual variation, maximum, 234-235 
Shafton, A, L., 185, 776 
Shuttleworth, F. K,, 104, 183, 783 
Significance in total outlet, 376, 378, 488- 
491 

Animal contacts, 378, 488-491, 669- 
670 

Extra-marital coitus, 489, 492^93, 587- 
588, 590 

Homosexual, 376, 378, 382, 488-493, 
610 

Marital intercourse, 380j 382, 489, 492- 
493, 564-568 

Masturbation, 376, 378, 380, 382, 488- 
493, 506-509 

Nocturnal emissions, 376, 378, 380, 382, 
488-493 

Petting, 376, 378, 488-491 
Post-marital coitus, 489 
Pre-marital intercourse, 376, 378, 489- 
491 

Prostitute intercourse, 376, 378, 489- 
493, 597-601 

Skilled laborers, 78, 427, 433 
Snedecor, G. If"., 92, 783 
Snow, W, F„ 514, 560, 606, 783 
Social data on histories, 63-64 
Social factors, 204-205, 327-447, 678 
Social level, 327-447 
Adolescence, age at, 188-189 
Animal contacts, 362-363, 671 
Coital positions, 372-374, 578-579, 604 
Coital techniques, 365-374, 540-542, 
571-582, 604-605 
Definition, 328-335 

Erotic responsiveness, 210, 345, 363- 
364, 521 

Extra-marital intercourse, 348, 350-354, 
380, 382-383, 431, 586-588, 592-593 


Social level. Continued 
Homosexual, 357-362, 376, 378, 382- 
384, 438-439, 442, 624-631, 634-636, ‘ 
640-654 

Marital intercourse, 355-357, 380, 432, 
569-570 

Masturbation, 339-343, 375-378, 380, 
422-423, 442, 444, 500-509 
Nocturnal emissions, 342-345, 376, 378,- 
380, 382, 424-425, 442, 444, 520-525 
Nudity, 365-367, 372, 386, 574, 581, 604 
Oral eroticism, 170, 368-373, 574-578, 
605 

Petting, 345-347, 365-373, 376-381, 
428-429, 442, 534-539, 541 
Pre-marital intercourse, 347-355, 364, 
376, 381-383, 426-427, 430-436, 442- 
445, 549-559 

Prostitute intercourse, 351-355, 376, 
434-435, 442, 588, 598-602 
Sources of first ejaculation, 190-192 
Sources of total outlet, 490-493 
Total intercourse, 316, 348-349, 358, 
400, 404, 410-411, 413, 430-431, 456, 
480-481, 566-567 

Total outlet, 335-339, 420-421, 442 
Sokolowsky, A., 205, 783 
Sorokin, P., 417, 783 
Source of 

First ejaculation, 190-192, 300-303 
Histories, 5, 13-16, 39 
Orgasm, 157, 193, 488 
Outlets, number of, 193-196, 228-229 
Outlets, relative importance, 376, 378, 
380, 382, 488-493 
Spalding, H, S,, 513, 560, 606, 770 
Spasm, in orgasm, 160-161 
Spontaneous ejaculation, 190-192,517-518 
Spouses, comparisons of data, 125-128 
Squier, R,, 618, 783 
Srole, L., 77, 331, 786 
Standard deviations, 89-92, 112-113, 738- 
748 

Statistical analyses, 20-21, 45, 63-108, 
109-119, 120-153 

Accumulative incidence curves, 114-119 
Comparing interviewers, 133-143 
Correlation coefficients, 114 
Cross-checks, 128-130 
Frequencies, 110-114 
Frequency class values, 111 
Incidence, active, 755-757 
Means, 112, 738-748 
Medians, 113-114, 749-754 
Memory, 120-153, 148-150, 183-185 
Mode, height, 760-762 
Mode, locus, 758-759 
Ogive, 115-117 

Pearsonianr, 114, 122-123, 126 



INDEX 


803 


Statistical analyses, Continued 
Percents of outlet, 114 
Range of variation, 763-765 
Re-takes, 121-125 
Smooth trends, 132 
Stability of method, 142-150 
Standard deviations, 89-92, 112-113, 
738-748 

Stratification, 75-82, 92 
Tetrachoric r, 114, 121-123, 126 
Twelve-way breakdown, 75-82, 92 
Validity of data, 120-153 
Stein, M., 103, 778 
Stekel, W,, 34, 783-784 
Stimuli, non-sexual, 164-165, 191-192 
Stith, R., Ill, 779 
Stone,- C.P., 615, 784 
Stone, H. M. and A., 514, 560, 606, 784 
Strain, F.B., 514, 784 
Strakosch, F. M., 29-30, 784 
Stratifie<J sampling, 75-82, 92 
Subareolar nodes, 185 
Subhmation, 205-213, 297-326, 528-530 
Suicide, 21 1 


Tactile stimulation, 157-163, 497-498, 
542-543 

Talmud, 202, 465, 473, 483, 547-548, 583, 
668 

Tanner, P., 543, 785 
Taxonomic method, 9, 16-21, 299 
Taylor; W, 5., 28, 30, 31, 499, 512, 546, 
597, 785 

Teachers, school, 223, 387 
Techniques 

Coital, 67, 365-374, 540-542, 571-582, 
604-605 

Masturbation, 509-511 
Petting, 367-373, 540-542, 571-582 
Tenenbaum, J., 514 

Terman, L. M., vii, 29, 31, 34, 309, 546, 
552, 556, 557, 560, 584, 618, 637, 785 
Tetrachoric r, 114, 121-123, 126 
Third sex, 612 
Thornton, V., 514, 785 
Thurnwald, R., 547, 583, 785 
Thyroid hormone, 204 
Timidity, 211, 633 
Total coitus 

Accumulative incidence, 98, 101, 137- 
138, 316-317, 396, 400-401, 404-405, 

565- 567 

Aging effects, 256 

Frequencies, 256, 278-280, 348-349, 
358, 410-411, 430-431, 456, 480-481 
Social level, 316, 348-349, 358, 400, 404, 
410-411, 413, 430-431, 456, 480-481, 

566- 567 


Total coitus, Continued 
Two generations, 396, 400-401, 404- 
405, 410 

Total outlet, 193-217 
Age at adolescence, 302-308, 324 
Aging effects, 219-221, 226-230, 266- 
267 

Average frequency, 194-195 
Frequencies, 193-217, 220-221, 226, 
266-269, 273, 302-307, 336-338, 410, 
420-421, 448, 466-467 
Marital status, 266-273 
Period of maximum, 219-221 
Post-marital, 266-267 
Religious background, 466-469, 472 
Rural-urban, 448, 451-453 
Social level, 335-339, 420-421, 442 
Two generations, 399, 401, 405, 410, 412 
Variation, 90, 195-203, 234, 324 
Trobrianders, 373 
Twelve-way breakdown, 75-82, 92 
Two generations, 150, 368-372, 394-417 


Under-development, defined, 199-203 
Underworld, defined, 78, 606 
Unresponsiveness, 157, 205-213, 544-546 
Upper white collar, 78-79, 433 
Ur excavations, 374 
Uranism, 612 

Urban-rural. See Rural-urban. 

Urethral insertions, 170 
U. S. Corrections, 105-109 
U. S, Public Health Service, 297, 513, 530, 
560, 606, 786 


Vaginal stimulation, 575-576 
Validity of data, 120-153. See also Statis- 
tical analyses. 

Determination of, 54-55, 120-153 
Frequencies, 123-126, 135, 143, 147, 
149 152-153 

Incid^ce, 121-124, 135-143, 147, 149, 

152 

Variation, individual 
Adolescence, 182-192 
Aging effects, 204, 234 
Animal contacts, 234, 671, 674 
Biologic factors, 203-205, 309-313, 327, 
660-663, 678 

Coital techniques, 571-582 
Extra-marital intercourse, 234, 586-589 
Homosexual, 234, 633-636 
Marital intercourse, 234, 569-571 
Masturbation, 91, 234, 324, 504-506 
Nocturnal emissions, 234, 521, 524 
Orgasm, 160-161, 178-180 
Petting, 234, 533, 538-539 



804 


INDEX 


Variation, individual, Continued 
Pre-marital intercourse, 234, 324, 553- 
555 

Prostitute intercourse, 234, 601-603 
Psychologic factors, 157, 204, 216-217, 
309, 327-328, 440-447, 509, 515-518, 
523, 529, 569-571, 661, 676-678 
Social significance, 197-203, 384-393 
Sociologic factors, 204-205, 327-447, 
678 

Total outlet, 90, 195-203, 234, 324 
VeckU V. <7., 297, 513, 517, 786 
Venereal disease, 364, 606, 608 
Vernacular, 52 
Vertical mobility, 417-439 
Virginity, attitudes on, 364, 379, 547- 
548 

Vital statistics, 121-124 
Vitamins, 204 
Voice change, 183-185 


War. See Armed forces. 

Warner, W, L„ 77, 331, 786 
Weatherhead^ L, D,, 513, 530, 543, 606, 
786 

Weighting tables, U. S. Corrections, 106- 
108 

Weinberger, L. M., 186, 786 
Weiss, 506, 514, 517, 525, 786 


Wells, F. L., vii, 27-28, 31, 33, 499, 519, 
780 

Werner, A. A., 221, 786 
Westermarck, £., 4, 483, 786 
Wet dreams. See Nocturnal emissions. 
Whelpton, P, K., 92-93, 786-787 
White collar group, lower, 78, 433 
White collar group, upper, 78-79, 433 
Wilbur, R. L., 606, 111 
Wile, I. S„ 499, 533, 552, 787 
Willoughby, R. R., 34, 514, 519, 533, 552, 
787 

Wissler, C., 547, 583, 787 
Wolfe, T. P., 159, 787 
Women. See Female. 

Wood, L. F., 560 
Wulffen, E., 513, 787 


Yerkes, R. M., vii, 205, 373, 787 
Y.M.C.A., 617 
Young, H. H., 186, 787 
Young, K„ 207, 787 
Young, W. C., 205, 615, 787 
Younger vs. older general] on, 150, 368- 
372, 394-417 


Zoo-erotic. See Animal contacts. 
Zoophilia. See Amimal contacts.